Time Perception & Entrainment by Dopamine, Serotonin & Hormones

Time: 0.35

- Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast

Time: 2.3

where we discuss science and science-based tools

Time: 4.93

for everyday life.

Time: 6.248

[bright guitar music]

Time: 9.25

I'm Andrew Huberman and I'm a professor of neurobiology

Time: 11.9

and ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine.

Time: 14.96

Today we are talking about time perception.

Time: 18.08

Our perception of time is perhaps the most important factor

Time: 21.96

in how we gauge our life,

Time: 24.07

that is whether or not we think we are being successful,

Time: 27.01

whether or not we are failing,

Time: 28.54

whether or not we live in fear,

Time: 30.31

whether or not we live in relation to things

Time: 32.47

in a way that's positive, and the reason for that

Time: 35.44

is that our perception of time is directly linked

Time: 38.39

to the neurochemical states that control mood,

Time: 41.23

stress, happiness, excitement,

Time: 44.07

and of course it frames the way

Time: 46.13

in which we evaluate our past,

Time: 48.72

so whether or not we think of our past

Time: 50.67

as successful or unsuccessful.

Time: 53.47

It frames our present,

Time: 54.67

whether or not we think we are on track or off track.

Time: 57.43

And it frames our sense of the future,

Time: 59.88

whether or not we think we have a bright future,

Time: 62.53

a dim future,

Time: 63.45

or whether or not the future is very uncertain or not.

Time: 67.29

Today, we're going to talk about the science

Time: 68.9

of time perception and we are going to talk about tools

Time: 72.17

and protocols that you can use that can enhance your ability

Time: 77.03

to dilate and contract time.

Time: 79.06

What do we mean by dilate and contract time?

Time: 81.97

We can control the speed at which we experience life.

Time: 85.5

We can slow things down

Time: 87.27

or we can speed our experience of life up.

Time: 89.92

And we can do that in a very direct and dynamic way.

Time: 93.01

It's actually not that hard

Time: 94.49

once you understand how time perception works.

Time: 96.68

So that's where we're headed.

Time: 97.56

I think you're going to come away from today's episode

Time: 99.73

with a lot of new knowledge and certainly with many tools

Time: 103.66

that you can try in your daily life,

Time: 105.33

whether or not that's work, sport,

Time: 106.8

relational, emotional, and so on.

Time: 109.44

Before we begin our discussion about time perception,

Time: 112.46

I'd like to answer some questions that I received

Time: 115

related to the episode on fasting

Time: 117.13

and time-restricted feeding.

Time: 118.89

If you haven't seen that episode,

Time: 120.13

this information should still be of use to you.

Time: 122.87

Time-restricted feeding involves eating

Time: 125.05

for a particular period of time

Time: 126.56

in each 24-hour cycle that's fairly regular.

Time: 129.73

So this would be an eight-hour most often

Time: 132.22

or a 10-hour block.

Time: 133.29

Some people do shorter feeding windows, but regardless,

Time: 136.72

that feeding window is supposed to fall

Time: 138.1

at more or less the same period within each 24-hour day.

Time: 140.84

This has a number of positive effects on gene expression

Time: 143.84

that regulate a number of positive effects

Time: 145.51

on the different tissues of the body, and for some people,

Time: 148.18

not all, but for some people makes weight loss easier

Time: 151.96

because of the way that they are not eating

Time: 154.07

for large periods of each 24-hour cycle.

Time: 156.86

In any event, one of the major questions I got

Time: 159.49

after that episode was do supplements break a fast?

Time: 164.04

And during that episode,

Time: 164.97

I talked about what breaks a fast is highly contextual.

Time: 168.3

It basically boils down

Time: 169.48

to whether or not something you ingest,

Time: 172.26

whether it be liquid or food,

Time: 174.57

increases your resting blood glucose,

Time: 176.5

how much it increases that resting blood glucose,

Time: 179.01

and how long that increase lasts.

Time: 181.8

So you can check out the episode

Time: 183.01

for more about what breaks a fast,

Time: 185.5

but to address this issue about supplements

Time: 188.22

and whether or not supplements in particular break a fast,

Time: 190.95

many of the questions were about Athletic Greens.

Time: 193.59

Athletic Greens is a sponsor of this podcast.

Time: 196.53

It is also a terrific supplement that I'd been taking

Time: 199.07

for more than a decade before this podcast launched.

Time: 202

And many people have been using

Time: 204.39

and continue to use Athletic Greens.

Time: 206.88

Does Athletic Greens break a fast?

Time: 208.96

Well, that will somewhat depend

Time: 210.26

on whether or not your resting blood glucose

Time: 212.43

tends to run high or low, but for most people,

Time: 215.08

including me, because I've measured it,

Time: 217.17

ingesting Athletic Greens does not break a fast

Time: 219.61

and if it happens to break a fast,

Time: 221.21

it would be a very transient break in fast.

Time: 223.84

So without knowing your resting blood glucose levels

Time: 226.58

on an individual basis, there's no way I can say for sure

Time: 229.09

that it doesn't break a fast, but chances are it does not

Time: 231.92

because it doesn't contain much carbohydrate or sugar

Time: 235.42

and it doesn't tend to therefore pull you

Time: 238.72

out of the molecular mill you associated

Time: 241.34

with low blood glucose states.

Time: 244.28

The other question I get is whether or not things

Time: 246.24

like fish oil break a fast, and once again,

Time: 249.6

this will be contextual,

Time: 251.01

but because fish oil is a fat, an essential fat,

Time: 253.93

mainly essential fatty acids, in particular EPA and DHA,

Time: 258.44

those don't tend to raise blood glucose very much.

Time: 260.92

In my case, having measured

Time: 262.79

using a continuous glucose monitor my resting blood glucose,

Time: 265.94

fish oil does not in any way change

Time: 267.75

my resting blood glucose.

Time: 269.68

Chances are it won't do that for most people as well.

Time: 272.19

So does fish oil break a fast? Chances are it does not.

