Using Play to Rewire & Improve Your Brain

Time: 0.36

- Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,

Time: 1.88

where we discuss science and science based tools

Time: 4.93

for everyday life.

Time: 6.208

[Upbeat music]

Time: 9.29

I'm Andrew Huberman,

Time: 10.5

and I'm a Professor of Neurobiology and Ophthalmology

Time: 13.19

at Stanford School of Medicine.

Time: 15.04

Today, we are going to talk about the biology,

Time: 17.21

psychology and utility of play.

Time: 20.931

Play is something that normally we associate

Time: 23.41

with children's games

Time: 25.2

and indeed with being a child,

Time: 27.74

much of our childhood development centers around play,

Time: 30.82

whether or not it's organized play

Time: 32.32

or spontaneous play,

Time: 34.57

but as adults, we also need to play.

Time: 37.32

And today I'm going to talk about

Time: 39.03

what I like to refer to as The Power of Play.

Time: 42.29

The Power of Play, resides in plays ability

Time: 45.6

to change our nervous system

Time: 47.63

for the better,

Time: 48.63

so that we can perform many activities,

Time: 51.5

not just play activities, better.

Time: 54.49

Play can also function

Time: 56.11

as a way to explore new ways of being

Time: 58.89

in different scenarios in work,

Time: 60.83

in relationships, in settings of all kind,

Time: 63.48

and indeed also in the relationship to oneself.

Time: 66.61

In fact,

Time: 67.443

we are going to explore how assuming different identities

Time: 70.22

during the same game of play

Time: 72.9

or the same forms of play

Time: 74.72

has been shown to be immensely powerful

Time: 76.634

for allowing people

Time: 78.14

to engage in more creative thinking

Time: 80.08

and dynamic thinking and indeed to better leaders

Time: 82.8

and more effective workers

Time: 84.14

and students and learners and happier people.

Time: 87.534

I'm also going to cover some data that shows

Time: 89.8

that learning to play properly

Time: 92.29

can enhance one's ability to focus

Time: 94.21

and is an active area of research

Time: 96.59

for treatment of things like ADHD,

Time: 99.47

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder,

Time: 101.66

just as a little sneak preview of where that's headed,

Time: 105.13

children who do not access enough play

Time: 107.96

during certain stages of childhood

Time: 109.83

are more prone to develop ADHD.

Time: 112.1

The good news,

Time: 112.933

is all of us regardless of whether or not we have ADHD

Time: 115.5

or not,

Time: 116.333

whether or not we had ample access to play

Time: 118.85

during childhood or not,

Time: 120.79

can engage and grow the neural circuits

Time: 123.824

that allow for this incredible power of play.

Time: 127.817

And this can be done again at any stage of life today.

Time: 130.29

We're going to talk about the protocols,

Time: 131.52

the science, we will review all of that.

Time: 133.33

And I promise you'll come away

Time: 134.85

with a lot of knowledge,

Time: 135.786

whether or not you're a parent,

Time: 137.57

whether or not you're a child,

Time: 138.8

whether or not you're a person of any age,

Time: 140.71

you're going to have tools

Time: 141.61

and knowledge that will benefit you.

Time: 143.67

Before we begin,

Time: 144.503

I want to share with you

Time: 145.42

the results of what I think to be an extremely exciting

Time: 148.37

and certainly an actionable study

Time: 150.03

that was just published in the journal Scientific Reports.

Time: 153.33

This is an excellent journal Nature Press journal,

Time: 156.25

Peer Reviewed, et cetera,

Time: 157.8

and the finding center around

Time: 159.9

what sorts of devices we happen to be reading on

Time: 162.97

and accessing information on

Time: 164.28

and how that's impacting our physiology

Time: 166.92

and our capacity to learn.

Time: 168.94

One of the more frequent questions I get

Time: 170.61

is what are all these devices, phones, tablets,

Time: 174.16

computers, video games et cetera,

Time: 175.73

doing to our brains.

Time: 177.27

And finally,

Time: 178.103

there's some good Peer reviewed data

Time: 179.53

to look at that and to address it directly.

Time: 183

This study, first author Honma

Time: 184.84

H O N M A

Time: 186.64

Honma et al,

Time: 188.22

is entitled

Time: 189.053

"Reading on a smartphone affects sigh generation.

Time: 192.02

That's SIGH sigh generation

Time: 193.82

brain activity and comprehension."

Time: 195.75

And to just summarize what they found,

Time: 198.53

they ran a study on 34 healthy individuals

Time: 203.04

and had them either read material on a smartphone

Time: 207.42

or on regular printed paper or a book.

Time: 211.08

And what they found is that

Time: 212.59

comprehension on devices in particular smartphones

Time: 215.671

is much poorer much worse than it is

Time: 220.13

when one reads on actual paper.

Time: 222.65

Now some of you may experience this yourselves.

Time: 226.03

Now they compared smartphones with paper.

Time: 228.61

And what they found was

Time: 230.05

that when they looked at people's breathing,

Time: 232.81

the normal patterns of breathing

Time: 234.29

that people were engaging in

Time: 235.5

did not differ between people reading on a smartphone

Time: 238.51

or reading from paper.

Time: 240.74

However, one particular feature of breathing did differ.

Time: 245.08

And that particular feature is what we call

Time: 246.73

physiological sighs.

Time: 248.13

I've talked a lot about physiological sighs on this podcast,

Time: 250.94

and on social media,

Time: 251.96

we had a terrific guest,

Time: 253.47

Professor, Jack Feldman from

Time: 254.78

University of California Los Angeles.

Time: 256.23

Who's a world expert in breathing and respiration

Time: 259.25

and its impacts on the brain

Time: 260.86

and how brain controls breathing and respiration

Time: 263.45

and what you can learn from that episode.

Time: 265.23

Or I'll just tell you again right now,

Time: 266.39

is that every five minutes or so,

Time: 268.72

whether or not we are asleep or awake,

Time: 270.18

we do what's called a physiological sigh,

Time: 271.91

which is a big, deep inhale,

Time: 273.75

often a double inhale followed by a long exhale.

Time: 277.23

It goes something like this.

Time: 278.323

[Inhaling]

Time: 280.361

[Sighs]

Time: 282.44

Now you might think, oh, I never breathe like that,

Time: 284.85

but you do,

Time: 286.01

unless there's something severely wrong with your brain stem

Time: 288.54

every five minutes or so

Time: 290.04

you do one of these physiological sighs,

Time: 291.9

which reopens all the little hundreds of millions of sacks

Time: 294.57

in your lungs called alveoli

Time: 296.27

that bring in more oxygen as a consequence of that

Time: 298.77

big deep, double inhale.

Time: 300.53

And then you are able to exhale carbon dioxide,

Time: 303.25

offload carbon dioxide

Time: 304.69

through that long exhale.

Time: 306.69

I've also encouraged people to use the physiological sigh

Time: 309.18

deliberately,

Time: 310.013

not just spontaneously

Time: 310.901

as a way to reduce their stress quickly.

Time: 313.42

And indeed my lab works on physiological sighs

Time: 315.63

and it's been exploring this

Time: 317.14

and they're quite effective in reducing

Time: 319.1

our stress very fast.

Time: 321.03

Reading on our smartphone,

Time: 322.64

seems to suppress physiological sigh.

Time: 325

People aren't aware that it's happening,

Time: 326.47

but it's happening.

Time: 327.35

Some people have talked about so-called email apnea,

Time: 330.26

which is the fact that people hold their breath

Time: 332.56

while they email or while they text.

Time: 334.22

And indeed many people do that.

Time: 335.42

This is distinct from email or texting apnea.

Time: 338.49

What's happening here is people are reading on the phone

Time: 340.93

and for whatever reason,

Time: 342.47

and I'll talk about what the likely reason is,

Time: 344.64

but for whatever reason,

Time: 345.87

they're suppressing their sighing

Time: 347.66

And as a consequence,

Time: 349.07

the brain is not getting enough oxygen

Time: 351.03

and is not offloading enough carbon dioxide.

Time: 353.56

And another finding in this study,

Time: 355.83

was that the prefrontal cortex,

Time: 357.48

an area of the brain that's involved in focus and attention

Time: 359.9

and learning

Time: 360.733

becomes hyperactive in a kind of desperate attempt to focus.

Time: 364.88

All of this can be summarized by saying,

Time: 366.84

if you happen to read on a device,

Time: 369.68

whether or not it's a tablet,

Time: 370.73

a standard computer screen of any kind,

Time: 373

but in particular on a smart phone,

Time: 375.01

regardless of how small or large that smartphone screen is,

Time: 378.29

you want to remind yourself

Time: 379.59

to engage in these physiological sighs fairly regularly.

Time: 383.4

And it might even be better to just

Time: 385.46

read the most or at least the key issues

Time: 389.16

and things that you're trying to learn

Time: 390.74

about the key information from paper,

Time: 392.68

either books or printed out material

Time: 394.37

of some other sort.

Time: 395.66

What's the underlying mechanism here?

Time: 397.07

Well,

Time: 398.02

one of the reasons I like this study so much,

Time: 399.728

is that it brings together two of my laboratories

Time: 402.56

and my particular interests in neuroscience,

Time: 404.99

which is how does our visual system

Time: 406.7

and the aperture meaning the size of our visual window

Time: 411.35

relate to our so-called autonomic function

Time: 413.36

or our internal state.

Time: 414.93

And basically what's happening here is

Time: 416.58

as any of us bring our visual window in more narrowly

Time: 420.86

as we contract our visual window,

Time: 422.37

which is exactly what happens

Time: 423.62

when we're looking at a little smartphone in front of us.

Time: 426.43

It seems to suppress the breathing apparatus

Time: 429.14

because we know that physiological sighs

Time: 431.02

are controlled by a specific set of neurons

Time: 432.94

in the brain stem called the parafacial nucleus

Time: 434.74

discovered by Dr. Jack Feldman.

Time: 436.43

And so there must be a mechanism whereby

Time: 438.7

when we tighten our visual window,

Time: 440.2

we somehow, and we don't know yet how this happens,

Time: 443.71

but somehow suppress the activity of these neurons

Time: 446.42

in the parafacial nucleus

Time: 447.38

that generate this physiological sighs.

Time: 449

So again,

Time: 449.833

you have two choices or I suppose you have many choices,

Time: 452.45

but two main choices

Time: 453.65

to contend with this new information.

Time: 455.22

One is that you remind yourself

Time: 458.01

to engage in deep breathing

Time: 459.28

and in particular, physiological sighs

Time: 460.79

every five minutes or so,

Time: 462

while reading anything or texting on your smartphone,

Time: 465.12

the other would be,

Time: 465.96

again, if there's material that you really need to learn

Time: 469.29

for sake of regurgitation later,

Time: 471.35

or for something particularly important,

Time: 473.353

try and read that from either a larger screen

Time: 476.08

or even better would be from printed materials or books.

Time: 478.98

Another reason I bring all that up

Time: 480.43

is that it relates to a larger theme,

Time: 481.9

which is that I get many, many questions about ADHD

Time: 485.32

and about people's challenges with focus

Time: 487.6

and much of what we're told these days is that

Time: 490.11

we are challenged with focus

Time: 491.33

because of the hundreds of videos

Time: 493.33

that we can see streaming by us in any moment on our phone,

Time: 495.94

which probably is true.

Time: 497.59

The fact that the information that we're reading

Time: 500.88

on the internet and on our phones

Time: 501.713

is emotionally disturbing or distressing in some way.

Time: 504.29

And that probably is true as well in many cases.

Time: 508.007

This study really points to the fact that independent

Time: 511.24

of the information that we are looking at

Time: 513.24

or consuming

Time: 514.27

independent whether or not it's movies or texts

Time: 516.32

or anything of that sort,

Time: 517.79

the mere size of the window, the aperture,

Time: 521.21

the screen that we are looking at

Time: 523.01

is also strongly impacting our ability to learn

Time: 526.14

and remember information.

Time: 527.66

So broaden that visual window

Time: 529.368

print things out,

Time: 531.04

look at a book.

Time: 531.873

I didn't design the system.

Time: 534.5

I always say, you know,

Time: 535.49

however our visual system and respiratory system

Time: 537.619

happened to evolve.

Time: 539.08

I wasn't consulted at the design phase.

Time: 540.64

This is just simply how your brain circuits work.

Time: 543.23

So if you want to learn things,

Time: 544.41

widen that visual window and even better print things out,

Time: 547.59

pick up a book or read on a tablet even,

Time: 550.41

but try and make that tablet larger

Time: 552.62

than a smartphone screen size.

Time: 554.49

Before we begin our discussion about The Power of Play,

Time: 557.59

I'd like to emphasize that this podcast

Time: 559.27

is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.

Time: 562.16

It is however,

Time: 562.993

part of my desire and effort to bring zero cost to consumer

Time: 565.61

information about science and science related tools

Time: 568.29

to the general public

Time: 569.7

in keeping with that theme,

Time: 570.78

I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.

Time: 573.53

Our first sponsor is Athletic Greens also now called AG1.

Time: 577.58

I started drinking Athletic Greens way back in 2012.

Time: 580.47

And so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast.

Time: 583.01

Athletic Greens is an all in one

Time: 584.44

vitamin mineral probiotic drink.

Time: 586.57

The reason I started drinking it in 2012,

Time: 588.91

and the reason that I still drink it once or twice a day,

Time: 591.69

is that with Athletic Greens,

Time: 593.34

I cover all of my basic foundational vitamin mineral

Time: 596.3

probiotic needs.

Time: 597.649

It's filled with adaptogens for recovery.

Time: 600.2

The probiotics are particularly important

Time: 602.16

because they encourage health

Time: 603.31

of the so-called gut microbiome.

Time: 605.33

We're going to be talking a lot about the gut microbiome

Time: 607.068

on this podcast in the weeks and months to come,

Time: 610.03

but to make a long story short,

Time: 612.04

we have a lot of bacteria living in our gut

Time: 614.21

that are healthy bacteria

Time: 615.41

that support them like digestion immune system,

Time: 617.51

metabolic function, hormone systems.

Time: 619.428

And perhaps most interestingly to me,

Time: 621.978

the gut microbiome supports brain function,

Time: 624.66

including mood cognition and so forth.

Time: 627.41

The probiotics in Athletic Greens

Time: 628.88

strongly support the gut microbiome

Time: 630.68

and thereby the other systems of the body,

Time: 632.88

including the gut brain access,

Time: 634.93

which I just referred to.

Time: 636.34

If you like to try Athletic Greens again,

Time: 638.1

also called AG1,

Time: 639.29

you can go to athleticgreens.com/huberman

Time: 642.57

to claim a special offer.

Time: 643.99

They're giving away five free travel packs

Time: 645.73

and a year supply of vitamin D3, K2,

Time: 648.75

vitamin D3 is extremely important

Time: 651.64

for many biological functions.

Time: 653.66

We are told that we can get enough D3 from sun exposure,

Time: 656.11

but many of us we're getting sun exposure.

Time: 658.56

And many of us who are not getting enough sun exposure

Time: 660.46

still don't have ample levels of vitamin D3

Time: 663.31

So supplementing with vitamin D3 is necessary

Time: 665.37

for many many people.

Time: 666.49

And K2 is important for cardiovascular function

Time: 669.19

for proper calcium balance, et cetera.

Time: 671.86

So again, if you go to athleticgreens.com/huberman,

Time: 674.63

you can get the five free travel packs

Time: 676.21

a year supply of vitamin the D3 K2,

Time: 678.42

and of course your Athletic Greens.

Time: 680.4

Today's episode is also brought to us by ROKA.

Time: 682.564

ROKA makes sunglasses and eyeglasses

Time: 684.79

that are of the absolute highest quality.

Time: 687

I've spent a lifetime working on the visual system.

Time: 689.44

And one of the many challenges that your visual system

Time: 692.22

has to deal with

Time: 693.2

is for instance, when you move from a bright environment

Time: 695.61

to a dimer environment, you have to adapt.

Time: 697.96

You have have to be able to see things clearly,

Time: 699.54

many sunglasses and eyeglasses don't allow that adaptation

Time: 702.32

to occur seamlessly,

Time: 703.68

ROKA sunglasses and eyeglasses

Time: 705.89

allow that to occur in concert with your biology

Time: 708.58

and in a way that makes it very seamless.

Time: 710.38

You just don't notice the transition at all,

Time: 712.51

which is wonderful.

Time: 713.42

I love it 'cause I don't have to constantly be taking off

Time: 715.34

and putting on my sunglasses when I'm outside.

Time: 717.49

I happen to wear readers at night, eye glass readers.

Time: 719.95

It's also terrific as the luminance

Time: 721.53

in your environment changes.

Time: 722.592

So they work under low luminance,

Time: 724.34

high luminance, et cetera.

Time: 725.94

They're very lightweight.

Time: 726.9

In fact, you can wear them when running, when cycling,

Time: 729.95

they were designed by two All-American swimmers

Time: 732.61

from Stanford.

Time: 733.443

And so originally they were designed

Time: 734.61

for the purpose of being worn during exercise,

Time: 736.99

but you can wear them anywhere.

Time: 738.49

They have a great aesthetic,

Time: 739.47

so you can wear them to dinner,

Time: 740.53

to work, et cetera.

Time: 741.563

And while a lot of performance eyeglasses out there,

Time: 744.23

in my opinion, make people look like cyborgs,

Time: 746.31

ROKA sunglasses and eyeglasses

Time: 747.78

have just a terrific aesthetic

Time: 749.07

that you can wear truly anywhere.

Time: 750.85

If you'd like to try ROKA glasses,

Time: 752.28

you can go to roka.com that's ROKA.com

Time: 755.62

and enter the code Huberman

Time: 756.93

to get 20% off your first order.

Time: 758.807

Again, that's ROKA ROKA.com and enter the code Huberman

Time: 762.89

at checkout.

Time: 763.723

Today's episode is also brought to us by Helix Sleep.

Time: 766.58

Helix Sleep makes mattresses and pillows

Time: 768.57

that are second to none.

Time: 770.12

I started sleeping on a Helix mattress about a year ago now,

Time: 773.41

and it's the best sleep that I've ever had.

Time: 775.61

What's unique about Helix mattresses

Time: 777.362

is that they tailor their mattresses to your sleep needs.

Time: 780.332

So if you go to their website,

Time: 782.47

you can take a brief two minute quiz

Time: 784.36

that asks questions like,

Time: 785.26

do you sleep on your stomach

Time: 786.38

or your back or on your side, maybe you don't know,

Time: 788.24

do you tend to run hot or cold throughout the night?

Time: 790.28

Or maybe you don't know,

Time: 791.24

and it matches you to the mattress.

Time: 793.08

that's perfect for your sleep needs.

Time: 794.73

I matched to the so-called Dusk mattress,

Time: 796.69

D U S K,

Time: 797.92

because I wanted a mattress that wasn't too firm

Time: 799.95

and not too soft.