Time: 276.23

And of course people wanted to know

Time: 277.87

about pill-type supplements, you know,

Time: 279.89

caffeine and things that raise dopamine

Time: 282.01

and their vitamins and minerals.

Time: 283.49

In general, if something doesn't contain sugar

Time: 286.27

or much carbohydrate of any kind,

Time: 289.33

it's not going to raise blood glucose very much.

Time: 291.54

Now, of course, protein can raise blood glucose

Time: 293.53

and fat can too as well, although to a lesser extent.

Time: 297.28

So again, this is all contextual,

Time: 299.03

but at least by the logic that I just spelled out,

Time: 302.13

Athletic Greens, fish oil, and most forms of supplements,

Time: 305.82

provided they don't have any sugar or protein content,

Time: 308.74

should not quote unquote break a fast.

Time: 311.74

Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize

Time: 313.56

that this podcast is separate from my teaching

Time: 315.81

and research roles at Stanford.

Time: 317.57

It is however part of my desire and effort

Time: 319.84

to bring zero cost to consumer information about science

Time: 322.237

and science-related tools to the general public.

Time: 325.04

In keeping with that theme,

Time: 326.07

I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.

Time: 328.9

Our first sponsor is ROKA.

Time: 331.18

ROKA makes eyeglasses and sunglasses

Time: 333.03

that are of the absolute highest quality.

Time: 335.66

I've spent a lifetime working on the visual system

Time: 338.08

and one of the key things about our visual system

Time: 340.08

is that it's designed so that when you move into areas

Time: 343.02

where it's sunny or where there are shadows,

Time: 345.67

you can still see things with crystal clarity.

Time: 348.86

Many sunglasses out there have the problem

Time: 351.03

that you have to keep taking them off

Time: 352.67

and putting them back on

Time: 353.52

depending on the overall so-called ambient environment

Time: 356.64

that you're in.

Time: 357.51

ROKA sunglasses have solved this problem

Time: 360.38

and their eyeglasses also have super clarity

Time: 363.86

regardless of overall ambient lighting, as we say.

Time: 366.53

In other words, you see everything very clearly

Time: 368.4

no matter where you are.

Time: 370.51

They also come in a number of different styles.

Time: 372.25

The aesthetics are really terrific.

Time: 373.62

So unlike a lot of performance glasses out there

Time: 375.333

that make people look like cyborgs,

Time: 377.36

you can wear them anywhere.

Time: 378.193

You can wear them to dinner.

Time: 379.08

You can wear them to school or work

Time: 380.37

or in social engagements and you can wear them running

Time: 382.29

and cycling and out doing your various activities.

Time: 385.18

If you'd like to try ROKA glasses,

Time: 386.61

you can go to ROKA that's R-O-K-A, .com

Time: 389.32

and enter the code Huberman

Time: 390.74

to save 20% off your first order.

Time: 393.3

Today's episode is also brought to us by Athletic Greens.

Time: 396.23

Athletic Greens is an all-in-one

Time: 397.69

vitamin mineral probiotic drink.

Time: 400.16

I started using Athletic Greens way back in 2012

Time: 403.17

and so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast.

Time: 405.83

The reason that I started using Athletic Greens

Time: 407.74

and the reason I still take Athletic Greens

Time: 409.27

once or twice every day is that it covers

Time: 411.89

all of my foundational needs for vitamins,

Time: 414.07

minerals, and probiotics.

Time: 415.81

In fact, when people ask me

Time: 417.39

what supplements they should take,

Time: 419.41

if I were going to recommend just one supplement,

Time: 421.17

it would be Athletic Greens because of the enormous number

Time: 423.92

of biological factors that it impacts in a positive way.

Time: 427.77

It has, as I mentioned, vitamins and minerals.

Time: 429.63

The probiotics are really important for the gut microbiome

Time: 432.61

and gut health which is important for the immune system

Time: 435.43

and for brain health and for mood

Time: 437.23

and a number of other important factors

Time: 439.15

including hormones and so on.

Time: 440.96

If you'd like to try Athletic Greens,

Time: 442.42

you can go to athleticgreens.com/huberman

Time: 445.33

to claim their special offer.

Time: 446.86

They'll give you five free travel packs

Time: 448.51

which make it really easy to mix up Athletic Greens

Time: 450.48

while you're on the road or in the car.

Time: 452.27

And they'll give you a year's supply of vitamin D3 K2.

Time: 455.8

Vitamin D3 and K2 have been shown to be really important

Time: 459.51

for a number of important aspects of your immediate

Time: 462.77

and long-term health including blood lipid profiles

Time: 465.36

and a number of other things.

Time: 466.62

Again, go to athleticgreens.com/huberman

Time: 469.45

to get the Athletic Greens, the five free travel packs,

Time: 471.6

and the year's supply of vitamin D3 K2.

Time: 474.66

Today's podcast is also brought to us by InsideTracker.

Time: 477.368

InsideTracker is a personalized nutrition platform

Time: 479.82

that analyzes data from your blood and DNA

Time: 482.21

to help you better understand your body

Time: 483.77

and help you reach your health goals.

Time: 485.88

I've long been a believer in getting regular blood work done

Time: 488.34

for the simple reason that many of the factors

Time: 490.84

that impact your immediate and long-term health

Time: 492.65

can only be analyzed from a quality blood test

Time: 495.31

and it's for that reason that I get my blood work done

Time: 498.08

once every four to six months.

Time: 499.9

Might seem like a lot, but it has been vital

Time: 502.48

in order to keep my health where I want it

Time: 504.93

and to ensure that my health trajectory is heading

Time: 507.34

in the direction that I'd like it to go

Time: 509.05

with each passing year.

Time: 510.94

The other thing about InsideTracker is they have DNA tests

Time: 513.83

which can tell you about the specific makeup of your genes

Time: 516.75

that can impact your particular nutrition,

Time: 520.12

lifestyle, and supplementation regimes.

Time: 522.48

They can also help you steer your immediate

Time: 524.52

and long-term health in the direction that you want to go.

Time: 527.31

They make the whole thing very, very easy.

Time: 529.55

You can have the blood test taken at home

Time: 531.21

or you can go to a local clinic.

Time: 533

Then the results come back and the wonderful thing is

Time: 536.16

the platform, the dashboard that they use

Time: 538.55

walks you through your data

Time: 540.41

and points to specific things related to nutrition.

Time: 543.43

Maybe you should be eating more of certain things,

Time: 545.85

eating less of others, supplementing in certain ways or not.

Time: 549.71

And lifestyle factors like exercise

Time: 552.04

in order to bring the numbers into the ranges

Time: 554

that are right for your immediate and long-term health.

Time: 556.57

If you'd like to try InsideTracker,

Time: 558.02

you can visit insidetracker.com/huberman

Time: 560.67

to get 25% off any of InsideTracker's plans.

Time: 563.38

Just use the code Huberman at checkout.

Time: 566.14

So let's talk about time perception

Time: 568.07

and the most fundamental aspect of time perception

Time: 570.56

is something called entrainment.

Time: 572.74

Entrainment is the way in which your internal processes,

Time: 576.63

your biology and your psychology,

Time: 579.08

are linked to some external thing.

Time: 582.34

And the most basic form of entrainment

Time: 584.44

that we are all a slave to all year round

Time: 588.71

for our entire life are so-called circannual rhythms.

Time: 593.76

We have neurons, nerve cells in our eye, in our brain,

Time: 598.45

and in our body that are marking off the passage of time

Time: 603.28

throughout the year,

Time: 604.42

literally a calendar system in your brain and body.

Time: 607.19

And the way this works is beautifully simple.

Time: 611.32

Light seen by your eyes inhibits, meaning it reduces,

Time: 616.84

the amount of a hormone released

Time: 618.47

in your brain called melatonin.

Time: 621.39

Melatonin has two major functions.

Time: 623.91

One function is to make you sleepy at night

Time: 626.17

and the other is to regulate some of the other hormones

Time: 629.29

of the body, in particular testosterone and estrogen.

Time: 633.46

When we view light,

Time: 635.09

we reduce the amount of melatonin released.

Time: 637.1

In fact, if you wake up in the middle of the night,

Time: 638.93

when melatonin typically is pretty high in your brain

Time: 641.47

and body and you flip on a bright light in the bathroom,

Time: 644.62

your melatonin levels crash down

Time: 646.66

to almost zero and stay there.

Time: 649.55

Light is a very powerful modulator of melatonin

Time: 652.86

and light inhibits melatonin.

Time: 656.42

Throughout the year, depending on where you live,

Time: 659.05

day length varies, and as a consequence,

Time: 662.1

the amount of light from the sun

Time: 664.86

that is available to you varies.

Time: 667.32

So when days are long, the amount of melatonin

Time: 670.97

in your brain and body that's released tends to be less.

Time: 674.42

There's less of it and it's released

Time: 675.95

for shorter amounts of time, okay?

Time: 678.4

Because light inhibits melatonin.

Time: 680.5

When days are very short, the amount of melatonin

Time: 683.46

that's released and the duration that that melatonin exists

Time: 686.65

in your brain and body tends to be much longer.

Time: 689.36

So melatonin correlates with day length.

Time: 693.87

And if we are viewing more light, we have less melatonin.

Time: 697.36

We view less light, we have more melatonin.

Time: 702.7

You see different amounts of light each day

Time: 705.48

but we have a process in our brain and body

Time: 708.93

that averages the amount of light that you're seeing

Time: 711.3

both from artificial sources and from sunlight

Time: 713.84

and measures that off, and it's so exquisitely precise

Time: 718.68

that for a given, say, eight-hour day in the spring,

Time: 723.25

'cause spring in the Northern Hemisphere or elsewhere,

Time: 725.67

you know, days are getting longer,

Time: 728.73

that means that the amount of melatonin

Time: 731.18

is getting progressively less and less

Time: 733.43

and that signal is conveyed to all the systems

Time: 735.7

of your brain and body.

Time: 737.07

And this is why most people, not all,

Time: 738.9

but most people feel like they have more energy

Time: 741.28

in the spring.

Time: 742.9

Conversely, when you have an eight-hour day

Time: 745.86

in the winter, the amount of melatonin

Time: 749.17

that corresponds to that eight-hour day

Time: 751.7

is getting progressively greater and greater because why?

Time: 755.51

Days are getting shorter so melatonin is increasing

Time: 758.66

from day to day to day.

Time: 760.52

Every cell and system of your body pays attention to this,

Time: 762.91

and as a consequence, most people, not all,

Time: 764.68

but most people feel they have a little less

Time: 766.97

or sometimes a lot less energy

Time: 768.62

and a slightly lower mood in the winter months.

Time: 771.52

Now, there are exceptions to this, of course,

Time: 773.83

but the melatonin signal is the way

Time: 776.67

in which your internal state, your mood,

Time: 780.26

your sense of energy, even your appetite is entrained,

Time: 784.91

is matched to some external event.

Time: 787.21

In this case, the event is the rotation

Time: 789.47

of the Earth around the sun.

Time: 791.38

There are other forms of entrainment,

Time: 793.6

meaning the matching of your brain and body

Time: 796.07

to things that are happening in your external environment.

Time: 798.84

One particularly interesting example of this

Time: 800.88

was published last year by Parikh et al. in "Cell Reports,"

Time: 804.01

Cell Press journal, excellent journal,

Time: 806

showing that across the calendar year,

Time: 808.77

the amount of testosterone and estrogen

Time: 811.32

that human beings make varies

Time: 813.78

such that in longer days,

Time: 815.87

they tend to make more testosterone and estrogen

Time: 818.31

than in shorter days.

Time: 819.47

And this was correlated with things like desire

Time: 822.23

to seek out romantic partners or have romantic interactions

Time: 825.13

with their existing partners, even aggression,

Time: 828.26

although not violent aggression,

Time: 830.87

but sense of kind of willingness to argue

Time: 833.02

and to get into kind of combative states

Time: 836.2

and overall energy and mood.