Time: 800.783

And so I sleep on a Dusk mattress.

Time: 802.138

You should figure out what mattress would be ideal for you.

Time: 804.956

If you're interested in upgrading your mattress,

Time: 807

you can go to helixsleep.com/huberman,

Time: 809.044

take their two minute sleep quiz

Time: 811.38

and the match you to a customized mattress.

Time: 813.21

And you'll get up to $200 off

Time: 815.47

any of your mattress orders and two free pillows.

Time: 817.76

Again, that's $200 off and two free pillows.

Time: 820.73

The pillows are excellent.

Time: 821.563

I also use Helix pillows.

Time: 823

They have a 10 year warranty.

Time: 824.27

You get to try out the mattress

Time: 825.67

for up to a hundred nights, risk free.

Time: 827.28

If you don't like it, they'll come pick it up,

Time: 828.58

take it away.

Time: 829.43

But I think you'll love it.

Time: 830.86

Again if interested,

Time: 831.7

you can go to helixsleep.com/huberman.

Time: 834.72

There can get up to $200 off

Time: 836.37

and the two free pillows.

Time: 837.91

Let's talk about play.

Time: 839.97

Now in researching this episode,

Time: 841.54

I thought that I was going to come across a bunch of papers

Time: 844.31

that say this brain area

Time: 846.16

connects to that brain area,

Time: 847.76

which controls play in animals.

Time: 849.7

And there's similar areas in babies and in adults.

Time: 852.63

And indeed, that's true.

Time: 853.6

And we will talk about brain circuitry,

Time: 856.12

but I think more importantly is to understand

Time: 858.59

what is the utility of play?

Time: 860.61

You know, why do we play when we're younger?

Time: 862.17

Why do we tend to play less as we get older?

Time: 864.6

And what in the world is play for?

Time: 867.48

Some of us would be categorized as more playful.

Time: 870.187

I'm sure that you know, people like this,

Time: 872.3

maybe you are like this

Time: 873.15

people that can walk into a room,

Time: 875.11

a social setting of any kind,

Time: 876.52

and they seem to already kind of have a playful,

Time: 879.2

maybe even a mischievous quality about them.

Time: 881.05

We'll talk about mischief a little bit later,

Time: 882.86

but they sort of look at an environment

Time: 884.92

or a social setting as an opportunity

Time: 886.79

for different kinds of novel interactions.

Time: 889.28

Other people and I'd probably put myself into this category.

Time: 891.832

If I walk into a novel environment,

Time: 893.87

I tend to be more in the mode of just assessing

Time: 896.7

what that environment is like.

Time: 897.938

I'm not a particularly spontaneously playful person,

Time: 901.07

although around certain individuals,

Time: 902.42

I might be more spontaneously playful.

Time: 904.93

We are all on a continuum of this kind of seriousness

Time: 907.96

to playful nature.

Time: 909.577

Turns out that all young animals,

Time: 912.42

including humans

Time: 913.71

have more playfulness and tend to engage

Time: 915.95

in more spontaneous play

Time: 917.43

in their earlier years

Time: 918.45

than in their later years.

Time: 920.74

And therein lies a very interesting portal

Time: 922.98

to understanding what the utility,

Time: 925.59

what the purpose of play is.

Time: 927.62

First of all,

Time: 928.453

I want to lay down a couple of key facts about play

Time: 931.85

that point to the fact that play is not just about games.

Time: 935.67

Play is about much, much more

Time: 938.13

and play and in particular,

Time: 939.85

how we played as a child

Time: 941.905

and still how we can play as adults

Time: 944.58

is really how we test

Time: 946.02

and expand our potential roles in all kinds of interactions.

Time: 950.72

One of the most important,

Time: 952.01

interesting and surprising features of play

Time: 954.69

that I'd like everyone to know about

Time: 956.7

is that it is homeostatically regulated.

Time: 959.63

Some of you are familiar with the term homeostasis.

Time: 961.65

Homeostasis is just this aspect of biological systems

Time: 965.26

to try and remain in balance.

Time: 967.926

You know, if you stay awake for a long period of time,

Time: 969.64

you tend to want to sleep for a long period of time.

Time: 972.67

If you slept for a long period of time

Time: 974.27

and you're very rested,

Time: 975.103

then you tend to be very energetic the next day.

Time: 976.81

And of course, I know people out there will immediately say,

Time: 978.9

oh, well, if I sleep too long,

Time: 980.15

then I'm groggy the next day.

Time: 981.25

Of course there are exceptions,

Time: 982.62

but in general,

Time: 983.579

sleep and wakefulness are in homeostatic balance.

Time: 987.9

Thirst and water consumption are in homeostatic balance.

Time: 991.07

If you don't drink any fluids for a while,

Time: 992.81

you tend to get more thirsty,

Time: 994.18

you drink fluids

Time: 995.59

and your thirst tends to diminish

Time: 997.47

likewise with food.

Time: 998.66

Likewise, with most all motivated behaviors.

Time: 1001.58

Well,

Time: 1002.413

one of the most important discoveries

Time: 1003.37

of the last century,

Time: 1004.498

was largely the work of a guy named Jaak Panksepp.

Time: 1007.69

No, it's not Jack it's Jaak Panksepp

Time: 1010.16

who really pioneered this understanding

Time: 1012.32

of the biology of play

Time: 1013.95

and relating that to the psychology of play

Time: 1015.87

in animals and humans.

Time: 1017.24

He's considered kind of luminary in the field of play,

Time: 1021.217

and what a great title to have, right?

Time: 1022.87

If you could have a title and be a scientific luminary,

Time: 1024.73

you might as well be the, the play guy.

Time: 1026.99

In fact he was known and I'll get into this later as to why,

Time: 1029.95

but he was known as the "Rat Tickler"

Time: 1032.18

because he tickled rats.

Time: 1033.36

And he actually found that rodents and animals of many kind

Time: 1036.47

generate laughter in response to tickling.

Time: 1039.28

And in fact,

Time: 1040.22

they don't have the capacity to tickle themselves

Time: 1042.68

something we'll also talk about why that is.

Time: 1044.72

And he was called the "Rat tickler,"

Time: 1046.66

but then he discovered that many species of animals

Time: 1049.63

engage in laughter in response to tickling

Time: 1051.49

and they tickle each other.

Time: 1052.78

And the reason you don't hear them laughing,

Time: 1054.73

no you can't hear your dog laughing

Time: 1056.33

that isn't laughing it's something else

Time: 1058.14

is that most animals besides humans laugh

Time: 1061.41

at kind of ultrasonic levels of auditory output.

Time: 1065.5

Meaning the frequencies of sound are just too high

Time: 1067.82

for you to hear,

Time: 1068.653

but with the appropriate devices,

Time: 1069.583

he was able with his colleagues

Time: 1071.7

to isolate this so-called the "Rat Laughter"

Time: 1074.78

and then turns out there's kitten laughter

Time: 1076.43

and there's puppy laughter.

Time: 1077.55

And of course there's human laughter.

Time: 1079.34

So Jaak Panksepp was a very interesting

Time: 1081.1

and pioneering person in this field.

Time: 1083.32

And he also discovered that play

Time: 1085.92

is homeostatically regulated.

Time: 1087.53

Meaning if animals including children

Time: 1089.92

are restricted from playing for a certain amount of time,

Time: 1092.73

they will play more when given the opportunity

Time: 1095.22

in the same way that if I food restrict you

Time: 1096.95

for a long period of time,

Time: 1097.977

you'll eat more when you are finally allowed to eat.

Time: 1100.27

Now this is important

Time: 1102.55

because it moves this thing that we call

Time: 1105.13

play from the dimension of higher order functions

Time: 1108.55

or things evolved recently,

Time: 1110.722

you know, that are really kind of at the front edge

Time: 1114.45

of human evolution,

Time: 1115.654

deeper into the circuitry of the brain,

Time: 1119.22

whereby we say the brain stem,

Time: 1121.96

the kind of ancient parts of the brain

Time: 1123.91

are going to be involved.

Time: 1124.82

And in fact, that's the case

Time: 1126.21

as we're going to learn later in the podcast,

Time: 1128.312

play is generated through the connectivity

Time: 1131.66

of many brain areas.

Time: 1133.23

But one of the key brain areas

Time: 1134.77

is an area called PAG Periaqueductal Gray.

Time: 1139.14

The Periaqueductal Gray is a brain stem area.

Time: 1142.194

So it's pretty far back

Time: 1144.03

as the brain kind of transitions into the spinal cord.

Time: 1146.66

And it's rich with neurons that make endogenous opioids.

Time: 1150.99

So these are not the kinds of opioids

Time: 1152.55

that are causing the opioid crisis.

Time: 1154.9

These are neurons that you and I all have

Time: 1157.82

that release endogenous, meaning self-made

Time: 1160.12

or biologically made opioids.

Time: 1162.62

They go by names like enkephalin

Time: 1164.51

and things of that sort.

Time: 1165.934

Play evokes small amounts of opioid release into the system.

Time: 1171.6

They kind of dope you up a little bit,

Time: 1173.41

not so much as one would see if

Time: 1176.66

one were to take exogenous opioids.

Time: 1178.28

And in fact exogenous opioids as we now know,

Time: 1181.2

are potentially very hazardous,

Time: 1183.91

highly high addiction potential

Time: 1185.73

high overdose potential.

Time: 1186.81

They cause all sorts of problems.

Time: 1188.21

Yes there are clinical uses for them,

Time: 1189.79

but they're causing a lot of problems nowadays,

Time: 1191.49

but these endogenous opioids

Time: 1193.17

are released in children and adults

Time: 1194.68

anytime we engage in play.

Time: 1197.54

And that turns out to be a very important chemical state

Time: 1200.58

because there's something about

Time: 1201.87

having an abundance of these endogenous opioids

Time: 1204.3

released into the brain

Time: 1205.702

that allows other areas of the brain,

Time: 1207.72

like the prefrontal cortex,

Time: 1209.08

the area of the front that's responsible

Time: 1210.65

for what we call executive function.

Time: 1212.12

Executive function is the ability to make predictions,

Time: 1214.38

to assess contingencies.

Time: 1216.92

Like if I do this,

Time: 1218.23

then that happens.

Time: 1219.063

If I do that, then that happens.

Time: 1220.61

Well prefrontal cortex is often seen

Time: 1222.57

as a kind of rigid executive of the whole brain.

Time: 1225.86

That's one way to view it,

Time: 1227.18

but probably a better way to view it

Time: 1228.62

is that the prefrontal cortex works

Time: 1230.69

in concert with these other are more primitive circuitries.

Time: 1234.07

And when the periaqueductal gray releases

Time: 1237.7

these endogenous opioids during play,

Time: 1240.2

the prefrontal cortex, doesn't get stupid.

Time: 1242.76

It actually gets smarter.

Time: 1244.26

It develops the ability to take on different roles

Time: 1247.83

and explore different contingencies.

Time: 1249.88

And we're going to talk about role play

Time: 1251.53

later in different contexts.

Time: 1253.05

And what we will find is that so much of play

Time: 1256.17

is really about exploring things in a way

Time: 1259.64

that feels safe enough to explore, right?

Time: 1262.82

This is not what happens when we drive down the street

Time: 1264.98

or when we bike down the street,

Time: 1266.154

when we are headed to work,

Time: 1267.73

commuting on our bicycle or walking or driving,

Time: 1270

we tend to be very linear

Time: 1271.22

and we tend to be very goal directed.

Time: 1272.87

We're not going to just take a new street just because

Time: 1275.25

we're not going to be spontaneously riding in the middle

Time: 1277.9

of the road

Time: 1278.733

and then on the sidewalk and then back and forth.

Time: 1280.25

Although I can remember as a kid,

Time: 1281.23

I was doing some of that.

Time: 1282.063

I like to jump off curb cuts when I was a kid.

Time: 1283.67

And then eventually I graduated, sorry to the cyclist,

Time: 1286.41

but I graduated to skateboarding.

Time: 1287.84

And then I look on skateboarding.

Time: 1289.39

You're always kind of exploring terrain,

Time: 1290.45

but you know, as I got older,

Time: 1291.99

actually I find myself becoming much more linear.

Time: 1293.9

I just don't play with my commute very much.

Time: 1296.44

It's really just about getting to work and then working.

Time: 1299.61

When endogenous opioids are in our system,

Time: 1301.88

when we're in this mode of play,

Time: 1304.82

the prefrontal cortex starts seeing

Time: 1307.32

and exploring many more possibilities of how we interact

Time: 1310.74

with our environment,

Time: 1311.95

with others and the roles that we can assume for ourselves.

Time: 1315.76

And so we're going to dissect one by one,

Time: 1318.39

the different aspects of play,

Time: 1320.04

role play, social play,

Time: 1321.57

individual play, imaginary play, competitive play

Time: 1324.393

they are enormous number of dimensions of play.

Time: 1326.65

And by the end of this episode,

Time: 1328.139

we are going to arrive at a very key feature.

Time: 1331.16

The key feature is one that's called

Time: 1333.8

your personal play identity.

Time: 1336.15

All of us have what we call a personal play identity.

Time: 1339.84

This personal play identity

Time: 1341.25

was laid down during development.

Time: 1343.2

And it is the identity that you assume in playful scenarios.

Time: 1347.6

And it is identity that you adopt in non playful scenarios.

Time: 1351.597

Now the great news is that your personal play identity

Time: 1354.82

is plastic throughout your entire lifespan.

Time: 1357.13

You can adjust your personal play identity

Time: 1358.98

in ways that will benefit you in work in relationships

Time: 1361.84

and your overall level of happiness.

Time: 1363.44

We will discuss protocols and ways to do that.

Time: 1365.99

But I do want to give a nod to the late jack,

Time: 1369.16

Jaak excuse me,

Time: 1370.323

Jaak Panksepp, "The rat tickler."

Time: 1373.27

And I also want to just give a nod to play generally,

Time: 1376.57

as we move forward in the discussion.

Time: 1377.94

What I'd love for everyone to do

Time: 1379.41

is to stop thinking about play as just a child activity,

Time: 1383.265

not just a sport related activity,

Time: 1387.11

but really as an exploration in contingencies.

Time: 1390.64

Again, it's an exploration of,

Time: 1393.24

if I do A, what happens?

Time: 1395.29

If I do B, what happens?

Time: 1396.89

If someone else takes on behavior or attitude C

Time: 1401.39

what am I going to do?

Time: 1402.74

And play is really where we can expand our catalog

Time: 1406.21

of potential outcomes,

Time: 1407.54

and it can be enormously enriching.

Time: 1409.64

And indeed as we'll talk about the tinkerers of the world,

Time: 1413.73

the true creatives,

Time: 1414.949

the people that building incredible technologies and art,

Time: 1418.7

and also that just have incredibly rich,

Time: 1420.71

emotional and intellectual and social lives

Time: 1423.4

all have a strong element of play.

Time: 1426.07

And so today I hope to convince you

Time: 1427.64

of some protocols that will allow you to expand

Time: 1430.13

your various roles in life,

Time: 1431.55

through the portal of play.

Time: 1433.31

So we establish that play is homeostatic,

Time: 1435.8

meaning we all need to do it.

Time: 1437.61

Many of us, including myself,

Time: 1439.593

haven't played that much as adults.

Time: 1441.96

We're all pretty busy.

Time: 1443.33

Number of us are stressed.

Time: 1444.36

We got a lot to do in life,

Time: 1445.372

but as children,

Time: 1447.24

most all of us engage in a lot of play.

Time: 1450.283

And in looking at the way that very young children

Time: 1454

and especially toddlers play,

Time: 1456.12

we can learn a lot

Time: 1457.46

because it reveals the fundamental rules

Time: 1459.97

by which the toddler brain interacts with the world.

Time: 1463.42

Now,

Time: 1464.253

one of the key things

Time: 1465.45

about the baby brain

Time: 1467.03

is that the baby brain somehow

Time: 1469.37

knows that it can't do everything in the world, right?

Time: 1472.59

If a baby needs something,

Time: 1475.12

it generally will cry or make some sort of vocalization

Time: 1478.87

or some sort of facial expression or combination of those.

Time: 1481.65

And the caretaker, whoever that may be will provide it.

Time: 1484.21

This is an ancient hard wired mechanism

Time: 1486.37

whereby the so-called autonomic nervous system

Time: 1488.27

that generates stress

Time: 1489.51

will create this kind of whining and discomfort,

Time: 1491.98

maybe arriving,

Time: 1492.92

maybe the baby gets kind of red in the face

Time: 1495.18

and the caretaker delivers some thing

Time: 1498.69

based on a good guess of what that baby needs.

Time: 1502.44

So maybe it's breast milk,

Time: 1503.98

maybe it's bottle milk.

Time: 1505.24

Maybe it's a diaper change.

Time: 1506.76

Maybe it's to be warmed up if the baby is cold,

Time: 1509.22

maybe it's to be cooled down if the baby's too warm,

Time: 1511.49

maybe if the baby's in its a little onesie thing,

Time: 1513.59

it's feeling restricted and it just wants to move

Time: 1515.48

and they'll get taken out of their,

Time: 1517.093

their crib or their stroller,

Time: 1518.81

whatever it is and allowed to stretch out on the floor.

Time: 1521.29

Remember the baby doesn't know exactly what it needs.

Time: 1523.46

It only knows the state of discomfort.

Time: 1525.95

And of course we don't know exactly what babies and toddlers

Time: 1528.57

are thinking

Time: 1529.44

because they can't express themselves with language yet.

Time: 1532.36

But what's key to understand

Time: 1533.65

is the rule or the contingency that is set up

Time: 1536.18

in that scenario.

Time: 1537.18

In that scenario,

Time: 1538.62

the child feels some discomfort

Time: 1542.34

expresses that discomfort verbally

Time: 1544.28

or through a facial expression or both

Time: 1546.17

and then some force some person from the outside world

Time: 1550.76

resolves it.

Time: 1551.93

And so the very young baby

Time: 1553.873

and indeed many children up to certain ages

Time: 1556.57

and let's confess many adults

Time: 1559.31

are not able to meet or adjust their internal states

Time: 1562.53

of stress.

Time: 1563.363

And so they look to things outside of them.

Time: 1564.69

That's the first rule,

Time: 1565.96

the fundamental rule

Time: 1567.19

that we all learn when we come into life,

Time: 1569.16

that when in a state of discomfort

Time: 1571.135

to look outside our immediate biology,

Time: 1574.493

beyond the confines of our skin and find a solution,

Time: 1577.95

a sip of water for adults, it might be sip of alcohol,

Time: 1581.07

right?

Time: 1581.903

Probably not the best tool to relieve stress,

Time: 1583.53

but that's one that many people do.

Time: 1584.72

In fact engaging for the baby that's hungry.