Time: 838.37

This is something that had been hypothesized for a long time

Time: 840.61

but it had never really been cleanly demonstrated.

Time: 842.98

And what they showed was that it's actually the skin

Time: 846.69

that's taking information about the amount of light

Time: 849.01

and converting it into these increases

Time: 850.99

in testosterone and estrogen.

Time: 852.73

Light exposure to the skin, turns out about two hours a day,

Time: 857.53

this was sunlight, in this case, to the upper body,

Time: 859.41

these people weren't naked, they were wearing clothes

Time: 861.11

but their arms were exposed,

Time: 863.01

their upper back and neck and face were exposed,

Time: 865.18

they were not wearing hats, resulted in large increases,

Time: 869.88

significant increases in testosterone and estrogen.

Time: 872.72

Now you could probably export a tool from that if you liked.

Time: 875.63

That's not really what this podcast is about

Time: 877.29

but it's very clear that because the skin is acting

Time: 880.72

as an endocrine orman, organ, excuse me,

Time: 882.93

as kind of a hormone-influencing organ,

Time: 886.16

that getting light on the skin, not just to the eyes,

Time: 889.13

can influence our sense of wellbeing

Time: 891.61

by these hormone pathways, and the threshold there again

Time: 894.62

seemed to be about two hours a day.

Time: 896.01

It doesn't have to be very bright outside.

Time: 897.69

There can be cloud cover and so on.

Time: 899.35

Many people will probably ask

Time: 901.8

will sunscreen inhibit this effect?

Time: 903.64

And it doesn't appear that it does.

Time: 905.64

Obviously prioritize skin health and avoiding skin cancer.

Time: 909.99

Sunscreen is kind of a controversial topic nowadays,

Time: 912.232

maybe the topic for another podcast episode at some point,

Time: 916.37

but nonetheless, what the Parikh et al. study shows

Time: 919.51

and that's most relevant to today's podcast

Time: 921.4

is that we are entrained, we are matched

Time: 924.65

to the external light-dark cycle

Time: 926.45

and as the day length changes, our hormones change.

Time: 929.65

And we can override that with exposure to bright lights.

Time: 933.13

You know, people go sit on tanning beds.

Time: 935.42

That's not a practice I particularly myself engage in,

Time: 938.48

but, you know, there are a number of different ways

Time: 939.6

that people can override these processes.

Time: 941.76

But the point is very simple.

Time: 944.28

The point is that our perception of time is both conscious,

Time: 949.22

you know, it's waiting, watching the clock tick down,

Time: 952.11

and there are these slower, what we call oscillatory,

Time: 954.74

meaning up and down repeatedly, slower oscillatory events

Time: 958.26

related to day length that are influencing our hormones

Time: 961.31

like melatonin, testosterone, and estrogen,

Time: 963.21

and therefore our mood, our outlook, and even our behavior.

Time: 966.44

The next level of time or bin of time, as we say,

Time: 971.69

that we are all entrained or matched to

Time: 974.95

is the so-called circadian time cycle,

Time: 977.64

which is 24-hour rhythm.

Time: 980.09

This is perhaps the most powerful rhythm

Time: 983.61

that we all contain and that none of us can escape from.

Time: 988.7

We all have this circadian clock

Time: 990.93

that resides over the roof of our mouth.

Time: 992.8

The cells in that circadian clock fire,

Time: 995.65

meaning they release chemicals into our brain and body

Time: 999.99

on a very regular rhythm.

Time: 1001.34

So across the 24-hour cycle,

Time: 1004.08

they will be very active at some periods

Time: 1006.36

and less active at others.

Time: 1009.13

Not surprisingly, there are periods of every 24-hour cycle

Time: 1012.27

when we are very active and we tend to be alert

Time: 1014.79

and others when we are asleep.

Time: 1017.07

Now, I've talked a lot about circadian rhythms

Time: 1018.84

and sleep on this podcast previously

Time: 1021.12

and so I don't want to repeat too much of that information

Time: 1023.33

in detail, but I'm just going to give a summary

Time: 1025.15

of how circadian entrainment works

Time: 1027.17

because I haven't really covered that

Time: 1030.06

in the context of time perception.

Time: 1033.41

We have the circadian clock. It oscillates.

Time: 1036.03

It goes up and down once every 24 hours and then repeats.

Time: 1039.83

Every cell of our body has a 24-hour oscillation

Time: 1042.86

in the expression of various genes.

Time: 1044.85

How that works is actually really simple,

Time: 1046.75

elegant, and interesting.

Time: 1049.2

DNA, genes, make RNA. RNA is converted into proteins.

Time: 1055.6

Every cell in our body has this beautiful 24-hour timer

Time: 1059.92

where a gene is expressed.

Time: 1062.15

And the important thing to understand about a given gene

Time: 1065.65

in this context is that that gene is inhibited,

Time: 1069.26

meaning it's reduced, by a particular protein,

Time: 1072.77

by a little biological molecule in that cell.

Time: 1075.85

So the gene gets expressed when there's very little

Time: 1079.44

of that other molecule around.

Time: 1081.37

DNA then becomes RNA.

Time: 1083.51

RNA is translated into a protein

Time: 1086.57

and that protein goes way, way up and the gene shuts down.

Time: 1089.73

But as that protein gets used up

Time: 1092.43

and its levels eventually drop low, low, low, low, low

Time: 1094.66

to zero, the gene cycle kicks in again

Time: 1097.307

and the gene gets expressed.

Time: 1098.57

The RNA gets expressed in the protein again.

Time: 1101.31

This all happens on a 24-hour cycle.

Time: 1103.62

So it's a little built-in timer

Time: 1105.11

in each and every one of our cells.

Time: 1106.64

And I didn't list off the genes,

Time: 1107.92

but for the aficionados out there,

Time: 1109.29

they go by names like PER for period, BMAL,

Time: 1111.98

CLOCK, and all these different things.

Time: 1113.16

We call them the clock genes and those clock genes regulate

Time: 1115.61

a number of different functions.

Time: 1116.59

So every cell in our body has a 24-hour cycle

Time: 1119.4

of gene and protein expression

Time: 1121.52

and the Earth rotates once every 24 hours

Time: 1124.86

and the processes that are happening

Time: 1126.81

in every cell of our body are linked.

Time: 1129.74

They are entrained, as we say,

Time: 1131.55

to the outside light-dark cycle

Time: 1133.18

because morning sunlight, evening sunlight,

Time: 1138.28

and the lack of light in the middle of the night

Time: 1142.24

make sure that the changes,

Time: 1145.2

these oscillations that are occurring

Time: 1146.97

within the cells of our brain and body are matched

Time: 1149.87

to the outside light-dark cycle.

Time: 1151.22

Don't want to go into all the details of how that happens

Time: 1153.7

but there's some very simple tools that one can use

Time: 1156.17

to ensure that your entrainment,

Time: 1158.65

your circadian entrainment, is precise.

Time: 1161.62

And I cannot emphasize enough how important it is

Time: 1164.7

that your circadian entrainment be precise, why?

Time: 1169.15

Because disruptions in circadian entrainment

Time: 1171.87

cause huge health problems.

Time: 1173.3

They increase cancer risk. They increase obesity.

Time: 1176.49

They increase mental health issues.

Time: 1178.85

They decrease wound healing.

Time: 1181.24

They decrease physical and mental performance.

Time: 1183.48

They disrupt hormones.

Time: 1184.8

You want your cells to be linked to the circadian cycle

Time: 1188.66

that's outside you and the circadian cycle outside you

Time: 1191.17

mainly consists of when there's sunlight

Time: 1194.03

and when there is not.

Time: 1194.96

And that's why the simple protocols

Time: 1196.94

to fall out of this whole discussion

Time: 1199.18

about circadian entrainment are the following.

Time: 1201.73

View 10 to 30 minutes of bright light,

Time: 1203.5

ideally sunlight, within an hour of waking

Time: 1206.44

assuming that you're waking early in the day especially.

Time: 1208.65

You wake up early in the day, get outside, see sunlight.

Time: 1210.88

Do that again in the afternoon or around evening,

Time: 1214.01

10 to 30 minutes, depending on how bright it is outside.

Time: 1217.04

Artificial lights throughout the day,

Time: 1219.09

or if you want to be awake and you wake up early

Time: 1221.97

and there's no sunlight outside,

Time: 1223.59

you can of course turn on artificial lights

Time: 1225.42

if you want to be awake but basically you want

Time: 1227

as much bright light, ideally from sunlight,

Time: 1229.314

coming in through your eyes throughout the day.

Time: 1231.69

And then in the evening,

Time: 1232.99

you want as little bright light coming in through your eyes.

Time: 1236.34

I've said this over and over and over again on this podcast.

Time: 1238.77

There's always a lot of negotiations,

Time: 1240.27

but I want to make a few things clear.

Time: 1241.97

Try not to wear sunglasses if you can do it safely.

Time: 1244.3

Fine to wear eye glasses or contacts.

Time: 1246.38

That's not going be a problem.

Time: 1249.61

The light viewing that you do and the avoidance of light

Time: 1252.24

at night set the fundamental layer of your time perception.

Time: 1259.64

One of the best ways to disrupt your perception of time

Time: 1262.48

in the ways that we're going to talk about

Time: 1263.95

in the subsequent portions of the podcast

Time: 1266.28

is to disrupt your circadian clock

Time: 1268.01

and that is not a good thing

Time: 1269.49

for a number of different reasons.

Time: 1271.44

There are other ways

Time: 1272.273

to so-called entrain your circadian clock.

Time: 1274.33

One of the best ways to do that is to engage

Time: 1277.36

in physical activity at fairly regular times of day.

Time: 1280.71

You don't have to do it every day

Time: 1281.76

but if you're going to exercise,

Time: 1283.39

try and exercise at a fairly consistent time of day.

Time: 1286.02

Probably better to exercise than to not exercise,

Time: 1289.08

even if you have to move that time of day,

Time: 1291.41

but light activity and, we'll talk about the third

Time: 1294.74

in a minute, food, are the major ways

Time: 1296.97

that you entrain your internal perception of time

Time: 1300.18

to the external events of the world,

Time: 1303.68

meaning the turning of the Earth

Time: 1305.56

and therefore the exposure to sunlight or not.

Time: 1308.73

So in addition to sunlight viewing in the morning

Time: 1312.86

and throughout the day and avoiding bright light

Time: 1314.42

at night of any kind, not just blue light,

Time: 1316.77

trying to get your activity,

Time: 1318.01

your exercise at fairly regular,

Time: 1320.12

within plus or minus two hours, from each day to the next

Time: 1324.03

is going to have a very positive effect

Time: 1325.92

on so-called circadian entrainment,

Time: 1328.14

and also eating at fairly regular times.

Time: 1330.79

However, this is exciting.

Time: 1333.24

The data mainly point to the fact that you need to eat

Time: 1335.72

within more or less the same time window each day,

Time: 1338.91

not that you always need to eat your meals

Time: 1340.93

at exactly the same time.

Time: 1342.77

So you don't necessarily have to eat lunch at noon

Time: 1346.04

and a snack at four and dinner at eight

Time: 1348.39

in order to keep your circadian entrainment

Time: 1350.69

aligned or sharp.

Time: 1353.48

You could for instance have a small snack at noon

Time: 1355.56

and then eat at two and then have dinner at six

Time: 1357.67

and then a small snack at eight.

Time: 1358.79

It doesn't so much matter when the exact meals fall

Time: 1362.51

so much as that they fall more or less

Time: 1364.1

within a consistent period or phase of each 24-hour cycle.

Time: 1370.56

What happens when this circadian clock

Time: 1373.23

starts getting disrupted?

Time: 1374.37

I mean, this is after all an episode about time perception.

Time: 1377.19

It's not an episode about circadian rhythms and entrainment.