Time: 1587.07

The bottle milk comes from the outside.

Time: 1588.79

As we gain more proficiency in moving through life

Time: 1591.15

and we can get things for ourselves.

Time: 1592.97

We still often bring things from the external world in

Time: 1595.382

to resolve this what I'm calling

Time: 1597.96

autonomic discomfort or autonomic dysregulation.

Time: 1603.584

That's not a game, but that's a rule.

Time: 1607.17

As we advance from infant to toddler,

Time: 1610.36

we start to think more in terms of where we are

Time: 1613.98

and what we own relative to what's out there in the world.

Time: 1618.04

And now in the world of child psychology,

Time: 1620.74

there's a somewhat famous poem

Time: 1622.65

that was written by a research child psychologist.

Time: 1625.81

His name was Burton White,

Time: 1627.24

and he wrote a poem called "The Toddlers Creed"

Time: 1629.847

"The Toddlers Creed" defines well,

Time: 1631.99

what the rules and contingencies of play are

Time: 1635.52

in very young children.

Time: 1636.75

And it reveals to us just how narrow

Time: 1639.59

and limited their world view is

Time: 1642.13

and how self entered their world is.

Time: 1644.91

So "The Toddlers Creed" read quickly,

Time: 1646.84

cuz' I don't want to take up too much time with this is.

Time: 1648.737

"If I want it.

Time: 1649.69

It's mine.

Time: 1650.74

If I give it to you and change my mind later,

Time: 1653.33

it's mine."

Time: 1654.163

For anyone that's played with a toddler,

Time: 1655.21

you can imagine this in your mind.

Time: 1656.727

"If I can take it away from you, it's mine.

Time: 1659.48

If I had a little while ago, it's mine.

Time: 1662.45

If we are building something together,

Time: 1664.013

all the pieces are mine.

Time: 1666.09

If it looks just like mine,

Time: 1667.6

it's mine.

Time: 1668.73

If it's mine, it will never belong to anyone else,

Time: 1671.13

no matter what."

Time: 1672.482

And of course, as we hear this

Time: 1675.44

sounds quite awful, right?

Time: 1676.73

And yet this is actually a reflection of what

Time: 1679.04

a healthy toddler would think about the world.

Time: 1681.89

That the objects and things,

Time: 1683.3

and even the people in the world are theirs,

Time: 1686.19

that they are actually possessions that belong to them.

Time: 1688.56

Now of course,

Time: 1689.49

some people never actually transition beyond this stage

Time: 1691.82

of moral and social development.

Time: 1694.35

And there are indeed some adults

Time: 1696.243

that fit "The Toddlers Creed."

Time: 1698.13

And you're welcome to share this with them.

Time: 1700.014

If ever you think that it might be of benefit

Time: 1702.31

to their self-reflection.

Time: 1704.19

But in all seriousness Burton White's

Time: 1707.457

"Toddlers Creed" is really grounded

Time: 1709.95

in this transition from when we are infants.

Time: 1712.52

And we have to have things delivered to us,

Time: 1714.75

to the point where we are toddlers

Time: 1716.44

and we can access things in the world,

Time: 1718.51

but we tend to assume that they are all ours.

Time: 1720.83

And then the next stage

Time: 1722.73

is the really key stage as it relates to play

Time: 1724.87

because is in the next stage of development

Time: 1727.46

is where young children start

Time: 1729.31

to interact with other children

Time: 1730.81

and there's an exchange and a possession

Time: 1733.34

and then a letting go of certain things,

Time: 1735.79

learning that not everything is yours

Time: 1738.11

and that the entire world is not about you

Time: 1740.19

is one of the key contingencies

Time: 1741.99

that is established during play.

Time: 1744.67

It's one of the key way in which children go

Time: 1747.22

from being very self-centered

Time: 1749.01

and basically unable to engage with other kids

Time: 1751.37

for very long,

Time: 1752.203

without some sort of eruption of crying

Time: 1754.08

and some sort of battle of, you know,

Time: 1756.4

kind of push pull over an object

Time: 1758.3

to things like sharing and things like cooperative play.

Time: 1761.56

So as we transition from forms of play

Time: 1763.72

that are all about the self

Time: 1765.14

that are all me, me, me, me,

Time: 1766.367

"The Toddlers Creed"

Time: 1767.435

to forms of play that involve

Time: 1769.502

some discomfort in assuming roles

Time: 1772.24

that maybe we don't want and not getting what we want.

Time: 1775.051

It's really an opportunity for the brain

Time: 1777.98

to start to explore different roles that people take,

Time: 1781.37

how they work as individuals

Time: 1782.7

and as pairs and in larger groups.

Time: 1785.22

And to do that in a low stakes environment, right?

Time: 1789.3

You wouldn't want this to be worked out

Time: 1791.35

on the battlefield or when searching for food

Time: 1793.89

or in some high stakes environment

Time: 1795.88

where the survival of the species was important.

Time: 1799.16

It appears that these circuitry for play evolved.

Time: 1801.92

So that rules and contingencies around who's most important,

Time: 1806.46

whether or not the group is important,

Time: 1807.84

whether or not individuals are going to be leaders

Time: 1809.87

or followers, et cetera,

Time: 1811.24

that can be explored in a low stakes environment.

Time: 1813.75

Now there are hundreds of different types of play

Time: 1815.86

and hundreds of different types of contingency testing.

Time: 1818.61

But the key theme here is that

Time: 1820.53

play allows children and adults for that matter

Time: 1824.95

to explore different outcomes

Time: 1826.82

in a kind of low stakes environment.

Time: 1828.57

If you're playing a board game or a card game,

Time: 1830.213

you might get really into that game,

Time: 1832.37

but unless there's a lot of money on the table, so to speak,

Time: 1834.57

or you're really playing for something important

Time: 1836.64

or unless your ego is swollen way out of proportion,

Time: 1839.03

to reality,

Time: 1840.22

if you lose, you might not feel good about it,

Time: 1842.47

but it's truly not the end of the world.

Time: 1844.07

And if you win,

Time: 1845.17

you might feel really good about it,

Time: 1846.44

but you're not really incredible.

Time: 1848.24

You were just incredible in that particular situation

Time: 1851.09

for that particular moment,

Time: 1852.25

it doesn't really transform the rest of your life,

Time: 1854.88

unless that game is of a particular type for sport

Time: 1857.52

for instance.

Time: 1858.86

We'll talk about sport later.

Time: 1860.55

So the key theme here is that play is contingency testing.

Time: 1864.74

play is contingency testing under conditions

Time: 1867.58

where the stakes are sufficiently low,

Time: 1870.46

that individuals should feel comfortable

Time: 1872.55

assuming different roles,

Time: 1873.66

even roles that they're not entirely comfortable

Time: 1876.51

within their outside life.

Time: 1878.59

And that all relates again

Time: 1880.18

to the release of these endogenous opioids,

Time: 1882.42

in this brain center periaqueductal gray

Time: 1884.46

and the way that it allows the prefrontal cortex

Time: 1886.98

in a very direct way I mean,

Time: 1888.2

truly it allows it in a biological way

Time: 1890.423

to expand the number of operations that it can run,

Time: 1894.64

and start thinking about, oh, well, okay.

Time: 1897.54

Normally I'm kind of a loner

Time: 1899.24

and I like to read and work

Time: 1900.932

and you know, hang out alone maybe and play alone

Time: 1903.14

but you know okay,

Time: 1904.01

I'll play a board game

Time: 1904.93

or a game of tennis

Time: 1906.24

where I have a partner and we're going to play as partners

Time: 1907.99

against two other people.

Time: 1909.08

Okay, that's a little uncomfortable, but I'll do it.

Time: 1911.8

And in doing that,

Time: 1912.68

you discover certain ways

Time: 1914

in which you are proficient in certain ways,

Time: 1915.92

in which you are less proficient,

Time: 1917.58

you discover that the other person

Time: 1919.09

actually tends to cheat a little bit

Time: 1922.01

or the other person is extremely rigid about the rules,

Time: 1925.15

or maybe it is extremely rigid about the way they organize

Time: 1927.68

their pieces on the board,

Time: 1929.13

or they're crossing the line

Time: 1930.29

into your side of the tennis court.

Time: 1931.76

There are all sorts of things that we learn

Time: 1933.54

in these rather low stake scenarios.

Time: 1935.68

That's the key theme here.

Time: 1937.35

So before I continue,

Time: 1938.54

I just want to point to a tool that anyone can use,

Time: 1941.66

but in particular, the less playful of the group.

Time: 1944.86

And I would put myself into this category.

Time: 1947.37

Again, I'm not somebody who really engages

Time: 1949.04

in spontaneous play.

Time: 1951.092

I enjoy sports.

Time: 1952.45

I enjoy exercise,

Time: 1953.62

but that is distinct from play

Time: 1954.99

because the sports and exercise that I engage in,

Time: 1957.06

I take pretty seriously.

Time: 1958.15

They're not low stakes for me.

Time: 1959.89

I put actually I put a lot of importance on them.

Time: 1961.98

Which is I'm saying all this,

Time: 1962.82

I probably should put a little less importance on them

Time: 1964.53

and have a little more fun with those.

Time: 1966.28

And yet what I'm about to tell you is that

Time: 1969.259

anyone and everyone can benefit from engaging

Time: 1973.37

in a bit more of this playful mindset,

Time: 1976.54

the playful mindset is not necessarily

Time: 1978.61

about smiling and jumping around or being silly.

Time: 1981.97

That's not it at all.

Time: 1982.803

It's not Tigger character

Time: 1984.68

from "Winnie the Pooh" necessarily.

Time: 1986.28

It could be,

Time: 1987.37

but it's really about allowing yourself

Time: 1990.92

to expand the number of outcomes

Time: 1994.09

that you're willing to entertain

Time: 1995.43

and to think about how you relate

Time: 1996.56

to those different outcomes.

Time: 1997.96

So what this means is putting yourself

Time: 2000.02

into scenarios where

Time: 2001.17

you might not be the top performer, right?

Time: 2004.04

Playing a game that you're not really that good at.

Time: 2006.14

I had this experience recently,

Time: 2007.353

friends that like to play cards,

Time: 2009.25

they like to do some low stake gambling.

Time: 2010.96

This is non illegal gambling ring.

Time: 2012.42

They play for trivial things.

Time: 2014.21

And I generally don't buy into the game.

Time: 2018.22

I generally don't play

Time: 2019.61

mostly because they end up winning and taking

Time: 2022.04

whatever it is that I have.

Time: 2023.71

But in the mode of assuming a more playful spirit,

Time: 2027.22

the idea would be well,

Time: 2028.33

if the stakes are low enough,

Time: 2030.52

then to play simply for the sake of playing,

Time: 2032.81

because there's something to learn there

Time: 2034.3

about the other people in the group

Time: 2035.84

and about one's self

Time: 2037.27

and how one reacts to things like

Time: 2040.23

someone who's clearly trying to take everybody's money

Time: 2043.44

or somebody who is clearly trying to cheat

Time: 2047.512

or somebody who's clearly very, very rigid

Time: 2050.51

about every last detail,

Time: 2051.73

including how the cards are dealt and shuffled right?

Time: 2054.113

There is learning in this exploration.

Time: 2056.85

And that is at a biological level, the prefrontal Cortex,

Time: 2060.87

starting to entertain different possibilities,

Time: 2063.43

starting to entertain different outcomes

Time: 2065.07

in this low stakes way.

Time: 2066.29

And if you think about it,

Time: 2067.71

that's not something that we allow ourselves

Time: 2069.88

to do very often.

Time: 2071.292

Even if we listen to new forms of music

Time: 2073.7

or we go see new art or new movies,

Time: 2076.35

those are new experiences,

Time: 2077.54

but that's not us making new predictions

Time: 2079.86

about what's going to happen next.

Time: 2081.25

It's not the brain working to figure out

Time: 2083.62

new possibilities.

Time: 2085.06

And so you can immediately see how

Time: 2086.78

just a small increase in your willingness

Time: 2090.07

to put yourself into conditions

Time: 2091.62

where you don't understand all the rules perhaps,

Time: 2093.5

or you're not super proficient at something,

Time: 2096.33

but you enter it

Time: 2097.48

because it is low stakes.

Time: 2099.36

And because there is information to learn

Time: 2101.09

about yourself and others

Time: 2102.29

could start to open up these prefrontal cortex circuits.

Time: 2105.3

And when I say open up,

Time: 2106.56

I don't mean that literally there's an opening

Time: 2108.33

in your skull.

Time: 2109.163

What I mean is that your prefrontal cortex

Time: 2112.02

can work in very rigid ways.

Time: 2113.56

Meaning if A then B,

Time: 2115.11

if I go down this street turn left and go that way to work,

Time: 2118.08

it is fast.

Time: 2118.913

If I go down the other street, it's slow.

Time: 2120.13

If there's a traffic jam there,

Time: 2121.38

I'm going to go there

Time: 2122.36

but it's starting to explore different possibilities.

Time: 2124.64

And there are very, very few opportunities in life

Time: 2128.34

to explore contingencies in this low stakes way,

Time: 2131.019

such that it engages neuroplasticity,

Time: 2133.31

the prefrontal cortex.

Time: 2134.8

So play is powerful

Time: 2136.67

at making your prefrontal cortex more plastic,

Time: 2140.34

more able to change in response to experience,

Time: 2142.29

but not just during the period of play,

Time: 2145.05

but in all scenarios,

Time: 2146.4

because you get one prefrontal cortex,

Time: 2147.9

you don't get a prefrontal cortex just for play.

Time: 2150.15

You get a prefrontal cortex that engages in everything.

Time: 2153.34

So going forward,

Time: 2154.173

I will layer on some more concrete aspects of tools,

Time: 2158.22

but for now,

Time: 2159.053

if you're somebody that doesn't consider yourself,

Time: 2160.56

particularly playful

Time: 2161.8

consider,

Time: 2162.633

and maybe even engage in just a little bit of play

Time: 2165.431

in some way,

Time: 2166.97

that is of discomfort to you

Time: 2168.81

with the understanding that is increasing

Time: 2170.998

your prefrontal cortical plasticity.

Time: 2173.55

Another really interesting and important aspect of play

Time: 2177.01

is so-called play postures.

Time: 2179.24

These are seen in animals,

Time: 2180.54

and these are seen in humans.

Time: 2182.42

And for those of you that are watching this podcast

Time: 2184.48

on YouTube, I'll do my best to adopt them here.

Time: 2187.45

For those of you that are listening,

Time: 2188.44

you'll just have to imagine them in your mind's eye,

Time: 2191.15

but Jaak Panksepp and indeed Darwin himself,

Time: 2195.04

study these play postures

Time: 2196.83

that all animal engage in.

Time: 2198.538

Perhaps the most familiar one

Time: 2201.71

is seen in dogs and in wolves

Time: 2204.34

where they will lower their head to the ground

Time: 2206.41

and they'll put their paws out in front of them

Time: 2208

and they will make eye contact

Time: 2209.82

with another typically dog or wolf

Time: 2212.678

to so-called call the play.

Time: 2215.36

Now, when they do this posture,

Time: 2217.34

it's obvious that they're lowering themselves.

Time: 2219.07

They're not in an aggressive stance,

Time: 2221.67

because they're lowering their head.

Time: 2223.25

And this is universally known among canines as play posture.

Time: 2226.523

There's some famous videos online.

Time: 2228.44

You can look these up

Time: 2229.56

of dogs actually doing this with bears

Time: 2232.01

that they're confronted with

Time: 2233.35

and the bears, at least in these videos

Time: 2236.238

in exchange also lowering their head,

Time: 2238.74

and there you see bear dog playful interactions.

Time: 2242.208

Now you always have to be cautious with bears in general.

Time: 2245.81

I would say you have to be cautious with bears,

Time: 2247.47

but this speaks to the universality of this bowing,

Time: 2252.01

this sort of the, the,

Time: 2252.86

what some people call the puppy bow

Time: 2254.17

or the play bow that dogs do

Time: 2256.15

turns out that humans do this as well.

Time: 2257.91

Although in a different form,

Time: 2259.22

I'm sure there are some that go into the,

Time: 2260.72

the down-dog play posture,

Time: 2262.14

but more typically when humans want to play,

Time: 2265.24

they will do a subtle or not so subtle head tilt.

Time: 2268.95

The head tilt with eyes open

Time: 2270.82

is considered the universal head

Time: 2272.94

and facial expression posture of play in humans.

Time: 2276

So when two people see one another,

Time: 2277.7

if they are aggressive towards one another,

Time: 2281.528

they will assume certain facial expressions and postures.

Time: 2283.06

But if they're feeling playful towards one another,

Time: 2284.9

oftentimes they'll tip their head to the side

Time: 2286.5

just a little bit and they'll open their eyes.

Time: 2288.24

They might even raise their eyebrows briefly.

Time: 2290.5

This has been seen again and again and again.

Time: 2292.71

Another hardwired feature of so-called play postures

Time: 2296.63

is what's called soft eyes.

Time: 2298.7

When animals are aggressive,

Time: 2300.3

or when they're sad,

Time: 2302.1

they tend to reduce the size of their eye openings

Time: 2304.92

by basically making their eyelids closer together,

Time: 2308.38

somewhat,

Time: 2309.213

by keeping their eyes together

Time: 2310.046

in particular for aggression,

Time: 2310.879

they'll bring their eyes towards

Time: 2311.97

what we call a vergence eye movement.

Time: 2313.35

Bring it towards the center

Time: 2314.49

that actually narrows the aperture of the visual field.

Time: 2318.81

When people or animals want to engage in play,

Time: 2321.31

they tend to open their eyelids somewhat,

Time: 2323.64

and they tend to purse their lips just a little bit.

Time: 2325.9

So it's not like stronger pursing when your lips like this,

Time: 2328.08

it's, pursing their lips.

Time: 2329.26

They'll open their eyes a little bit.

Time: 2330.31

And they'll often do the head tilt as well.

Time: 2331.89

Sometimes with a little bit of a smile.

Time: 2333.7

These are reflexive these are not trained up.

Time: 2336.19

Children do this.

Time: 2337.23

Adults do this dogs, wolves do this.

Time: 2340.5

Even certain birds will do this.

Time: 2342.48

Most birds have eyes on the side of their heads,

Time: 2344.11

but they do a sort of form of this soft eyes approach.

Time: 2346.7

And certainly in raptors,

Time: 2347.95

you see a softening of the eyes

Time: 2349.2

and indeed raptors like Hawks and Eagles

Time: 2351.67

they actually do have a certain form of play,

Time: 2353.63

but only early in life.

Time: 2355.186

The other thing that we see during play,

Time: 2357.84

or what are called partial postures,

Time: 2359.74

partial postures are a kind of play enactment

Time: 2363.146

of postures that would otherwise be threatening.

Time: 2366.19

So a partial posture that we see during play in animals

Time: 2370.27

and humans that relates to aggressive play.

Time: 2372.95

So things like wrestling

Time: 2374.24

or things like rough and tumble play,

Time: 2376.04

which is very common in animals and kids and some adults.

Time: 2379.145

Is that because there's going to be physical interaction

Time: 2382.618

in animals,

Time: 2384.19

what will happen is

Time: 2385.396

they will march toward one another often very slowly,

Time: 2388.71

but rather than having their hair up,

Time: 2391.33

which is we call piloerection,

Time: 2393.11

which is when the hair goes up,

Time: 2394.24

animals do this to make themselves look bigger.

Time: 2395.84

Think of about the, the cat.

Time: 2396.8

That's trying to look bigger or an animal

Time: 2398.62

that's being aggressive, trying to look bigger

Time: 2400.46

in the presence of a foe,

Time: 2404.6

a different animal that they're either going to try

Time: 2406.48

and kill or fight in some way,

Time: 2408.11

even if it's to defend themselves,

Time: 2410.09

partial postures occur when animals will approach

Time: 2412.95

one another,

Time: 2413.783

but they'll keep their fur down.

Time: 2415.54

Humans will do this to,

Time: 2416.539

they will approach during play,

Time: 2419.1

but unless it's highly competitive play

Time: 2421.14

like a football game or a boxing match,

Time: 2423.88

they will actually shrink their body size somewhat.

Time: 2426.37

We have hair on our bodies,

Time: 2428.028

some of us more than others,

Time: 2429.627

and that hair is capable of piloerection.

Time: 2433.29

It can stand up.

Time: 2434.123

That's the hair standing up on end phenomenon,

Time: 2436.605

but most of us don't have enough hair on our bodies

Time: 2439.65

that we can actually use that to make ourselves larger.

Time: 2442.75

So what you see with people who are about to engage in play

Time: 2446.07

is they tend to make their body a little bit smaller

Time: 2448.71

unless they are highly competitive

Time: 2450.47

and highly competitive play

Time: 2452.01

is its own distinct form of play.

Time: 2453.58

That we'll talk about later,

Time: 2454.45

such as during sport, when the stakes are high,

Time: 2456.97

a Super Bowl football game,

Time: 2459.88

I'm revealing my ignorance about sports here.

Time: 2461.688

The Super Bowl as it's typically called

Time: 2464.74

is a very high stakes game, right?

Time: 2467.07

Salaries depend on it.

Time: 2468.22

Sponsorships depend on it.

Time: 2469.55

It's on television,

Time: 2471.23

reputations depend on it.

Time: 2473.38

So that's not really playing a game.

Time: 2475.63

That's playing a very high stakes game

Time: 2477.58

and there you're not going to see these partial postures.

Time: 2480.23

You're not going to see soft eyes and tilting of the head.

Time: 2483.1

At least not between the opposing players on the team,

Time: 2485.33

you're going to see quite the opposite.

Time: 2486.88

Grunting, screaming, shouldering

Time: 2488.99

people not blinking lowering their eyes,

Time: 2491.41

or rather shrinking their eyes down to be,

Time: 2494.45

to appear more aggressive.

Time: 2495.75

These kinds of things

Time: 2496.583

staring right through the other person,

Time: 2498.09

you know verbal threats et cetera.

Time: 2499.52

So that's not really play,

Time: 2501.13

even though we say they're playing a game of football,

Time: 2503.31

it's very high stakes play.

Time: 2505.847

What I'm referring to here

Time: 2506.92

is when it's fairly low stakes.

Time: 2508.77

And we see this again in animals and human.

Time: 2510.63

So there are many, many of these partial postures.

Time: 2512.5

Again, they happen spontaneously.

Time: 2514.72

So if someone ever looks at you

Time: 2516.11

and they tilt their head a little bit

Time: 2517.33

and they raise their eyebrows

Time: 2518.56

and they maybe smile a little bit,

Time: 2519.94

they're looking at you playfully,

Time: 2521.62

that's the universal human exchange of, I want to play.

Time: 2525.87

Do you want to play?

Time: 2527.07

There's another play expression

Time: 2528.81

that is considered the most extreme of the come on

Time: 2531.7

let's play express and postures.

Time: 2534.03

And this is one that's seen in a lot of primates

Time: 2535.9

and indeed in some humans as well.

Time: 2537.8

And that's the eyes wide open

Time: 2539.91

and believe it or not tongue out,

Time: 2541.73

it's the, that kind of silly thing.

Time: 2544.54

That's I don't think that I've ever done that before.

Time: 2546.248

Just that kind of thing

Time: 2548.459

is basically what primate species of all kinds.

Time: 2552.75

And indeed we are old world primates as well,

Time: 2554.671

do when they want to say,

Time: 2557.35

I'm definitely here to play

Time: 2559.45

and that's why I'm here.

Time: 2560.94

Okay it's it has this kind of silly look or connotation.

Time: 2563.93

But if you watch chimpanzees or you look at Bonobos,

Time: 2567.77

or even in the so-called new world monkeys,

Time: 2569.91

which tend to be the smaller monkeys,

Time: 2571.9

old world monkeys tend to be the ones that in general,

Time: 2575.38

see the world as we do,

Time: 2576.72

they have what we call trichromacy.

Time: 2578.75

They're the ones that often can look very human-like.

Time: 2580.85

The new world monkeys tend to be the little ones

Time: 2582.511

in general I'll give you a little trick here.

Time: 2584.68

Little tool based on primatology.

Time: 2587.03

If you see a monkey and it's making very slow movements,

Time: 2590.01

or you see an ape of any kinds, making very slow movements,

Time: 2592.17

very likely to be an old world primate.

Time: 2594.8

If you see a monkey

Time: 2597.5

and it's, it can very quick movements

Time: 2599.53

like it's doing this kind of thing,

Time: 2600.72

like it's like a could be a squirrel monkey

Time: 2603.34

could be a marmoset,

Time: 2604.4

likely to be a new world monkey.

Time: 2606.04

And they don't see the world that the same way we do,

Time: 2607.95

they see the world more like a dog.

Time: 2609.25

They don't really see reds.

Time: 2610.3

They see reds as orange et cetera.

Time: 2612.65

Okay that's not a hard and fast rule.

Time: 2614.08

And I'm sure the primatologists are going to come after me

Time: 2616.81

with whatever primatologists come after you

Time: 2618.67

with monkey biscuits or something like that.

Time: 2620.24

But in general, it's a good rule.

Time: 2621.5

If you're at the zoo and you see a slow moving monkey

Time: 2623.961

with slow deliberate gestures kind of moves its eyes

Time: 2627.16

makes eye contact every once in a while,

Time: 2628.74

those tend to be the old world primates,

Time: 2630.09

those kind of jittery ones

Time: 2631.41

that look like they're really nervous wrapping their tail

Time: 2633.1

and kind of hiding there in a little bundle.

Time: 2634.81

Those tend to be the new world monkeys, okay.

Time: 2637.31

Again, not a black and white type division,

Time: 2640.57

but that'll get you most of the way.

Time: 2644

So the whole purpose of these partial postures

Time: 2646.35

or the tongue out thing

Time: 2648.02

is to limit power in deliberate ways

Time: 2651.42

to really take bodily expressions

Time: 2653.27

that could be portrayed or could be

Time: 2657.468

interpreted as aggressive

Time: 2660.01

or as threatening or as wanting to mate,

Time: 2663.37

or as willing wanting to do anything for that matter.

Time: 2665.8

And to limit the power with which they are expressed

Time: 2669.23

in very deliberate way.

Time: 2670.33

So that's the putting the hair down,

Time: 2672.43

despite getting into a fighting stance.

Time: 2674.49

That's saying let's fight,

Time: 2675.55

but I'm not really here to fight fight.

Time: 2677.21

It's low stakes fighting.

Time: 2678.57

Like if I pin you,

Time: 2679.65

then I'll let you go.

Time: 2680.73

Or if you pin me, then you ought let me go.

Time: 2682.85

And so immediately you can start to see

Time: 2685.09

how play starts to call into action,

Time: 2689.06

social dynamics in which both parties

Time: 2692.52

have to make some sort of agreement

Time: 2694.02

about how high the stakes are.

Time: 2696.34

Now, the failures to do this are also very informative

Time: 2699.7

in how we develop in social groups.

Time: 2701.81

And this also can inform why some people

Time: 2704.24

really play well with others and other people don't.

Time: 2706.37

And some people seem to get along well with groups

Time: 2708.24

and can handle other people.

Time: 2709.073

And some people are very rigid.

Time: 2711.344

In fact, I have an anecdote about this when I was a kid,

Time: 2714.3

we used to play this game.

Time: 2715.24

It's not a game I suggest,

Time: 2716.22

but we used to do what were called "Dirt Clod Wars."

Time: 2718.78

So a friend of mine,

Time: 2719.99

his parents were generally not home in the afternoon.

Time: 2722.358

So we must have been somewhere around 10 or 11 years old.

Time: 2725.7

And we would set up these two big dirt mounds.

Time: 2727.73

We would shovel them to big dirt mounds on two sides

Time: 2729.74

of the yard.

Time: 2730.573

And then we would just take dirt clods

Time: 2732.67

and we throw them at one another and just have

Time: 2734.41

dirt clod wars.

Time: 2735.243

Again, not suggesting this

Time: 2736.84

I'm not responsible for what happens

Time: 2738.78

if you do,

Time: 2739.69

but there were rules

Time: 2740.94

and the rules were, for instance,

Time: 2742.827

you couldn't pack rocks into the dirt clods

Time: 2745.64

and you could run across to the other side

Time: 2749.16

and you could jump on the other person's mound

Time: 2750.5

and you could throw dirt clods in there.

Time: 2751.69

I guess, as the stuff that we thought was entertaining.

Time: 2754.84

But if someone got hit in the head,

Time: 2757.48

generally there was an unspoken rule

Time: 2759.75

that you kind of stop

Time: 2760.77

and see whether or not they were damaged

Time: 2762.35

or not before you'd continue,

Time: 2763.5

you couldn't continue pelting them.

Time: 2764.92

And of course, people broke this rule.

Time: 2766.28

In fact I remember one kid

Time: 2767.31

I'm not going to name him

Time: 2768.24

because actually he's grown

Time: 2769.72

into a very actually prominent and functional adult,

Time: 2772.928

but he got hit once in the head.

Time: 2775.15

And then I think someone had thrown a dirt clod

Time: 2777.07

shortly thereafter,

Time: 2777.938

and all of a sudden he just went into a rage,

Time: 2781.07

picking up rocks and sticks and attacking another kid.

Time: 2783.97

And so clearly that was a case in which

Time: 2786.63

the rules of the game were now being violated,

Time: 2789.03

but it served a very important purpose.

Time: 2790.7

There was, you know,

Time: 2791.533

the typical thing that the,

Time: 2793

there were some tears I think,

Time: 2794.4

as I recall from one kid or the other,

Time: 2796

there was like snot coming out of the nose

Time: 2797.47

and turning bright red, a kid went home.

Time: 2799.63

It was a mess.

Time: 2800.463

The parents had to say something,

Time: 2801.92

or maybe there was a phone call.

Time: 2803.15

I don't quite recall how it got resolved.

Time: 2805.11

But the idea is that

Time: 2806.47

there's an agreed upon set of rules

Time: 2808.92

about how high the stakes are

Time: 2810.4

and what we're all going to do.

Time: 2811.65

And this is separate from sport

Time: 2812.79

where there are clearly defined rules

Time: 2814.55

about what's out of bounds.

Time: 2816.37

What's in-bounds.

Time: 2817.203

What sorts of behaviors will get you a yellow card

Time: 2818.91

or a red card for instance on the soccer field,

Time: 2820.97

all animals including humans

Time: 2823.05

are doing this low stakes contingency testing

Time: 2825.91

and all animals including humans you'll find,

Time: 2828.42

start to up the stakes.

Time: 2829.986

And inevitably in group play,

Time: 2832.7

one member of the group will kind of break rules.

Time: 2835.35

You see this also in puppies.

Time: 2837.37

So for instance, puppies will bite one another

Time: 2839.71

with those sharp little needle-like puppy teeth.

Time: 2841.73

I remember when Costello had those teeth,

Time: 2843.04

those things were so darn sharp

Time: 2844.96

and puppies will yelp

Time: 2846.75

when one of their littermates bites them,

Time: 2848.98

that Yelp actually serves a very important

Time: 2851.7

inhibitory function.

Time: 2852.83

This is well defined

Time: 2853.9

to tell the other one that's too tough,

Time: 2855.63

and this is how animals learn soft bite.

Time: 2858.25

Okay.

Time: 2859.083

If they don't get that feedback from other littermates,

Time: 2861.36

they never actually learn.

Time: 2862.45

What's too hard and what's soft.

Time: 2864.61

And so humans do this as well.

Time: 2866.67

Now you can look at your adult,

Time: 2868.28

counterparts,

Time: 2869.113

and indeed we should probably look at ourselves and ask,

Time: 2870.964

you know did we learn proper play contingency

Time: 2873.6

when we were younger?

Time: 2874.433

Do we tend to take things too seriously?

Time: 2875.88

Do we tend to overreact aggressively

Time: 2878.62

when other people are clearly engaging in,

Time: 2881.31

you know, playful, jabbing or sarcasm

Time: 2884.24

or things of that sort.

Time: 2885.29

So each of you will have a different experience of this,

Time: 2887.03

but the point is that play serves many functions,

Time: 2891.27

it's not just about the self.

Time: 2892.44

It's also about interactions between multiple people.

Time: 2894.77

It's about rule testing

Time: 2895.99

and low stakes contingency

Time: 2897.16

rule breaking also serves an important role

Time: 2899.34

as is with the example of the "Dirt Clod War"

Time: 2902.13

puppies biting other puppies et cetera.

Time: 2904.49

And last but not least,

Time: 2906.2

there are different forms of play

Time: 2908.1

that help us establish who we will become as adults.

Time: 2911.98

One of the more powerful of these

Time: 2913.46

is role play when children and sometimes adults

Time: 2918.54

will take on different roles that are distinct

Time: 2921.36

from their natural world roles

Time: 2923.55

in order to for instance, establish hierarchies.

Time: 2926.65

So someone's going to be the leader

Time: 2927.92

and someone's going to be the follower.

Time: 2929.16

Someone will be dominant.

Time: 2930.1

Someone will be submissive.

Time: 2931.54

Someone will work alone.

Time: 2934

Other people will work in a group.

Time: 2935.68

These kinds of role playing

Time: 2937.39

are again ways in which the prefrontal cortex

Time: 2940.19

has to expand the number of operations

Time: 2942.86

in neuroscience we call these algorithms

Time: 2944.72

that it has to run in order to make predictions.

Time: 2946.53

You have to take in a lot of information

Time: 2948.1

about your environment all the time

Time: 2950.36

and make predictions.

Time: 2951.55

But if you are suddenly cast into a new role,

Time: 2954.48

well then you definitely have to

Time: 2956.03

make even more predictions from a different standpoint.

Time: 2959.22

So these are very powerful for teaching the brain,

Time: 2961.44

how to function.

Time: 2962.61

I had a sister growing up, I still have a sister,

Time: 2964.34

fortunately,

Time: 2965.27

and she and her friends largely played with dolls

Time: 2968.64

and doll houses in the room next door.

Time: 2970.65

And they take on different roles.

Time: 2972.34

In fact, some kids,

Time: 2973.86

if they play alone

Time: 2975.1

will start to take on the role of leader

Time: 2977.57

by taking on an imaginary or creating an imaginary friend.

Time: 2980.96

And, you know, my apologies to my sibling,

Time: 2983.05

but for a long time,

Time: 2984.36

she had an imaginary friend

Time: 2986.28

eventually that imaginary friend disappeared.

Time: 2988.649

I don't know the science around imaginary friends

Time: 2991.15

and what it makes them disappear or not

Time: 2993.03

at what stage of development,

Time: 2994.59

but imaginary friends are pretty common.

Time: 2996.34

And that's just another way of being able to,

Time: 2998.286

you know boss somebody around,

Time: 3000.29

if that's your thing,

Time: 3001.21

or to do engage in cooperative play.

Time: 3003.6

So we can look at this stage of development,

Time: 3006.26

we call childhood

Time: 3007.17

and we can look at each stage of it and we can say, wow,

Time: 3009.92

there are all these different dimensions of play

Time: 3011.82

that really are about testing out

Time: 3014.05

how we feel comfortable or uncomfortable,

Time: 3016.44

how we react good or bad,

Time: 3019.34

how we react with stress or with glee

Time: 3022.83

when others behave in certain ways.

Time: 3024.93

And so what I'm hoping is coming through

Time: 3027.19

is that play is not just about having fun.

Time: 3030.07

Play is about testing.

Time: 3032.29

It's about experimenting

Time: 3033.52

and it's about expanding your brain's capacity.

Time: 3036.07

And that's through early in development,

Time: 3037.44

and it's through throughout the lifespan.

Time: 3039.946

So at this point in the discussion,

Time: 3040.779

I want to take a step back,

Time: 3041.827

look at the biology and neurochemistry of play

Time: 3044.81

just a little bit.

Time: 3046.21

And that really define what is effective play.

Time: 3049.76

If the goal of play is to explore different contingencies

Time: 3052.546

in low stakes environments

Time: 3054.296

and to expand the function of our prefrontal cortex

Time: 3057.87

so that we can see new possibilities

Time: 3059.69

and new ways of being become more flexible, more creative,

Time: 3062.8

more effective outside of the games of play

Time: 3066.205

or the arenas of play, I should say.

Time: 3068.89

Well then we should be asking,

Time: 3071.37

how do I know if I'm playing?

Time: 3072.64

How do I know if I'm playing correctly?

Time: 3074.878

Turns out there's an answer to that.

Time: 3076.837

Earlier I referred to this brain area,

Time: 3079.47

the periaqueductal gray that releases opioids,

Time: 3083.12

endogenous opioids into our brain and body,

Time: 3086.09

and tends to relax us a bit.

Time: 3089.41

It actually is what leads to these things like soft eyes

Time: 3092.41

and head tilts and puppies making, you know,

Time: 3095.315

puppy postures and things of that sort,

Time: 3097.47

and how that opens up the number of different functions

Time: 3101.29

or algorithms that the prefrontal cortex can run,

Time: 3104.44

but there's another piece of the puzzle,

Time: 3105.91

which is for something to genuinely be play and playful,

Time: 3110.29

and for it to have this effect of expanding our brain

Time: 3114.66

and engaging neuroplasticity of really changing our brain

Time: 3117.61

so that we can see and engaging more possible behaviors

Time: 3120.61

and thoughts et cetera.

Time: 3122.15

We also have to have low amounts of adrenaline,

Time: 3125.917

so called epinephrine in our brain in body.