Time: 1380.38

Well, there's a classic study by Aschoff done in 1985

Time: 1385.87

that's now been repeated many times

Time: 1387.64

where they had people go into environments

Time: 1390.13

where they didn't have clocks and they didn't have windows

Time: 1392.51

and they didn't have watches

Time: 1394.05

and they were sometimes even in constant dark

Time: 1395.79

or constant light, and they evaluated

Time: 1398.99

how well people perceive the passage of time

Time: 1402.53

on shorter timescales

Time: 1404.24

and what they found was really interesting.

Time: 1405.79

What they found is that people underestimate

Time: 1408.19

how long they were in these isolated environments.

Time: 1410.21

So after 42 days or so, they'd ask people

Time: 1413.34

how long do you think you've been in here?

Time: 1414.5

And people would say 28 days or 36 days.

Time: 1418.18

They generally underestimated how long they had been

Time: 1421.17

in this very odd environment with no clocks

Time: 1423.4

or watches or exposure to sunlight

Time: 1424.91

or regular rhythms of artificial light.

Time: 1428.34

In addition, they found that their perception

Time: 1431.78

of shorter time intervals was also really disrupted.

Time: 1435.43

So if they ask them to measure off two minutes,

Time: 1438.071

normally people are pretty good

Time: 1439.75

at measuring off two minutes.

Time: 1440.76

People come within, you know, 5 to 15 seconds at most.

Time: 1445.07

If you'd kind of have to sit there and just wait,

Time: 1447.5

you have a pretty good idea of when two minutes is up.

Time: 1449.117

You say two minutes is up.

Time: 1450.65

Well, when people's circadian clocks

Time: 1453.14

or circadian entrainment, I should say, was disrupted,

Time: 1456.11

their perception of time measurement on shorter timescales

Time: 1460.8

of minutes or even seconds was greatly disrupted.

Time: 1464.21

And as we'll see in a couple of minutes,

Time: 1465.75

that actually causes great problems

Time: 1468.42

for how you contend with work,

Time: 1470.28

how you contend with challenges of different kinds.

Time: 1473.1

You want your circadian entrainment to be pretty locked in

Time: 1477.92

or pretty entrained to the outside light-dark cycle

Time: 1480.9

so that your perception of time

Time: 1483.05

on shorter time intervals can be precise

Time: 1486.21

because the ability to perceive time accurately

Time: 1488.91

for the given task or given thing

Time: 1491.25

that you're involved in turns out

Time: 1492.91

to be one of the most fundamental ways that predicts

Time: 1496.12

how well or poorly you perform that thing or task.

Time: 1499.63

So we've talked about circannual entrainment,

Time: 1502.49

the matching of the cells and tissues and organs of our body

Time: 1505.65

to the 365-day journey that the Earth takes

Time: 1509.46

around the sun each year

Time: 1511.35

and we talked about circadian entrainment,

Time: 1513.87

the way that the 24-hour genetic and protein clocks

Time: 1518.23

of each and every one of our cells is matched

Time: 1520.99

to the rotation of the Earth on its axis

Time: 1523.74

and the exposure or lack of exposure to the sun

Time: 1528.2

because of that rotation on its axis.

Time: 1531

Next I'd like to talk about so-called ultradian entrainment.

Time: 1534.825

Ultradian rhythms are rhythms of about 90 minutes or so

Time: 1538.93

and all of our existence is broken up

Time: 1541.67

into these 90-minute ultradian cycles.

Time: 1544.23

When you go to sleep at night,

Time: 1545.47

whether or not you sleep six hours or four hours

Time: 1547.24

or eight hours or 10 hours,

Time: 1549.08

that entire period of sleep is broken up

Time: 1551.74

into these 90-minute ultradian cycles.

Time: 1554.29

Early in the night, you tend to have more slow-wave sleep.

Time: 1556.29

Later in the night, you tend to have more REM sleep.

Time: 1558.35

But nonetheless, your sleep is broken up

Time: 1560.74

into these 90-minute cycles.

Time: 1562.44

However, when you wake up in the morning,

Time: 1564.69

many of the things that you do are governed

Time: 1567.81

by these ultradian rhythms.

Time: 1570.98

For instance, if you were to work,

Time: 1574.15

meaning do math or try and learn a language

Time: 1577.32

or do physical work of any kind or work out,

Time: 1580.94

the 90-minute time block seems to be the one

Time: 1583.82

in which the brain can enter a state of focus

Time: 1587.21

and alertness and do hard work and focus, focus, focus

Time: 1589.68

and then at about 90 minutes, there's a significant drop

Time: 1594.38

in your ability to engage in this mental or physical work.

Time: 1598.09

Now, everybody from, you know, the self-help literature

Time: 1602.45

to the business literature to the pop psychology literature

Time: 1605.42

has tried to leverage these ultradian cycles

Time: 1608.06

by saying if you're going to do something hard

Time: 1610.75

and you want to focus on it, limit it to 90 minutes or less.

Time: 1613.14

And I am one of those people

Time: 1614.19

who's also joined that conversation

Time: 1615.88

and indeed I use 90-minute work cycles,

Time: 1618.13

and I think they are extremely powerful.

Time: 1620.73

One should never expect

Time: 1621.78

that you're going to drop immediately

Time: 1623.19

into a state of high focus at the beginning

Time: 1625.71

and then remain there for 90 minutes.

Time: 1627.64

We all struggle to varying degrees to achieve focus

Time: 1630.91

and motivation and drive within those 90-minute cycles.

Time: 1633.64

But it is true, meaning there is ample literature

Time: 1637.54

to support the idea that after about 90 minutes,

Time: 1641.76

we tend to go into a state of less ability to focus.

Time: 1645.48

So while this isn't time perception per se,

Time: 1648.95

it is again an example of entrainment.

Time: 1651.81

What are we entraining to, right?

Time: 1654.22

Just because we can focus for 90 minutes

Time: 1656.06

and then not so well at 100 minutes or 120 minutes,

Time: 1659.6

what are we entraining to?