Time: 3128.949

Now the background science for this is quite extensive,

Time: 3132.73

but for those of you that are interested in papers

Time: 3135.21

and manuscripts,

Time: 3136.17

perhaps the best one is a review published

Time: 3138.51

in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

Time: 3140.86

by the very Jaak Panksepp

Time: 3142.5

although he has a co-author,

Time: 3143.81

which is Stephen Siviy,

Time: 3145.39

S I V I Y

Time: 3147.61

I'll provide a link to this in the caption show notes.

Time: 3150.46

And the title of this paper is;

Time: 3152.047

"In Search of the Neurobiological Substrates for

Time: 3154.3

Social Playfulness in Mammalian Brains."

Time: 3157.2

And it's a quite extensive review,

Time: 3159.07

but it basically boils down

Time: 3160.3

to some key findings

Time: 3162.5

whereby, any sorts of drugs or behaviors or scenarios

Time: 3167.81

that increase levels of adrenaline too much,

Time: 3171.67

will tend to inhibit play

Time: 3173.87

and drugs and scenarios and I'm not suggesting

Time: 3178.233

recreational drugs here,

Time: 3179.066

but these were experiments that were done

Time: 3180

in the laboratory setting

Time: 3181.69

that increase the endogenous opioid output

Time: 3185.16

will tend to increase playfulness.

Time: 3187.21

And so really the state of mind

Time: 3189.92

that one needs to adopt when playing is,

Time: 3193.58

first of all you have to engage in the play it,

Time: 3196.26

whatever it happens to be with some degree of focus

Time: 3199.45

and seriousness

Time: 3200.64

and focus and seriousness in the neurobiological context

Time: 3203.37

generally means epinephrine

Time: 3204.808

being able to focus is largely reliant on

Time: 3208.41

things like adrenaline epinephrine,

Time: 3210.21

but also the presence of dopamine,

Time: 3212.11

which is a molecule that generates motivation

Time: 3214.32

and focus in concert with epinephrine,

Time: 3216.82

but also that these endogenous opioids be liberated.

Time: 3220.26

And it's really the low stakes feature of play

Time: 3222.83

that allows those endogenous opioids to be liberated.

Time: 3225.72

What do I mean by that?

Time: 3226.56

Well, if you are very, very concerned about the outcome,

Time: 3229.27

like you've put a lot of money on the table in a given game,

Time: 3232.286

or you're a football player in the Super Bowl,

Time: 3235.856

or you're playing a game for which,

Time: 3237.87

you know, defeating the other person

Time: 3239.626

or your team winning is absolutely crucial to you.

Time: 3243.3

Well then that's not really going to who engage

Time: 3245.55

the play circuitry.

Time: 3246.926

On the contrary,

Time: 3249.35

if you are engaging in those same behaviors

Time: 3251.6

or any other behavior in a way

Time: 3252.87

that you're simply there to explore,

Time: 3255.14

but you don't have high levels of adrenaline in your system,

Time: 3257.33

you're not stressed about the potential outcome.

Time: 3259.332

Well, then that constitutes play.

Time: 3261.676

Now that's somewhat obvious on the one hand

Time: 3264.55

that you take seriously what you take seriously,

Time: 3266.3

and you can be more playful about things

Time: 3267.59

that you don't take so seriously,

Time: 3269.25

but what is absolutely not obvious

Time: 3272.63

is that the state of playfulness

Time: 3275.8

is actually what allows you to perform best

Time: 3278.946

because the state of playfulness

Time: 3281.13

offers you the opportunity to engage

Time: 3283.26

in novel types of behaviors and interactions

Time: 3285.052

that you would not otherwise be able to access

Time: 3288.3

if you are so focused on the outcome. Okay.

Time: 3291.33

So a state of playfulness is absolutely critical,

Time: 3293.94

not just during play,

Time: 3295.56

but during competitive scenarios of any kind.

Time: 3298.3

I actually started to cultivate a practice related to this.

Time: 3301.92

When I was in college,

Time: 3303.23

I had this kind of general practice

Time: 3306.03

of when I wanted to learn something.

Time: 3307.466

I would tell myself that it was the most important

Time: 3311.38

information in the world

Time: 3312.45

and that I was very very interested in it.

Time: 3314.59

I would kind of lie to myself

Time: 3315.93

and say oh, I'm super interested in,

Time: 3317.61

I won't name the topics,

Time: 3318.69

but super interested in this or super interested in that.

Time: 3320.99

And I could sort of delude myself into being hyper focused

Time: 3324.07

on whatever it is

Time: 3324.959

that I was learning

Time: 3326.74

in ways that surprised me.

Time: 3328.595

However,

Time: 3330.21

when we are hyper focused on something

Time: 3333.28

and we are rigidly attached to the outcome,

Time: 3335.99

we can't engage in flexible thinking.

Time: 3338.45

So it's a great tool to be hyper-focused on something

Time: 3341.37

and take it very, very seriously

Time: 3344.665

when we're simply trying to learn things

Time: 3345.95

by kind of rote memory,

Time: 3347.04

learn things and regurgitate,

Time: 3348.22

learn and regurgitate of the sort that,

Time: 3349.627

you know, I'm frankly a lot of schooling involves.

Time: 3352.345

But if we are trying to get better at something,

Time: 3355.45

we sort of hit a wall in athletic performance

Time: 3357.102

or in cognitive performance,

Time: 3358.96

where we're not creative enough,

Time: 3360.28

where we're finding

Time: 3361.36

let's just use a sports example

Time: 3362.77

that, you know we only have a certain number of moves

Time: 3365.66

that we can deploy

Time: 3366.82

or certain number of swings of the racket

Time: 3368.41

that we can deploy.

Time: 3369.6

The way to actually expand your practice

Time: 3372.32

is to engage in this kind of low stakes thinking,

Time: 3374.858

the idea that, well, I'm just going to kind of play and tinker.

Time: 3378.23

I'm going to explore in a way

Time: 3379.73

that it doesn't really matter if the ball goes back over

Time: 3381.67

the net,

Time: 3382.503

doesn't really matter if the ball goes in the hole

Time: 3383.97

and it's counterintuitive because you think,

Time: 3385.94

no, the thing that we need to do is drill and drill

Time: 3388.29

and drill and drill.

Time: 3389.123

And indeed there's a place for that.

Time: 3390.143

But this mode of play with modest levels

Time: 3394.78

of endogenous opioids being released in our system,

Time: 3397.23

plus low levels of adrenaline, right?

Time: 3399.75

Epinephrine low levels of epinephrine and adrenaline

Time: 3401.939

are possible only when the stakes are low enough

Time: 3405.52

that we're not stressed.

Time: 3407.21

Well that combination really allows the prefrontal cortex

Time: 3410.2

to explore different possibilities

Time: 3412.19

in ways that can truly expand our capabilities over time.

Time: 3415.98

Now, this has been seen again and again,

Time: 3417.47

also in the business sector,

Time: 3419.119

some of the more challenging,

Time: 3422.33

or I should say compare additive companies to get jobs at

Time: 3424.84

are very interested in hiring people

Time: 3426.63

that as children were so called tinkerers,

Time: 3429.04

and actually NASA was first famous for this,

Time: 3432.09

that many of the people that achieved great success

Time: 3435.2

in engineering at NASA,

Time: 3436.467

when they looked back into their childhood histories,

Time: 3440

those people tended to be tinkerers.

Time: 3441.57

They were people that would kind of play with things

Time: 3443.38

in way that wasn't about rigidly following a recipe

Time: 3446.85

or an instruction manual.

Time: 3448.15

Great cooks discover new forms of food.

Time: 3451.088

Indeed created entire genres of food

Time: 3454.046

by way of being tinkerers.

Time: 3456.6

Okay, musicians do this.

Time: 3458.287

I grew up playing various sports,

Time: 3460.85

but skateboarding was one that I was particularly involved

Time: 3463.27

in for a long time.

Time: 3464.27

One of the greatest skateboarders of all time

Time: 3465.96

is some of you may recognize his name

Time: 3468.12

as the Great Rodney Mullen.

Time: 3469.68

And Rodney was kind of famous for evolving the sport

Time: 3474.17

and continuing to evolve the sport in ways

Time: 3475.53

that no one could predict

Time: 3476.6

using skateboards and all sorts of ways

Time: 3478.55

that no one had thought of previously.

Time: 3480.29

And of course there are other skateboarders

Time: 3481.5

that did that as well,

Time: 3482.64

but he's particularly well known for that.

Time: 3484.239

And his process is his own.

Time: 3486.82

I can't speak to it too much,

Time: 3487.967

but he was also known as a kind of a tinker

Time: 3490.74

as somebody who would spend a lot of time,

Time: 3492.59

just kind of flipping the board

Time: 3493.99

and just flipping it into the air

Time: 3495.67

and watching the ways in which it flipped

Time: 3497.25

and kind of studying the physics of it really

Time: 3499.92

and expanding on his existing understanding

Time: 3502.84

of what could happen on a skateboard

Time: 3504.8

by way of just playing.

Time: 3506.21

Now, he took it very seriously,

Time: 3507.99

but it's this kind of razor's edge

Time: 3510.12

between taking something very seriously,

Time: 3511.77

but also tinkering and playing and exploring

Time: 3514.21

and just seeing what happens and kind of like,

Time: 3516.03

well, let's just see what happens if we did this,

Time: 3517.716

that mindset is extremely powerful

Time: 3520.79

to export from this thing that we call play

Time: 3523.39

into what we could call more serious endeavor

Time: 3525.78

of one's occupation or sport,

Time: 3527.84

whether or not it's behind a desk

Time: 3529.14

or whether or not it's running around on a field

Time: 3531.14

really for or engineering, any endeavor.

Time: 3533.73

And so the whole purpose of this episode on play

Time: 3537.81

is yes on the one hand

Time: 3539.46

to illustrate the incredible evolutionary utility of play

Time: 3543.55

for setting up the self

Time: 3545.39

and relation of the self to others,

Time: 3546.87

indeed for setting up cultures entirely,

Time: 3548.81

cuz' cultures will watch sport together

Time: 3550.89

or they'll celebrate their team winning.

Time: 3552.64

I mean, World Cup,

Time: 3553.48

I've never been a big soccer fan,

Time: 3554.96

even though my dad is Argentine, but it's incredible.

Time: 3558.14

I mean the entire world kind of lights up

Time: 3560.5

and gets engaged around whether or not their team,

Time: 3562.67

their country is going to win the Olympics also

Time: 3564.52

being another example,

Time: 3565.83

but play and sport are not quite the same

Time: 3568.68

as I've pointed out before.

Time: 3570

And for all of us who are thinking about tools

Time: 3572.04

and things that we can extract from science

Time: 3573.64

to enrich our lives,

Time: 3575.36

I would say for those of you that are already playing

Time: 3577.66

on a regular basis

Time: 3578.99

in one form or another

Time: 3579.96

terrific start to expand other forms of play

Time: 3583.04

in particular forms of play

Time: 3584.23

that involve new groups of individuals.

Time: 3587.1

So if you're somebody that typically plays

Time: 3588.93

one-on-one with somebody,

Time: 3590.05

try to expand into playing as teams.

Time: 3592.06

If you're somebody who only plays alone,

Time: 3594.278

then try to expand into playing in perhaps

Time: 3597.89

one-on-one first and in groups,

Time: 3599.28

this is the way that your brain learns

Time: 3601.05

and evolves and changes and gets better.

Time: 3603.32

And I raised this because

Time: 3604.48

another one of the top 10 questions I get

Time: 3606.53

is how can I keep my brain young?

Time: 3608.46

How can I continue to learn?

Time: 3610.02

How can I get better in school

Time: 3612.64

and sport in life and relationships, et cetera,

Time: 3614.78

emotionally, cognitively,

Time: 3616.668

and on and on and on.

Time: 3618.35

And yes,

Time: 3619.51

there are supplements that can support neuroplasticity.

Time: 3622.01

Yes, there are brain games and apps

Time: 3624.02

that can support neuroplasticity.

Time: 3625.83

But if you really want to engage neuroplasticity at any age,

Time: 3629.468

what you need to do is return to the same sorts

Time: 3631.9

of practices and tools

Time: 3633.61

that your nervous system naturally used

Time: 3636.39

throughout development.

Time: 3637.33

And it evolved over hundreds of thousands of years

Time: 3639.89

to trigger this thing that we call neuroplasticity.

Time: 3642.205

And the reason this is so important is because

Time: 3644.29

it starts to move us away from what some people call hacks.

Time: 3646.85

I define hacks as using one thing for a different purpose

Time: 3649.71

to kind of get a shortcut.

Time: 3651.65

I don't really like the term, frankly,

Time: 3653.61

and I don't like it because it's not grounded

Time: 3657.22

in any biological mechanism.

Time: 3658.708

But when we look at play,

Time: 3660.3

we can say, play is the portal to plastic.

Time: 3663.26

Play at every stage of life

Time: 3664.87

is the way in which we learned the rules

Time: 3666.7

for that stage of life.

Time: 3668.38

And play is the way in which we were able to test

Time: 3671.3

how we might function in the real world context.

Time: 3674.76

So play is powerful.

Time: 3677.6

And we could even say that play is the most powerful portal

Time: 3680.58

to plasticity.

Time: 3681.51

The reason for that is that

Time: 3682.987

yes, this high opioid, low epinephrine or adrenaline state

Time: 3689.29

is what opens up play.

Time: 3690.728

But then inside of the arena of play,

Time: 3693.94

when the prefrontal cortex is running

Time: 3695.42

all these different possibilities in this low stakes way.

Time: 3699.01

But with some degree of focus,

Time: 3700.579

there are a number of other chemicals that are deployed.

Time: 3704

Things like Brain-derived neurotrophic factor

Time: 3706.11

and other growth factors that actually

Time: 3707.89

trigger the rewiring of brain circuits

Time: 3710.69

that allow for it to expand.

Time: 3712.75

And indeed that's what is neuroplasticity.

Time: 3715.56

If you're interested in those chemicals

Time: 3718.03

and kind of arena of things that happen

Time: 3721.14

when one engages in neuroplasticity,

Time: 3723.12

there's a vast literature out there.

Time: 3725.07

But one of the more popular books

Time: 3727.23

that I think is quite good

Time: 3728.16

is from my friend and colleague, John Ratey,

Time: 3730.34

who's a psychiatrist at Harvard Medical School.

Time: 3732.3

That's R A T E Y.

Time: 3733.91

He wrote the book "Spark"

Time: 3735.27

a few years back,

Time: 3737.05

and I think it's still very relevant.

Time: 3738.54

And John talks about

Time: 3741.05

the important role that play

Time: 3742.946

exerts in the neuroplasticity process

Time: 3745.7

and points to a number of different protocols

Time: 3747.37

that one can engage in.

Time: 3748.62

He also points to the importance of navigating

Time: 3751.02

new environments

Time: 3751.853

to not just go on the same hike every week,

Time: 3753.73

or take the same walk,

Time: 3754.67

but actually get into new novel environments.

Time: 3756.87

So you're starting to sense a theme here.

Time: 3758.18

There's novelty,

Time: 3759.088

exploring contingencies,

Time: 3761.02

keeping the stakes relatively low,

Time: 3762.91

et cetera, et cetera.

Time: 3764.17

But these really are the gates

Time: 3765.79

to this holy grail that we call neuroplasticity.

Time: 3768.6

Neuroplasticity as I've talked about in the podcast before

Time: 3771.41

is a two step process.

Time: 3772.68

It involves focusing very intensely

Time: 3775.4

or at least focusing somewhat on whatever it is

Time: 3777.9

that one is trying to learn,

Time: 3779.04

and then engaging in deep rest,

Time: 3781.64

ideally deep sleep in the following nights.

Time: 3783.98

And I've also talked about the benefits of things

Time: 3785.9

like naps and Yoga Nidra so-called NSDR,

Time: 3788.45

Non Sleep Deep Rest

Time: 3789.88

for enhancing or accelerating plasticity.

Time: 3792.58

You can check out the episodes on Focus at hubermanlab.com

Time: 3796.95

or the episodes on How to Learn Faster.

Time: 3799.29

The detail all of those.

Time: 3800.21

We had a newsletter that lists out

Time: 3802.73

all the tools for neuroplasticity enhancing neuroplasticity,

Time: 3806.21

all that is available.

Time: 3807.24

Zero cost to you at hubermalab.com, et cetera.

Time: 3810.71

You can just download that information,

Time: 3813.17

but John's book that newsletter those episodes.

Time: 3816.56

They really point to this two step process

Time: 3818.27

where it's focus and then rest

Time: 3819.158

focus and then rest

Time: 3821.49

and play is its own unique form

Time: 3824.23

of focus and then rest, focus and rest.

Time: 3826.99

It's not the same as learning something for sake of school

Time: 3830.03

or critically trying to learn a motor behavior

Time: 3832.67

for sake of sport.

Time: 3833.92

It's really about expanding the number of things

Time: 3836.56

that you could learn down the line.

Time: 3839.46

Okay, so said once again.

Time: 3841.35

So I just want to make sure it's abundantly clear

Time: 3843.92

play is about establishing a broader framework

Time: 3847.03

within which you can learn new things.

Time: 3848.99

It's not about learning some specific thing.

Time: 3851.36

It's not about the game you happen to be playing.

Time: 3853.96

It's not about the dollhouse that the kids are playing with

Time: 3856.64

so that they can become amazing dollhouse players

Time: 3858.95

when they grow up. Right.

Time: 3860.21

The dirt Clod war that I referred to earlier

Time: 3862.292

for better or for worse

Time: 3864.04

was not about becoming the best dirt clod thrower

Time: 3866.252

or winning the trophy for dirt clods

Time: 3868.78

in the neighborhood.

Time: 3869.613

Although we actually had a trophy

Time: 3870.61

for the best dirt clod team.

Time: 3871.822

Alas, it was not my team that year.

Time: 3874.481

But the point is that you're learning rules

Time: 3877.73

and establishing a broader foundation of practices

Time: 3881.12

that then can learn more things within that context.

Time: 3884.83

Thus far I've tried to convince you

Time: 3886.34

through a combination of data and anecdote and explanation

Time: 3889.7

that adopting a stance of playfulness

Time: 3892.77

and indeed engaging in play

Time: 3894.41

on a somewhat regular basis

Time: 3896.07

could be beneficial to you

Time: 3897.17

regardless of circumstances or goals.

Time: 3899.675

If I haven't done that already,

Time: 3902.21

what I'm about to tell you,

Time: 3903.22

hopefully will push you over the line.

Time: 3905.72

It turns out that when you look across the kingdom

Time: 3909.55

of all animals,

Time: 3911.27

what you find is that animals

Time: 3914.41

that engage in playful behaviors

Time: 3916.64

for the longest period of time

Time: 3919.05

are also the animals that have the greatest degree

Time: 3922.04

of neuroplasticity,

Time: 3923.652

the brain and nervous system's ability

Time: 3925.84

to change in response to experience,

Time: 3928.75

put differently,

Time: 3929.81

animals that only play for a very small fraction

Time: 3932.81

of their entire life

Time: 3934.38

have very rigid brains that don't learn new things,

Time: 3937.85

whereas animals that play for a long period

Time: 3941.28

throughout their life

Time: 3942.48

have very plastic brains.

Time: 3944.92

And there's even some evidence that's at this point,

Time: 3947.99

largely anecdotal,

Time: 3948.97

but there's some data starting to emerge

Time: 3951.2

that adults that maintain a playful stance

Time: 3954.55

that engage in things again

Time: 3956.1

that are low stakes contingency exploring

Time: 3962.28

important enough that people focus

Time: 3964.99

and that people pay attention to what they're doing,

Time: 3966.81

but that they are not, you know,

Time: 3968.55

filled with adrenaline, you know,

Time: 3969.97

freaked out about the outcome being A or B,

Time: 3972.71

they're not super, super competitive,

Time: 3974.3

maybe just a little bit competitive

Time: 3975.7

or not competitive at all.

Time: 3977.47

That allows for more ongoing plasticity.

Time: 3980.84

And one of the people that comes to mind

Time: 3983.28

in thinking about this is of course the Physicist,

Time: 3985.85

and I should say the Great Physicist, Richard Feynman,

Time: 3988.85

Nobel Prize Winner Professor at Caltech

Time: 3990.822

was involved in the Manhattan Project,

Time: 3993.43

but was also known for being a lifelong tinker, right?

Time: 3998.19

He also is a mischievous tinker.

Time: 4000.87

If you read any of the books about Feynman or by Feynman

Time: 4003.877

"Surely You're Joking."

Time: 4004.71

Mr. Feynman, or "What Do You Care What Other People Think?"

Time: 4007.1

these are wonderful short stories,

Time: 4008.34

mostly about Feynman,

Time: 4009.8

doing things like

Time: 4010.982

picking all the locks at the Los Alamos Laboratory

Time: 4014.94

and putting all the top secret documents

Time: 4016.57

out on the floor of the office

Time: 4017.91

so that when people came men in the morning,

Time: 4019.22

they were all out there.

Time: 4020.183

Obviously they weren't released to the general public.

Time: 4023.17

He didn't want to threaten national security,

Time: 4025.11

but playing pranks like that.

Time: 4026.95

And actually Caltech,

Time: 4028.62

I don't know if this is still the case,

Time: 4029.85

but Caltech where he was employed

Time: 4031.2

was always known for doing very

Time: 4033.1

technologically challenging pranks.

Time: 4036.86

They're not known for their athletic pros at Caltech,

Time: 4039.34

sorry, Caltech,

Time: 4040.51

but they were known for example,

Time: 4042.721

disrupting the scoreboard at the Rose Bowl in Pasadena,

Time: 4046.77

for instance,

Time: 4047.603

and things of that sort

Time: 4048.45

through technological feats, at least at the time

Time: 4051.24

required a lot of playfulness

Time: 4053.6

and technological pros.

Time: 4055.26

So if you look in science or you look in art,

Time: 4057.89

or you look in medicine or you in any domain,

Time: 4060.3

what you find is the people that continue to evolve

Time: 4063.31

new practices

Time: 4064.414

tend to be people that were tinkerers.

Time: 4066.212

People that are very creative,

Time: 4068.72

tend to be people that are unafraid

Time: 4071.24

of exploring things in a, this low stakes way.

Time: 4074.11

They're not so rigidly attached to the outcome

Time: 4076.35

that they have to do everything perfectly all the time.

Time: 4078.5

Now they might cloak these playful behaviors

Time: 4081.17

so that their final works

Time: 4082.7

always look perfect or always look incredible,

Time: 4085.1

but they have this kind of playful nature about them.

Time: 4087

I would venture even say that the,

Time: 4088.71

the street artist Banksy, for instance,

Time: 4090.94

obviously an incredible artist puts a ton of thought

Time: 4093.08

and preparation into their work,

Time: 4095.732

but there's a kind of playfulness to the whole thing too,

Time: 4099.92

of using two dimensional paintings

Time: 4102.37

in concert with three dimensional city dwellings

Time: 4104.3

in ways that,

Time: 4105.133

you know I think that most people hadn't previously,

Time: 4106.96

there were other people like Christo and, you know,

Time: 4108.82

artists of that sort that did that.

Time: 4110.41

But I think Banksy is kind of recognized

Time: 4112.85

as the modern, the modern rendition

Time: 4115.6

of that kind of playfulness using cities in ways

Time: 4118.01

that most people don't use cities

Time: 4119.19

using art in ways that most people don't use art,

Time: 4122.53

for instance.

Time: 4123.76

So to go back to the example of Feynman.

Time: 4126.04

Feynman was somebody who learned to paint

Time: 4128.88

and draw quite well into his sixties.

Time: 4130.723

He was somewhat famous or infamous,

Time: 4133.26

I should say for Bongo Drumming on the roof of Caltech.

Time: 4136.3

I say infamous because

Time: 4137.95

he was known also for doing that naked.

Time: 4140.49

Something that certainly not in concert

Time: 4142.58

with the ethical standards and behaviors

Time: 4144.74

of universities today.

Time: 4145.99

But Feynman had this playful spirit as a child.

Time: 4150.24

He had that playful spirit as a teenager,

Time: 4151.9

and he had that playful spirit as an adult.

Time: 4154.43

And that's one of the whole hallmarks of Feynman

Time: 4156.85

was that he wasn't just a rigid physicist

Time: 4160.04

who could explain things clearly to the general public.

Time: 4162.3

He always carried through this playful spirit

Time: 4165.9

and in some of his writings,

Time: 4167.3

he pointed to the fact that that playful spirit

Time: 4169.21

was something that he worked very hard

Time: 4171.12

to continue to cultivate in himself

Time: 4174.36

because it was the way in which he could see

Time: 4176.34

the world differently

Time: 4177.55

and to indeed make great discoveries

Time: 4179.9

in the field of physics,

Time: 4180.96

but also to kind of evolve his relationship

Time: 4183.28

to life more generally.

Time: 4184.23

And so he comes to mind

Time: 4185.324

as a prominent example of somebody who did this.

Time: 4188.275

And if I could achieve anything with this episode,

Time: 4190.876

besides teaching you something about the biology of play,

Time: 4193.21

it would be to teach you about the utility of play.

Time: 4196.254

Again, I'm,

Time: 4197.37

don't consider myself a particularly playful person

Time: 4200

by nature,

Time: 4200.9

but I've tried over the years to adopt

Time: 4203.11

this stance of exploring things that are, you know,

Time: 4206.58

very focused on contingencies of different kinds,

Time: 4209.27

but keep the stakes low enough

Time: 4210.77

that I can have some fun doing them.

Time: 4212.31

And I like to think that it's benefited me somewhat.

Time: 4215

Now, I'd like to drill a little bit further

Time: 4216.43

into this thing that we called neuroplasticity.

Time: 4218.45

Again neuroplasticity is the brain and nervous system's

Time: 4221.99

ability to change in response to experience.

Time: 4224.38

And I should just say that throughout the entire lifespan,

Time: 4227.44

the nervous system can change very quickly

Time: 4229.5

in response to negative experiences.

Time: 4231.94

We can almost all engage in what's called one trial learning

Time: 4234.82

where if something really terrible or traumatic happens

Time: 4237.24

to us,

Time: 4238.073

our nervous system will rewire almost immediately,

Time: 4240.92

at least within a few days,

Time: 4242.51

such that we tend to want to avoid the experience

Time: 4245.21

that led to that trauma.

Time: 4247.6

Now the whole business of why people return to things

Time: 4250.46

that are traumatic to them

Time: 4251.64

is a whole other issue.

Time: 4252.51

There are books about things like trauma bonding.

Time: 4255.63

There's the so-called repetition compulsion

Time: 4258.42

from psychoanalysis

Time: 4259.49

that people go back into trauma to retest

Time: 4261.66

and gain new opportunities to overcome the trauma,

Time: 4264.22

et cetera, et cetera.

Time: 4265.053

But in general, what I'm referring to here is,

Time: 4267

you know,

Time: 4267.833

you have a bad experience at the swimming pool

Time: 4269.19

when you're a kid

Time: 4270.023

where someone holds your head underwater too long,

Time: 4271.057

and then you just don't want to get back in the water.

Time: 4273.04

That's one trial learning of sorts,

Time: 4275.101

that of course can be overcomed

Time: 4277.354

through proper exposure therapy

Time: 4279.56

or someone that you trust taking you there,

Time: 4281.68

or any number of behaviors

Time: 4283.72

that allow you to overcome that,

Time: 4285.22

be that particular scenario and experience something new

Time: 4287.89

in that same context.

Time: 4289.94

But across the lifespan,

Time: 4292.17

the learn earning of new things, new contingencies,

Time: 4294.64

new possibilities

Time: 4296.152

occurs very differently

Time: 4297.97

from about age zero when we're born

Time: 4300.49

until about age 25 and thereafter.

Time: 4303.03

So from about, about,

Time: 4304.85

I want to emphasize approximately age 25 onward

Time: 4307.672

neuroplasticity occurs through the process

Time: 4310.72

that is exactly as I described before, focus, rest,

Time: 4314.17

focus, rest, we focus very intensely.

Time: 4316.81

We can't do the thing.

Time: 4317.84

We can't do the new movement.

Time: 4319.01

We can't do the golf swing.

Time: 4320.22

We can't learn the math.

Time: 4321.514

We try, we try, we try, we try, we sleep a few nights.

Time: 4324.75

And then all of a sudden we can do it, right,

Time: 4325.973

because the rewiring actually occurs during deep rest

Time: 4328.6

or naps,

Time: 4329.52

but mostly during deep sleep.

Time: 4332.43

From birth till about out age 25 however,

Time: 4335.32

we can learn things, new things,

Time: 4337.67

and new contingencies,

Time: 4339.8

not just negative things

Time: 4341.25

and traumatic things

Time: 4342.293

through somewhat passive exposure to those things, right.

Time: 4345.51

I will never forget the first time

Time: 4347.51

that we went on a family trip to Washington DC.

Time: 4350.94

And we went to the Smithsonian.

Time: 4352.16

I got to see the, the old fighter planes.

Time: 4354.07

And I think,

Time: 4354.903

I think the Kitty Hawk

Time: 4356.52

or the first one of the first planes was there.

Time: 4358.43

Anyway, I obviously my recollection isn't terrific.

Time: 4360.73

My hippocampus is, is flailing on that one,

Time: 4363.54

but I'll never forget the trip.

Time: 4364.81

And I'll never forget who went.

Time: 4366.01

And I think I was probably eight or nine years old.

Time: 4368.061

It's embedded somewhere in my memory.

Time: 4370.57

And so just through passive experience

Time: 4373.06

and my focusing on the things that excited me

Time: 4374.931

about that trip,

Time: 4376.55

I have a recollection of that experience.

Time: 4378.26

I didn't have to deliberately focus.

Time: 4380.41

I didn't, wasn't telling myself focused,

Time: 4381.98

you're going to need to remember this trip someday

Time: 4383.59

and you're going to be podcasting about this,

Time: 4385.1

you know, in 39 years, or whenever

Time: 4387.95

again, I forget exactly how old I was.

Time: 4389.62

But the key feature here is that

Time: 4392.61

the developing brain is able

Time: 4394.91

to learn through passive experience

Time: 4397.82

because the neurons, the nerve cells in the developing brain

Time: 4401.553

are much more over-connected

Time: 4405.78

than they will be later in life.

Time: 4407.77

The way to think about this is sort of,

Time: 4409.01

if you use Google maps,

Time: 4410.22

as I do too often I think

Time: 4413.54

when I drive,

Time: 4414.83

there are a number of roads and pathways

Time: 4416.54

that would get you from point A to point B.

Time: 4418.44

We could imagine those as neural circuits,

Time: 4421.54

or we could imagine neural circuits as those roads.

Time: 4424.503

Early in development, the nerve connections

Time: 4428.16

are much more extensive.

Time: 4429.78

It it's like having a Google maps

Time: 4432.05

that where everything is connected to everything

Time: 4433.99

through tiny little cross streets.

Time: 4435.27

And the whole thing is just a complete mess.

Time: 4437.58

But then by taking particular routes

Time: 4439.58

of behavior of thought of emotion,

Time: 4442.98

certain routes become well established.

Time: 4446.25

And the other routes that are not taken

Time: 4448.274

simply disappear.

Time: 4450.29

Now in the biological context in the brain,

Time: 4452.71

we call that process pruning.

Time: 4454.8

And the simple way to envision this is early in development.

Time: 4457.41

You have many, many more neurons than you will have

Time: 4460.41

as an adult.

Time: 4461.46

Those neurons are extensively interconnected,

Time: 4463.811

and approximately 40% of those interconnections

Time: 4467.5

will disappear by the time you're 25 years old,

Time: 4470.06

they are gone.

Time: 4471.24

They are actively removed through processes

Time: 4473.9

that involve things like glial cells that come in

Time: 4476.55

and literally sneak their little processes

Time: 4479.13

in between neurons at the synapse,

Time: 4481.32

which are the points of contact and communication

Time: 4483.09

between neurons and push those apart,

Time: 4485.03

even eat neurons, right?

Time: 4487.33

There's some incredible work from for instance,

Time: 4490.25

Beth Stevens' Lab at Harvard Medical School,

Time: 4492.42

showing that glial cells go in and eat synapses

Time: 4496.72

that are not functional for that particular circuit.

Time: 4500.38

Now, what this tells us is that much of our learning

Time: 4503.85

during development is the removal of incorrect connections,

Time: 4507.21

but it also involves the strengthening of connections

Time: 4509.88

that are going to serve certain emotions, certain functions,

Time: 4513.67

motor functions, cognitive functions, et cetera.

Time: 4517.77

The process of play

Time: 4520.3

is largely a process of engaging pruning

Time: 4523.64

of neural connections

Time: 4524.95

and strengthening of the remaining connections.

Time: 4528.02

I'm sure that many of you have heard the term

Time: 4529.55

fire together wire together.

Time: 4531.282

That phrase is often incorrectly attributed

Time: 4535.46

to the great Donald Hebb, who indeed was great.

Time: 4537.79

Did incredible work a psychologist from Canada

Time: 4540.1

who established a lot of the basic cellar learning rules

Time: 4542.81

for learning and memory,

Time: 4544.38

but it was the also great Dr. Carla Shatz,

Time: 4548.21

who is now at Stanford

Time: 4549.77

and was at Berkeley and Harvard as well,

Time: 4552.64

but who is at Stanford Medical School

Time: 4554.91

who coined this term fire together wire together.

Time: 4557.21

Indeed that's what happens

Time: 4559.02

when children play

Time: 4560.531

when adolescents play

Time: 4562.78

and when young adults play,

Time: 4564.67

whether or not it's social play

Time: 4566.308

or play with an object,

Time: 4567.94

whether or not it's a sport or a play of any kind

Time: 4570.84

imaginary play imaginary friend play,

Time: 4574.12

there is a strengthening of certain neural connections

Time: 4576.5

and a pruning away of up to 40%,

Time: 4579.27

perhaps even more of connections

Time: 4581.098

that are not necessary for certain types of behaviors,

Time: 4586.46

emotions, and thoughts.

Time: 4588.2

And what this means is

Time: 4589.63

that it is through the process of play,

Time: 4592.36

that we become who we are as adults.

Time: 4594.75

And as I mentioned earlier,

Time: 4595.86

it is through the process of play

Time: 4597.921

that we are able to adjust who we are as adults.

Time: 4601.64

Now, there are bounds on this process,

Time: 4604.071

as far as I know there's never been a reported case

Time: 4607.47

of an individual who had a hyperplastic

Time: 4610.12

or I should say a brain that was as plastic in adulthood

Time: 4615.54

as it was in childhood.

Time: 4617.55

But what this tells us is that

Time: 4619.85

what we do in the process of play as children

Time: 4623.56

is really how we set up the rules

Time: 4625.04

for how we behave as adults in almost all domains,

Time: 4628.15

which is really incredible.

Time: 4629.49

And of course the reassuring thing is that

Time: 4631.26

playing as an adult

Time: 4632.57

will allow you to expand on those neural circuits.

Time: 4635.09

You can literally grow new connections.

Time: 4637

Some of you may be saying,

Time: 4637.88

does it create new neurons

Time: 4639.158

for better or for worse?

Time: 4641.4

It does not seem that many new neurons are added

Time: 4644.92

to your brain in adulthood.

Time: 4646.31

There are some papers that report a few neurons

Time: 4648.97

in certain brain areas, isolated brain areas,

Time: 4651.57

but by and large,

Time: 4652.403

most of the rewiring of neural connections

Time: 4654.1

is the removal of certain connections.

Time: 4656.02

This process we're calling Pruning

Time: 4657.68

and the strengthening of the remaining connections

Time: 4660.5

that make those kind of Google maps roads.

Time: 4662.34

And the analogy I laid out before

Time: 4663.86

thicker and more robust think about as

Time: 4665.97

taking little trails

Time: 4667.18

and turning them into roads, then paving those roads.

Time: 4670.43

Then turning those roads into highways,

Time: 4672

then putting up more lanes on those highways

Time: 4674.72

and eliminating all the small little back-country roads

Time: 4677.32

that one could take.

Time: 4678.68

And again,

Time: 4679.513

this is an analogy for what is happening

Time: 4681.19

at the level of neuro circuitry.

Time: 4683.02

Now,

Time: 4683.853

one of the key findings that has emerged from the literature

Time: 4688.23

is children that have been subjected to trauma

Time: 4693.28

or immense amounts of stress of any kind,

Time: 4696.566

have a harder time both engaging in play,

Time: 4699.71

but also a harder time accessing neuroplasticity later

Time: 4702.62

in life.

Time: 4703.72

The good news is this is not a permanent effect.

Time: 4706.28

And we'll talk about some of the ways to overcome that

Time: 4708.276

in a moment,

Time: 4709.58

but this should make sense to you

Time: 4712.1

because earlier we talked about

Time: 4714.24

how a high level of adrenaline

Time: 4717.14

epinephrine in the brain and body

Time: 4718.82

actually inhibits blocks the circuits

Time: 4722.03

in the brain and body that generate play behavior.

Time: 4725.4

And when I say that,

Time: 4726.233

I mean that in a very concrete way,

Time: 4727.93

that epinephrine and adrenaline

Time: 4730

can actually suppress the sorts of circuitry

Time: 4732.69

that can lead to things like soft eyes

Time: 4734.92

or tongue out, or the head tilt,

Time: 4737.87

or what we called partial postures

Time: 4740.49

of being able to engage in,

Time: 4742.202

you know, a rough and tumble play,

Time: 4744.51

but not take that to the point of outright aggression

Time: 4746.89

and damaging the other person or them damaging you.