Time: 1660.69

Well, what you're entraining to is the release

Time: 1662.49

of particular neurochemicals,

Time: 1664.921

in this case, acetylcholine and dopamine

Time: 1667.23

that allow your brain to focus

Time: 1669.17

for particular periods of time, 90 minutes or so,

Time: 1672.28

and after about 90 minutes or so,

Time: 1675.54

the amount of those chemicals that can be released

Time: 1677.66

tends to drop very low,

Time: 1678.63

which is why your ability to focus becomes diminished.

Time: 1682.81

If one would like to explore more about the kind of backbone

Time: 1685.64

and basis of these ultradian rhythms,

Time: 1688.72

it goes by a different name.

Time: 1689.92

This was originally called the basic rest-activity cycle.

Time: 1693.99

This was proposed many years ago by Nathaniel Kleitman.

Time: 1697.66

It was established to be true

Time: 1699.68

within sleep states as I mentioned before.

Time: 1701.93

Then it was debated for a long time

Time: 1703.47

whether or not these 90-minute cycles also control

Time: 1707.03

our ability to focus and perform work in wakeful states

Time: 1710.04

and it turns out that they do.

Time: 1711.06

Now there's a lot of literature to support that.

Time: 1713.86

I always get the question how do you know

Time: 1715.47

when the 90 minute cycle begins?

Time: 1717.19

In other words, let's say you wake up at 8 AM

Time: 1720.24

and you just finished a 90-minute sleep cycle.

Time: 1722.37

Does that mean that your next 90-minute cycle

Time: 1725.13

where you could do work begins right at 8:01?

Time: 1727.72

No, the interesting thing

Time: 1729.73

about these basic rest-activity cycles,

Time: 1731.76

these ultradian rhythms,

Time: 1733.06

is that you can initiate them whenever you want.

Time: 1736.14

This is not like a circadian rhythm

Time: 1737.86

which is a hardwired unerring signal of 24 hours.

Time: 1743.05

The ultradian rhythms that occur

Time: 1744.68

during sleep are hardwired, unerring.

Time: 1748.87

You don't get the option of making your sleep cycles

Time: 1751.28

120 minutes or five minutes.

Time: 1753.51

You don't get that option.

Time: 1755.69

But if you decide that you want to apply ultradian rhythms

Time: 1758.63

to work and performance, you can set a clock

Time: 1761.44

and decide, okay, now the focus begins.

Time: 1764.27

Now the work begins and this 90-minute cycle is the period

Time: 1767.49

in which I'm going to do work.

Time: 1768.69

And I actually do this, you know,

Time: 1770.26

mid morning and sometimes twice a day,

Time: 1772.04

I do a 90-minute cycle where I limit all distraction

Time: 1775.76

as much as possible, put away my phone,

Time: 1777.37

often turn off the internet as well.

Time: 1778.8

I talked about this in an episode

Time: 1780.57

on kind of an optimal workday, at least for me,

Time: 1784.56

just to give an example of how this might work.

Time: 1786.73

But I want to emphasize again

Time: 1788.91

that these ultradian rhythms are ones that you set.

Time: 1792.09

So you decide I'm going to work for 90 minutes.

Time: 1794.65

What you can't negotiate, however,

Time: 1796.68

is that at about 100 minutes or 120 minutes,

Time: 1799.8

no matter who you are,

Time: 1800.77

you're going to see a diminishment in performance.

Time: 1803.72

You're not going to focus as well.

Time: 1805.94

And that's again because of the way

Time: 1807.31

that these 90-minute cycles are linked to the ability

Time: 1810.05

of the neurons that release acetylcholine and dopamine

Time: 1812.75

and to some extent norepinephrine,

Time: 1814.11

the things that give us narrow focus,

Time: 1815.63

motivation, and drive, the way

Time: 1817.36

that these 90-minute cycles are involved in those circuits.

Time: 1821.95

After about 90 minutes,

Time: 1823.08

those circuits are far less willing to engage

Time: 1825.7

and therefore it's much harder to continue

Time: 1828.6

to focus to a high degree.

Time: 1830.69

Some people like to do multiple 90-minute cycles

Time: 1833.01

per day of focus.

Time: 1833.88

In that case, you need to separate them out.

Time: 1835.62

You can't do one 90-minute cycle,

Time: 1837.27

then go right into another 90-minute cycle,

Time: 1839.06

then another 90-minutes cycle.

Time: 1840.19

You can't cheat these circuits related to acetylcholine

Time: 1843.26

and dopamine and norepinephrine unfortunately.

Time: 1846.29

I suggest that people do no more than three

Time: 1849.22

and ideally it would be two

Time: 1851.02

or just one of these 90-minute cycles.

Time: 1853.38

Why did I say ideally? Well, they are very taxing.

Time: 1856.62

You are in a very narrow tunnel of focus.

Time: 1859.37

So for me, I can do one mid morning.

Time: 1862.04

I can probably do another one in the afternoon.

Time: 1864.27

This is not the kind of work that's like checking email

Time: 1866.41

or text messaging or social media.

Time: 1867.88

This is very focused hard work,

Time: 1869.759

working on hard problems of various kinds,

Time: 1872.73

and this will be different for everybody.

Time: 1874.77

So I recommend that they be spaced

Time: 1876.27

by at least two to four hours

Time: 1878.65

and most people probably won't be able

Time: 1880.69

to handle more than two per day.

Time: 1882.58

There are probably some mutants out there

Time: 1884.28

that could do three or four, but that's exceedingly rare.

Time: 1887.28

I think even one a day is going to feel

Time: 1889.23

like a significant mental investment,

Time: 1891.5

and afterwards you're going to feel pretty taxed.

Time: 1893.87

So now we've talked about circannual,

Time: 1895.81

circadian, and ultradian rhythms,

Time: 1899.07

but we haven't really talked about time perception per se.

Time: 1902.41

We've mainly talked

Time: 1903.41

about the subconscious slow oscillatory ways

Time: 1907.05

in which we are entrained or matched to the year

Time: 1909.79

or to the day and these ultradian cycles

Time: 1912.88

that we can impose on our work

Time: 1915.41

and that we can leverage toward more focus if we like.

Time: 1920.07

But what about the actual perception of time?