Time: 4750.23

So when I say that,

Time: 4751.92

you know, trauma and stress can inhibit neuroplasticity

Time: 4755.43

by way of inhibiting play

Time: 4757.271

at a deeper neuro biological level.

Time: 4759.58

What I'm really saying is

Time: 4760.8

that the high levels of adrenaline

Time: 4762.55

that are generated from trauma and stress actually

Time: 4765.06

shut down the circuits

Time: 4766.68

that allow a child or a young adult

Time: 4769.63

to enter the game of play

Time: 4770.92

or in the game of play

Time: 4772.6

in the same way that a child or young adult

Time: 4775.41

who didn't have high levels of adrenaline

Time: 4777.74

in their system could possibly engage in.

Time: 4781.04

Now,

Time: 4781.873

the good news is that many of the existing trauma therapies

Time: 4784.79

that are out there now,

Time: 4785.623

including things like EMDR

Time: 4786.99

Exposure Therapy, Cognitive Behavioral therapy,

Time: 4789.59

and on and on,

Time: 4790.61

including some of the therapies

Time: 4793.85

that are more neurochemical,

Time: 4795.22

things like ketamine, or are more engineering based,

Time: 4798.473

things like Transcranial magnetic stimulation for instance,

Time: 4802.15

many of those are paired with forms of talk therapy

Time: 4805.58

that are really about the same thing that play is about,

Time: 4808.42

which is exploring different contingencies.

Time: 4810.292

It's about exploring different types

Time: 4813.52

of emotional experiences

Time: 4815.59

as they relate to the same sort of scenario

Time: 4818.16

that created the trauma.

Time: 4819.4

And we did an entire episode on fear and trauma,

Time: 4821.94

and I recommend you check out that episode.

Time: 4823.77

It's easy to find again at hubermanlab.com,

Time: 4825.92

it's on YouTube, Apples, Spotify, et cetera, et cetera,

Time: 4828.11

very easy to find.

Time: 4829.01

And there I talk all about trauma treatments

Time: 4831.082

and the various kinds of trauma treatments

Time: 4833.29

that are out there,

Time: 4834.123

their efficacy in different scenarios and traumas and so on.

Time: 4838

But the point I'd like to make now is that

Time: 4840.05

the reason why children who experience a lot of trauma

Time: 4842.76

and stress have limited plasticity later on

Time: 4845.882

is because the neurochemical substrates

Time: 4848.96

that are created from trauma and stress,

Time: 4851.42

because after all

Time: 4852.253

stress is epinephrine and epinephrine is stress.

Time: 4854.92

Those are inseparable

Time: 4856.67

and the way in which it more or less shuts down

Time: 4859.42

or at least inhibits suppresses

Time: 4862.342

those play circuits.

Time: 4864.485

And again the reassuring thing

Time: 4865.719

is that by engaging in play as adults,

Time: 4867.46

we can reactivate some of those circuits

Time: 4869.46

and reopen the plasticity.

Time: 4871.18

In fact one very prominent trauma treatment now,

Time: 4874.72

especially for people that have been subjected

Time: 4877.25

to very severe traumas,

Time: 4878.679

in the ongoing sense,

Time: 4880.41

meaning traumas that went on for many, many years

Time: 4883.25

is to get them to engage in play

Time: 4886.04

in things like dance

Time: 4887.57

in basically getting them to engage

Time: 4889.84

their bodily movements

Time: 4890.86

in ways that they would otherwise

Time: 4892.65

not feel comfortable to engage in.

Time: 4894.52

And I find this area so interesting

Time: 4896.68

because on the face of it, you could say,

Time: 4898.52

oh, that's kind of, you know,

Time: 4899.47

is that really biomedical treatment?

Time: 4900.95

You know, you're taking people who traumatized

Time: 4902.32

and having them dance.

Time: 4903.45

I mean, it seems kind of silly on the one hand,

Time: 4905.48

depending on your, you know,

Time: 4907.168

your particular orientation.

Time: 4910.98

But on the other hand,

Time: 4912.32

it's actually quite profound and quite grounded

Time: 4916.23

in the mechanisms by which the brain circuits change.

Time: 4919.44

So again,

Time: 4921.05

back to this original principle,

Time: 4922.37

which is that play, isn't just one portal to plasticity

Time: 4925.69

play is the fundamental portal to plasticity

Time: 4928.86

and that play and dance

Time: 4930.22

and exploration of novel movements,

Time: 4932.14

exploration of novel athletic movements

Time: 4934.496

are the route

Time: 4936.43

by which we access new ways of thinking

Time: 4939.28

new contingencies.

Time: 4940.597

And I find it wonderful that the trauma release

Time: 4944.2

and the psychiatric and psychology community

Time: 4947.12

are exploring things like play and dance

Time: 4950.97

and other forms of reopening these circuits

Time: 4954.3

because indeed we would all love

Time: 4956.776

for there to be a magic pill

Time: 4958.9

by which trauma could be erased

Time: 4960.32

and new memories could be laid down

Time: 4961.91

or a device that could do that.

Time: 4963.57

But frankly, if you ask me or a number of my colleagues,

Time: 4966.8

whether or not that's likely to happen anytime soon

Time: 4968.76

in an effective way,

Time: 4969.593

I think the short answer is going to be no,

Time: 4971.91

that there are going to be chemicals

Time: 4973.37

and things that can augment and support that process,

Time: 4977.24

but that there's not going to be just a magic pill

Time: 4980.01

that will suddenly reverse trauma altogether.

Time: 4982.08

That it's always going to be a case whereby

Time: 4984.57

shifts in neurochemical states

Time: 4986.256

are going to have to be combined

Time: 4987.8

with new ways of thinking and new behaviors.

Time: 4989.84

And I find it wonderful in reassuring

Time: 4991.9

that people are looking at play and play behavior

Time: 4994.6

as a not just one tiny shard of possibility there,

Time: 4998.67

but that it might actually be the main driver

Time: 5001.03

and a highly productive lever

Time: 5003.4

by which to rewire the traumatized brain.

Time: 5006.01

So if you're like me, you might be thinking,

Time: 5007.709

okay, I'm willing to be more playful.

Time: 5010.876

I'm willing to explore,

Time: 5012.54

play as a portal to plasticity.

Time: 5014.289

And that all makes good sense,

Time: 5017.175

but what should I play?

Time: 5019.28

What should I do?

Time: 5020.56

Well, we've already established that you want to keep

Time: 5023.69

your adrenaline low.

Time: 5024.66

You have to keep the stakes slow enough

Time: 5026.67

that you're not going to get totally consumed by the outcome.

Time: 5029.632

Now, for some people who are highly competitive,

Time: 5032.38

that's going to be challenging.

Time: 5033.67

And yet I don't want to make it seem

Time: 5035.42

as if you can't be competitive during play.

Time: 5037.73

There are many forms of competitive play

Time: 5039.96

that because you are a competitive person,

Time: 5043.4

allow you to drive great joy from that competitive play.

Time: 5047.23

I have a friend who's particularly good at horseshoes.

Time: 5050.09

I'm not particularly good at horseshoes,

Time: 5051.54

but whenever we play horseshoes,

Time: 5052.98

I can tell he's out there to crush me on horseshoes.

Time: 5057.57

And it's just one of these things where, you know,

Time: 5059.52

I can tell he derives great pleasure

Time: 5061.46

from crushing me at a game of horseshoes.

Time: 5063.583

I can't say because I haven't actually done

Time: 5067.016

the microdialysis,

Time: 5067.849

which is a way of extracting chemistry

Time: 5069.44

from the brain in real time,

Time: 5071.13

nor have I recorded from his brain or image it in a scanner,

Time: 5074.42

whether or not he has high levels of epinephrine

Time: 5076.72

or low levels of epinephrine.

Time: 5078

During those games of horseshoes,

Time: 5079.07

I suspect his low levels of epinephrine

Time: 5081.04

and high levels of dopamine, especially when he wins.

Time: 5083.76

And he has, he wins every time.

Time: 5085.73

But the win is that you can be competitive during play,

Time: 5089.35

provided that you were enjoying yourself.

Time: 5092.03

Okay, you can be competitive

Time: 5093.44

provided that you were enjoying yourself.

Time: 5095.519

There are particular forms of play

Time: 5098.53

that lend themselves best to neuroplasticity.

Time: 5101.91

And those particular forms of play again,

Time: 5104.63

are not designed to necessarily just engage

Time: 5107.12

the plasticity that allows you to perform that behavior,

Time: 5110.02

but rather to expand the number of possibilities

Time: 5113.26

for your brain to change in general throughout life

Time: 5116.55

and the two major forms of those

Time: 5119.06

for which there's good Peer Reviewed Research

Time: 5121.29

is to engage in novel forms of movement,

Time: 5124.66

including different speeds of movement.

Time: 5127.2

So let's say for instance, you're somebody who runs.

Time: 5129.93

I happen to like running.

Time: 5130.88

I try and run three times a week.

Time: 5132.308

And generally when I run, I run forward,

Time: 5135.48

I don't run backward, although recently,

Time: 5137.08

because I've become very excited

Time: 5138.6

about the work of so-called "Knees Over Toes Guy,"

Time: 5141.35

his name is Ben Parker,

Time: 5143.02

but he goes by "Knees Over Toes Guy" on Instagram.

Time: 5145.75

I've never met him,

Time: 5146.583

but we've exchanged a few messages back and forth.

Time: 5149.15

And some of his practices involve

Time: 5151.73

walking backwards or doing sled poles backwards.

Time: 5155.54

I found these to be very beneficial

Time: 5157

for my back and for my,

Time: 5158.81

you know, interior tibialis

Time: 5160.27

and some things that have really helped

Time: 5161.75

with my posture and so forth.

Time: 5163.96

But in general, when I run, I run forward,

Time: 5165.57

I don't tend to run backward that much.

Time: 5167.33

And I might do that for a few minutes at the end,

Time: 5169.13

but not so much throughout the entire run.

Time: 5172.61

Running doesn't lend itself to a lot of novel forms

Time: 5175.58

of movement, lateral movements.

Time: 5176.86

So for the nerds out there movement in the Sagittal plane

Time: 5180.97

or angled movements,

Time: 5182.136

but it does appear that things like dance or sports

Time: 5185.73

where you end up generating a lot of dynamic movements

Time: 5189.15

where there's jumping,

Time: 5190.1

where there's movement at different angles,

Time: 5192.07

where there's ducking, where there's leaping,

Time: 5194.36

that basically involve a lot of dynamic movement.

Time: 5196.93

And aren't just strictly linear.

Time: 5199.11

Those seem to open the portals for plasticity.

Time: 5202.108

And that's because they mimic a lot of the brain circuitry

Time: 5206.42

that is associated with play.

Time: 5208.52

And the reason for that is the way in which

Time: 5210.3

those dynamic movements

Time: 5211.46

and movements of different speeds

Time: 5213.35

engage the vestibular system.

Time: 5215.03

The balance system,

Time: 5215.98

the vestibular system is in the inner ear,

Time: 5217.96

relates to the cerebellum,

Time: 5219.4

which translates the mini brain.

Time: 5220.457

You got a little mini brain in the back of your brain.

Time: 5222.58

It brings together visual information in a very direct way.

Time: 5225.919

I talked a lot about this in the episode

Time: 5228.17

on how to learn faster.

Time: 5229.58

So if you want to go in depth on how vestibular

Time: 5231.658

and different types of motor movements can open plasticity,

Time: 5234.43

I talk a little bit more, I should say a lot more there,

Time: 5236.74

but suffice to say that engaging in play

Time: 5241.01

that has a lot of dynamic movement

Time: 5242.36

or movements of different speeds,

Time: 5243.65

things like dance,

Time: 5244.53

things like sports,

Time: 5245.363

like soccer where you're moving in different dimensions.

Time: 5247.098

That tends to be very conducive

Time: 5249.679

to what we would call play related circuitry

Time: 5252.36

provided you don't take it too seriously.

Time: 5254.61

You don't get those high levels of epinephrine.

Time: 5256.23

Now for those of you that

Time: 5257.748

are also interested in non-physical

Time: 5261.31

or non-athletic forms of play

Time: 5263.64

that can really expand plasticity.

Time: 5265.579

There's some very interesting research

Time: 5267.53

about the game of Chess.

Time: 5269.29

I don't play the game of Chess.

Time: 5270.82

I've played a few times.

Time: 5271.95

I confess I don't know how to move all the pieces.

Time: 5273.95

So I'm not going to try and describe that here,

Time: 5275.401

but I've always wanted to learn Chess.

Time: 5276.827

And I think after reading some of the Peer Reviewed Research

Time: 5279.37

about chess and play and neuroplasticity,

Time: 5281.43

now I understand why

Time: 5282.92

there's a really nice paper

Time: 5285.13

published in the International Journal of Research

Time: 5287.16

in Education and Science in 2017.

Time: 5289.92

And the title of this paper is,

Time: 5291.252

"Is Chess Just a Game or Is It a Mirror

Time: 5295.03

That Reflects a Child's Inner World?"

Time: 5298.43

That's a very, a very intense title

Time: 5300.11

for a biologist like me,

Time: 5301.564

but this paper is so interesting

Time: 5304.8

because what it really points to is

Time: 5306.14

the fact that in a single game chess,

Time: 5309.57

you have at least as I understand two players,

Time: 5311.871

and those two players are moving pieces on the chess board,

Time: 5317.17

for which each piece can do different things, right?

Time: 5320.45

Can move in different ways under different scenarios,

Time: 5322.56

but they're different rules for different pieces.

Time: 5324.3

And so each player actually has to assume

Time: 5326.61

multiple identities during the same game.

Time: 5329.75

And each of those identities has different rules

Time: 5332.17

and ways of interacting.

Time: 5333.56

So in a way we can think of chess as one game,

Time: 5336.39

but actually chess is a kind of a substrate

Time: 5340.49

for exploring multiple roles for different characters.

Time: 5343.82

And this is quite a bit different

Time: 5345.23

than for instance, video games

Time: 5346.71

where somebody has their favorite video game player,

Time: 5349.66

or they have an avatar.

Time: 5350.87

And they're always in the same role.

Time: 5352.34

It's also quite a bit different

Time: 5353.65

for when you engage in any kind of play

Time: 5355.49

where you are yourself,

Time: 5356.57

you're just being you in that game.

Time: 5358.65

And so now I'm highly incentivized to explore chess.

Time: 5362.111

You see quotes out there, for instance,

Time: 5365.55

things like "Chess is life or jiu jitsu is life."

Time: 5369.1

I always assumed that that meant that someone's entire life

Time: 5372.82

was chess

Time: 5373.653

or their entire life was jiu jitsu for instance,

Time: 5376.712

but in reading over the research about chess in particular,

Time: 5380.95

but at also certain forms of martial arts,

Time: 5382.78

also certain forms of dance.

Time: 5384.27

What one finds is that

Time: 5386.2

indeed those games are life

Time: 5388.53

in the sense that they involve adopting multiple roles

Time: 5392.93

and exploring contingencies in a number of different ways.

Time: 5395.69

So there are some games

Time: 5396.88

that allow you to explore a much vaster landscape

Time: 5400.48

of movements or of mental roles

Time: 5402.57

or of ways of engaging in strategic movement

Time: 5405.62

as is the case with chess.

Time: 5407.34

And so when you hear that,

Time: 5408.393

you know, activity blank is life.

Time: 5410.932

It often reflects the passion for that activity,

Time: 5413.719

but I think looked at differently.

Time: 5415.55

It also reflects the fact that that activity

Time: 5418.323

is a portal through which you can explore life

Time: 5421.52

through many, many different lenses.

Time: 5424.16

And I think that that's especially powerful

Time: 5426.374

in terms of thinking about how play

Time: 5428.33

can be leveraged for plasticity.

Time: 5429.99

So for those of you that are interested in leveraging play

Time: 5433.01

for neuroplasticity and expanding your mind,

Time: 5436.2

if you will,

Time: 5437.72

I highly recommend picking an activity

Time: 5440.2

that will allow you to adopt different roles

Time: 5443.592

within that activity

Time: 5444.425

where it's not rigidly linear.

Time: 5445.68

This is actually a way in which

Time: 5447.45

I start to depart from this modern and important,

Time: 5451.53

but somewhat narrow idea.

Time: 5453.86

That exercise is the only route to plasticity.

Time: 5456.993

Yes, it's true.

Time: 5458.4

I have Nobel Prize winning colleagues

Time: 5460.313

that swim for two miles a day

Time: 5462.7

and have done on that for a long time.

Time: 5464.07

And they will tell you,

Time: 5464.95

I always think more clearly after my swimming

Time: 5466.504

and I certainly in my experience after a good run

Time: 5470.58

or a good workout, my mind seems to work best.

Time: 5473.203

Unless of course, that workout was very, very intense.

Time: 5475.81

I've talked about this before.

Time: 5476.87

If you do work out very, very hard

Time: 5479.19

in whether or not it's aerobic

Time: 5480.37

or resistance training or sport of any kind,

Time: 5483.157

you brain won't function as well afterwards,

Time: 5485.43

mostly because of the diversion of oxygen

Time: 5487.56

to tissues away from your brain,

Time: 5489.9

you actually are getting less oxygen to your brain,

Time: 5491.88

but in general,

Time: 5492.713

most of us feel that if we exercise regularly,

Time: 5494.33

our brain functions better.

Time: 5495.825

But there are activities that extend beyond linear exercise,

Time: 5499.652

beyond just generating the same sets of movements

Time: 5503.06

over and over again,

Time: 5503.893

when or not it's exercise or not.

Time: 5505.4

And that's really what play is.

Time: 5507.4

Play is about dynamically exploring different kinds

Time: 5511.6

of movements,

Time: 5512.433

dynamically exploring different kinds of thoughts,

Time: 5514.59

dynamically exploring different kinds of roles

Time: 5517.8

that one could adopt.

Time: 5518.911

And that is the way that the brain learns new things.

Time: 5522.6

So I encourage you to explore chess.

Time: 5524.61

I intend to learn chess this year.

Time: 5525.97

I'm very excited to do that.

Time: 5527.37

Now, if you already play chess

Time: 5529.2

and you are an expert chess player,

Time: 5531.09

you actually will derive less benefit

Time: 5533.86

in terms of this play induced neuroplasticity

Time: 5536.98

than you would, for instance,

Time: 5538.19

if you went out,

Time: 5539.023

and I don't know, played a game of soccer

Time: 5541.04

or did something that was very novel

Time: 5542.41

for your nervous system,

Time: 5543.63

because in that novelty

Time: 5544.732

and in that exploration of new behaviors

Time: 5548.09

and new ways of thinking,

Time: 5549.373

you are opening the portal to plasticity,

Time: 5551.553

whereas in doing what you already know how to do

Time: 5554.18

and trying just to perform better and better at it,

Time: 5556.803

you will get better at chess,

Time: 5558.77

but again, that's just chess.

Time: 5560.8

You are not expanding the realms

Time: 5562.82

in which you can become more plastic,

Time: 5566

that you are able to learn new things

Time: 5568.48

in relationship,

Time: 5569.45

in life, in finance and friendship, et cetera.

Time: 5572.04

In researching this episode,

Time: 5573.54

one of the most interesting areas I discovered

Time: 5576.28

was this notion of personal play identity,

Time: 5579.49

personal play identity is a term

Time: 5581.93

that at least to my knowledge was coined by

Time: 5583.98

a Turkish researcher by the name.

Time: 5586.95

And forgive me, I'm going to mispronounce this

Time: 5589.06

is Gökhan Güneş

Time: 5591.54

G O K H A N,

Time: 5594.06

last name G U N E S.

Time: 5596.54

And forgive me Gökhan

Time: 5598.05

and if we have any Turkish speaking members of the audience,

Time: 5601.43

please put the correction in the comment section on YouTube,

Time: 5605.963

make it fanatic so I can understand what it is.

Time: 5609.02

Please I'd love to correct it and apologies,

Time: 5611.65

or who knows if I got it right

Time: 5613.49

then it was pure luck.

Time: 5614.712

Gökhan Güneş has coined this term personal play identity,

Time: 5619.163

and the key role that personal play identity

Time: 5624.48

establishes in who we see ourselves as being,

Time: 5628.43

and not just in the context of play.

Time: 5630.58

Personal play identity

Time: 5631.94

has four well defined dimensions.

Time: 5634.51

And I should say that if you're interested

Time: 5635.83

in learning more about this,

Time: 5636.88

the paper that I found particularly informative

Time: 5641.58

is published in Current Psychology

Time: 5644.2

and the title is "Personal play identity

Time: 5646.17

and the fundamental elements in its development process."

Time: 5649.3

And the author of course,

Time: 5650.452

is Gökhan Güneş G U N E S last name.

Time: 5652.79

This is from 2021.

Time: 5654.05

So recent review.

Time: 5656.57

There are four components to personal play identity,

Time: 5658.971

how you play,

Time: 5662.41

your personality,

Time: 5664.404

socio culture and environment.

Time: 5667.72

So that's the third one that's together,

Time: 5669.24

socio culture environment,

Time: 5670.35

and economics and technology.

Time: 5672.88

Now that sounds somewhat complex,

Time: 5674.51

and this paper is somewhat complex,

Time: 5675.97

but basically what it says is that

Time: 5677.622

we bring together certain aspects of ourselves

Time: 5682.41

and how we react to different place scenarios

Time: 5685.21

when we're younger.

Time: 5686.043

And we bring that forward into the world.

Time: 5688.41

In all context as adults.

Time: 5690.72

To illustrate this,

Time: 5691.77

I'm going to ask you a question,

Time: 5693.75

when you were a child,

Time: 5695.26

let's say 10 years old,

Time: 5698.29

would you have considered yourself competitive?

Time: 5701.37

Would you have considered yourself

Time: 5702.85

somebody who's cooperative?

Time: 5704.25

And realize of course

Time: 5705.083

that those are not mutually exclusive,

Time: 5707.01

you could be competitive and cooperative.

Time: 5709.01

Would you consider yourself somebody

Time: 5710.65

that preferred to play alone

Time: 5712.62

or preferred to play with one or two close friends?

Time: 5715.68

Or were you somebody that really enjoyed playing

Time: 5718.23

in large groups?

Time: 5719.471

Here's a key one.

Time: 5721.6

Were you somebody that enjoyed

Time: 5724.15

playing the leader in one moment

Time: 5726.09

and was equally okay

Time: 5728.36

with being a follower at a later moment?

Time: 5731.01

Were you okay with having your role switched

Time: 5733.37

midway through a game?

Time: 5735.21

Were you get up upset or be delighted

Time: 5738.57

or not care at all about having to switch teams

Time: 5741.2

during the middle of game

Time: 5742.033

because your team was winning right?

Time: 5744.39

To even things out,

Time: 5745.61

you can imagine how that would play out internally.

Time: 5748.75

You would immediately register

Time: 5750.07

that you must be a valuable player

Time: 5751.43

because you're being moved off the winning team

Time: 5753.12

toward the losing team.

Time: 5754.652

But then again,

Time: 5756.07

you're now being forced to join the losing team.

Time: 5759.21

How did you feel about that?

Time: 5760.883

Were you somebody that was comfortable

Time: 5763.01

with other people breaking rules

Time: 5764.46

or perhaps even yourself breaking rules

Time: 5767

or bending rules,

Time: 5769.14

kind of be able to find term,

Time: 5770.7

or were you somebody that

Time: 5772.54

really needed to know all the rules?

Time: 5774.08

And if everyone didn't rigidly adhere to those rules

Time: 5777.44

was quite disturbed by that?

Time: 5780.452

The number of questions goes on and on and on.

Time: 5782.77

And I will provide a link to a paper

Time: 5784.37

that asks a number of questions

Time: 5786.61

that helps you arrive at a sort of score of sorts

Time: 5789.92

or an index of what Güneş and others have referred

Time: 5793.62

to as personal play identity.

Time: 5795.67

The point is that

Time: 5796.71

if we look back to our early adolescence,

Time: 5799.51

somewhere between 10 and 14 years old,

Time: 5802.53

a peak time for social development,

Time: 5805.2

a peak time for play of various kinds,

Time: 5808.21

a peak time for motor development,

Time: 5810.342

a peak time of psychosocial development,

Time: 5814.1

where we learn where we fit into hierarchies.

Time: 5816.35

As we relate to members of the same sex

Time: 5818.79

of the opposite sex, et cetera,

Time: 5821.26

we can start to get a portal into

Time: 5822.77

how and why we show up to various activities in work

Time: 5826.767

and relationship, et cetera, as adults.

Time: 5829.96

In fact,

Time: 5831.5

I'll venture to say that if we go into that process

Time: 5834.03

for ourselves for five or 10 minutes,

Time: 5835.89

you start to see some remarkable parallels

Time: 5838.09

between the way you were at that stage

Time: 5840.44

and your tendencies and your preferences as adults.

Time: 5844.12

We tend to look at our early childhood experiences

Time: 5846.87

and our families,

Time: 5848.22

and to some degree our friends

Time: 5849.47

in terms of how we become who we become.

Time: 5853.82

I've talked about the incredible work of Allan Schore

Time: 5856.84

on previous episodes of the podcast.

Time: 5858.8

Allan Schore is a psychiatrist

Time: 5861.26

and has done extensive work

Time: 5863.14

on how parent child interactions in particular baby

Time: 5866.052

and mother,

Time: 5867.25

but also baby and father

Time: 5869.48

shaped the brain

Time: 5870.98

and the brain and emotional system's ability

Time: 5873.43

to go from states of elation and excitement,

Time: 5875.593

the so-called dopamine epinephrine type circuitry

Time: 5879.75

to the more warm soothing types of calm interactions

Time: 5884.05

that in broad terms could be described

Time: 5886.05

as more serotonin oxytocin and things of that sort.

Time: 5890.16

That work really points to the key roles

Time: 5892.67

that the caregiver

Time: 5894.032

and the child you

Time: 5896.49

engaged in an early life.

Time: 5898.34

And that is incredible work.

Time: 5899.558

I do hope to host Dr. Schore on the podcast

Time: 5903.17

at some point in the not too distant future,

Time: 5906.02

but equally important of course,

Time: 5908.17

are the interactions that we export

Time: 5910.06

from that early laying down of biological circuitry

Time: 5913.99

and psychological circuitry,

Time: 5915.518

to the way we play by ourselves

Time: 5918.36

and the way we play with others

Time: 5920.262

in small numbers or in great numbers.

Time: 5922.831

And of course it would be the case

Time: 5925.73

that how we played as a 10 or 12 year old

Time: 5928.6

would impact how we behave as a 16 year old

Time: 5931.09

and as a 20 year old and as a 30 year old

Time: 5933.27

and so on and so on.

Time: 5935.181

One of my favorite things about developmental biology

Time: 5938.26

and developmental psychology

Time: 5939.518

is that it is grounded in the fact

Time: 5941.123

that we don't just have a childhood and an adulthood.

Time: 5944.41

There isn't just our child self and our adult self.

Time: 5947.11

And even though there are transitions around

Time: 5949.13

the mechanisms that underline neuroplasticity

Time: 5951.3

at approximately age 25,

Time: 5954.76

it is simply the case that development

Time: 5957.129

is our entire lifespan,

Time: 5959.03

that our lifespan is one long developmental arc.

Time: 5962.59

How long depends on our genetics, our lifestyle

Time: 5965.88

accidents, injury and disease of course,

Time: 5968.73

but it is one long developmental arc.

Time: 5971.6

And so it shouldn't surprise us at all

Time: 5973.33

that how we learn to play as a 10 year old or 12 year old

Time: 5976.14

would impact how we play and interact with people

Time: 5979.05

as a teenager and a young adult

Time: 5981.13

and on and on and on.

Time: 5982.872

And that play is the place in which we explore

Time: 5986.84

in which we learn

Time: 5988.09

play is the substrate

Time: 5989.72

by which our nervous system changes us

Time: 5991.87

from this hyper-connected batch of neurons,

Time: 5995.58

where everything is connected to everything,

Time: 5997.67

more or less

Time: 5998.94

to a brain and nervous system whereby

Time: 6002.6

certain circuits work with immense proficiency

Time: 6005.33

and others are less accessible to us.

Time: 6007.82

But again,

Time: 6008.653

the wonderful thing about the human nervous system

Time: 6011.16

is that because it is plastic

Time: 6014.5

for the entire lifespan

Time: 6016.63

and because these two elements of focus and rest

Time: 6019.33

can be deployed again and again, and again,

Time: 6021.99

just because neural circuits didn't form

Time: 6023.99

does not mean that they can't form later in life.

Time: 6027

And today we've been focusing on how play itself,

Time: 6031.03

the same substrate that we use during development

Time: 6033.38

to become who we are,

Time: 6034.96

is the portal by which

Time: 6036.17

we can change who we are

Time: 6037.54

for the better.

Time: 6038.55

So I hope I've convinced you

Time: 6039.73

that play is an extremely important

Time: 6041.99

fundamental homeostatically regulated

Time: 6044.76

aspect of our nervous system,

Time: 6046.4

which is just a mouthful of nerd speak to say,

Time: 6050.1

play can change your brain for the better.

Time: 6052.58

And that is true for every stage of life.

Time: 6055.27

The recommendation that I make,

Time: 6057.09

and certainly the one that I'm going to direct it myself

Time: 6059.75

as well

Time: 6060.95

is to try and engage in at least one hour

Time: 6064.34

of pure play per week.

Time: 6066.65

Now I came to that recommendation

Time: 6068.518

because of the literature that says,

Time: 6071.2

well, you need to engage something pretty repetitively.

Time: 6073.734

It should be novel.

Time: 6075.61

So this wouldn't be something

Time: 6076.81

that you are exceptionally good at already.

Time: 6079.44

If you insist on doing something

Time: 6081.17

that you're already exceptionally good at,

Time: 6083.12

then you want to really do some free form,

Time: 6085.89

low stakes tinkering.

Time: 6087.6

So make it safe,

Time: 6088.5

but make it free form.

Time: 6090.8

So really explore things with that.

Time: 6092.81

Some people call this beginner's mind.

Time: 6094.52

Although I find that a little abstract,

Time: 6096.09

I like the notion of beginner's mind,

Time: 6097.47

but sort of like,

Time: 6098.303

how do you know if you're in beginner's mind?

Time: 6099.7

I think beginner's mind is sort of the expectation

Time: 6102.01

that you're not going to do it well yet,

Time: 6103.548

but play extends beyond beginner's mind.

Time: 6107.14

Play is really about

Time: 6108.51

not even worrying if you're going to get good at it

Time: 6110.65

or really proficient at it.

Time: 6112.17

It's really about exploring contingencies

Time: 6114.74

with truly low stakes.

Time: 6116.27

That's what will allow you to access

Time: 6117.99

these neurochemical combinations

Time: 6120.05

of elevated endogenous opioids,

Time: 6121.87

low epinephrine, et cetera,

Time: 6123.2

that will open up neuroplasticity.

Time: 6124.998

For those of you that need a little more guidance

Time: 6127.4

on how to play.

Time: 6129.13

There's a book out there

Time: 6130.11

actually learned about this from Tim Ferriss' blog,

Time: 6132.59

it's called "Play It Away: A Workaholic's Cure for Anxiety."

Time: 6135.85

So that's more focused on anxiety.

Time: 6137.17

The author is Charlie Hoehn,

Time: 6139.08

last name H O E H N

Time: 6142.47

will provide a link for it in the show notes and caption,

Time: 6144.327

"Play It Away: A Workaholic's Cure for Anxiety."

Time: 6147.67

But books and other resources aside,

Time: 6150.96

I think one hour of play per week

Time: 6152.85

is a reasonable amount of time

Time: 6155

to engage in dedicated play behavior.

Time: 6157.8

For the purpose of opening up these neural circuits

Time: 6160.79

for plasticity,

Time: 6162.179

the key feature of course,

Time: 6163.57

is to not have immense proficiency in that given activity,

Time: 6167.23

or at least not the way you perform it.

Time: 6169.09

And if you do gain proficiency in that activity,

Time: 6171.738

well, then it becomes something else

Time: 6173.64

it's no longer about play it's performance.

Time: 6175.649

So in that case,

Time: 6177.55

you would then want to adopt a new play behavior.

Time: 6180.74

You'll notice that I largely avoided

Time: 6182.35

using the word fun throughout this episode.

Time: 6185.07

Fun is a somewhat abstract term

Time: 6186.92

and like many emotions and many verbal descriptions

Time: 6191.65

of experience.

Time: 6192.5

It falls short in the context of

Time: 6194.93

a neurobiologic goal discussion about play.

Time: 6197.93

If you have fun, terrific.

Time: 6199.7

Some people might find,

Time: 6200.79

however that engaging in play is kind of uncomfortable.

Time: 6203.78

Well there, your goal then should be to lower your level

Time: 6205.91

of discomfort

Time: 6206.94

by focusing less on the outcomes

Time: 6209

and just simply engaging in the behavior because,

Time: 6211.538

well, I'm telling you that it's good for you,

Time: 6214.08

but hopefully you will tell yourself

Time: 6215.53

that it's good for you

Time: 6216.363

and that you will experience that it's good for you.

Time: 6219.67

The literature certainly points to that.

Time: 6221.74

And the literature certainly points to the fact that

Time: 6224.57

play is the way that we are built.

Time: 6226.8

We are built to play.

Time: 6228.67

We have brain circuits from back to front

Time: 6231.82

and within our body that are there

Time: 6233.59

for play and they don't disappear.

Time: 6235.31

They do not get pruned away

Time: 6237.22

as we go from development to adulthood.

Time: 6239.24

So if ever you needed a neurobiological explanation

Time: 6242.09

for why play is important throughout the lifespan,

Time: 6244.89

it's that.

Time: 6245.91

It's that biology does not waste resources.

Time: 6248.74

It's a extremely efficient.

Time: 6250.3

And were the circuits for play not to be important

Time: 6254.07

in adulthood.

Time: 6254.903

They would've been pruned away, but I guarantee you,

Time: 6257.07

they are there in your brain and nervous system now,

Time: 6259.77

they will be there tomorrow

Time: 6260.76

and they will be there going forward.

Time: 6262.35

So my suggestion is that you use them.

Time: 6264.38

One hour per week.

Time: 6265.97

If you're learning from and or enjoying this podcast,

Time: 6268.059

please subscribe to our YouTube Channel.

Time: 6270.46

That's at terrific zero cost way to support us.

Time: 6273.2

In addition,

Time: 6274.033

please subscribe to the podcast on Spotify

Time: 6276.37

and or Apple

Time: 6277.54

and on Apple,

Time: 6278.373

you have the opportunity to leave us up

Time: 6279.88

to a five star review.

Time: 6281.68

Please leave us comments and suggestions and feedback,

Time: 6284.8

including suggestions for future podcast guests

Time: 6287.93

in the comments section on YouTube,

Time: 6289.58

we do read all those comments.

Time: 6291.684

Please also check out the sponsors mentioned

Time: 6293.96

at the beginning of the episode,

Time: 6295.15

that's the best way to support this podcast.

Time: 6297.7

We also have a Patreon it's patreon.com/andrewhuberman.

Time: 6302.25

And there you can support the podcast at any level

Time: 6304.51

that you like.

Time: 6305.87

If you're not already following us on Twitter and Instagram,

Time: 6308.46

we are hubermanlab on Twitter

Time: 6309.92

and also hubermanlab on Instagram,

Time: 6312.72

on Instagram,

Time: 6313.81

I do short posts that are related to topics

Time: 6316.78

covered on the podcast, but also some additional topics.

Time: 6319.63

So these would include science based tools

Time: 6321.88

for things like focus for sleep,

Time: 6323.41

for learning and many other topics as well.

Time: 6325.89

In previous episodes of the Huberman Lab Podcast,

Time: 6328.07

we often refer to supplements.

Time: 6329.978

Now supplements aren't necessary or correct for everybody,

Time: 6333.56

but many people derive tremendous benefit from them

Time: 6336.13

for things like sleep and focus and so on.

Time: 6339.08

We've partnered with Thorne that's T H O R N E

Time: 6342.14

because Thorn supplements are of the very highest quality

Time: 6344.94

in terms of the ingredients they use.

Time: 6346.6

And the specificity of the amounts of the ingredients

Time: 6349.361

that are listed on the bottle

Time: 6351.07

are actually what is in those bottle,

Time: 6353.41

which is not the case for many supplement brands out there.

Time: 6356.26

If you're interested in seeing the supplements

Time: 6357.71

that I take, you can go to thorne.com/u/huberman

Time: 6362.04

So that's thorne.com/u/huberman.

Time: 6367.8

And those supplements you can purchase at 20% off.

Time: 6370.71

And if you navigate deeper into the Thorne site,

Time: 6373.09

through that URL portal,

Time: 6374.95

you can also get 20% off any of the other supplements

Time: 6377.64

that Thorne makes.

Time: 6378.63

Thank you once again,

Time: 6379.49

for joining me for this discussion

Time: 6381.16

about the incredible biology and psychology

Time: 6383.73

and power of this thing that we call play

Time: 6386.67

and last but certainly not least

Time: 6388.9

thank you for your interest in science.

Time: 6390.659

[Upbeat music]

Copyright © 2024. All rights reserved.