Time: 1922.05

What actually controls how fast

Time: 1924.53

or how slowly we perceive time going by?

Time: 1928.3

There are basically three forms of time perception

Time: 1931.09

that we should all be aware of.

Time: 1932.82

One is our perception of the passage of time in the present,

Time: 1935.76

how quickly or slowly things seem to be happening for us.

Time: 1939.53

This is kind of like an interval timer ticking off time.

Time: 1942.66

Tick, tick, tick, tick, tick, tick.

Time: 1944.38

It's either fine-slicing like that,

Time: 1946.36

or tick, tick, tick.

Time: 1949.85

We have interval timers.

Time: 1951.46

I'll discuss the basis of those interval timers.

Time: 1953.75

We also engage in what's called prospective timing

Time: 1956.46

which is like a stopwatch measuring off things

Time: 1958.82

as they go forward.

Time: 1960.26

That might sound a little bit like what I just described

Time: 1962.19

but it's actually a little bit different.

Time: 1964.27

For instance, if I told you to start measuring off

Time: 1968.9

a two-minute time interval into the future,

Time: 1971.84

you could do that pretty well.

Time: 1974.75

But if I told you you had to measure

Time: 1976.01

a five-minute time interval into the future

Time: 1978.41

and you couldn't use any clocks or watches

Time: 1980.3

or your phone or anything like that,

Time: 1982.27

you would have to set the tick marks.

Time: 1985.36

You'd have to decide how many times you were going to count off

Time: 1989.28

during that five-minute time block.

Time: 1991.98

There's also retrospective time

Time: 1994.1

which is how you measure off time in the past.

Time: 1997.72

So if I say, you know, last week,

Time: 2000.44

I know you went to the park,

Time: 2001.78

you did some things with friends, you know,

Time: 2003.3

you went out in the evening.

Time: 2005.36

How long was it between lunch

Time: 2008.06

and when you went to dinner with friends?

Time: 2009.34

You'd probably think, okay, well,

Time: 2010.49

I remember I went to dinner at seven

Time: 2011.94

and we had lunch right around two.

Time: 2013.88

You're using memory to reconstruct certain sets of events

Time: 2017.98

in the past and get a sense of their relative positioning

Time: 2020.98

within time, okay?

Time: 2022.38

So we have retrospective,

Time: 2023.8

current time interval measurements,

Time: 2025.33

and then prospective time measurement into the future.

Time: 2030.2

The beauty of time perception in the human nervous system

Time: 2034.71

is that it boils down to a couple of simple molecules

Time: 2039.18

that govern whether or not we are fine-slicing time

Time: 2043.2

or whether or not we are batching time in larger bins.

Time: 2047.37

Those molecules go by names that maybe you've heard,

Time: 2049.99

things like dopamine and norepinephrine,

Time: 2052.117

neuromodulators, called neuromodulators

Time: 2054.3

because they modulate, they change the way

Time: 2055.873

that other neural circuits work.

Time: 2058.63

Also things like serotonin.

Time: 2061.26

Serotonin is released from a different site

Time: 2063.26

in the brain than dopamine or epinephrin is

Time: 2065.92

and has a different effect on time perception.

Time: 2069.11

So just to give you an example of how things like dopamine

Time: 2072.16

and serotonin can modulate our perception of time,

Time: 2076.37

want to focus on a little bit of literature

Time: 2078.96

that now has been done fortunately in animals and humans

Time: 2081.98

and which essentially shows that the more dopamine

Time: 2086.23

that's released into our brain,

Time: 2089.47

the more we tend to overestimate the amount of time

Time: 2092.6

that has just passed.

Time: 2094.21

Let me repeat that.

Time: 2095.09

The more dopamine that is released into our brain,

Time: 2098.47

the more we tend to overestimate how much time has passed.

Time: 2102.05

These experiments are very straight, straightforward,

Time: 2104.18

excuse me, and they're very objective, which is really nice,

Time: 2107.02

which is you can give people or an animal a drug

Time: 2110.58

that increases the amount of dopamine

Time: 2112.82

and then ask them to measure off

Time: 2115.27

without any measurement device like a watch or a clock

Time: 2118.46

when one minute has passed.

Time: 2120.9

As dopamine levels rise in the brain,

Time: 2123.56

people tend to think that the minute is up before a minute.

Time: 2128.79

So they, at the 38-second mark, they'll say,

Time: 2131.23

okay, I think a minute is up.

Time: 2132.19

So they've overestimated how much time has passed, okay?

Time: 2136

The higher the level of dopamine,

Time: 2137.6

the more people tend to overestimate.

Time: 2139.72

Now, it's also true that norepinephrine,

Time: 2143.18

also called noradrenaline, plays a role

Time: 2145.44

and its role is very similar to that of dopamine

Time: 2148.01

and that's because norepinephrine

Time: 2149.35

and dopamine are close cousins,

Time: 2151.07

as some of you may recall

Time: 2152.87

that they are actually manufactured from one another, okay?

Time: 2156.45

So dopamine can actually make epinephrin

Time: 2159.58

and norepinephrine biochemically.

Time: 2162.15

There's a cascade in which dopamine can be made

Time: 2164.77

into norepinephrine and epinephrin, which is remarkable.

Time: 2168

How does having elevated levels of dopamine

Time: 2170.04

and norepinephrine cause one to overestimate

Time: 2172.74

how much time has passed?

Time: 2174.58

Well, it does it because of the way

Time: 2176.85

that it causes fine-slicing of your time bins.

Time: 2181.4

So fine-slicing of time bins

Time: 2183.44

is like increasing the frame rate on your camera, right?

Time: 2187.25

Slow motion is achieved in movies

Time: 2191.3

and elsewhere by increasing the frame rate.

Time: 2194.44

So if you take a movie at 30 frames per second and watch it,

Time: 2199.23

it will appear to have a certain speed, right?

Time: 2201.19

'Cause those are just snapshots, 30 frames per second.

Time: 2204.74

In contrast, if you took that same movie

Time: 2207.25

at 4,000 frames per second, you are fine-slicing

Time: 2210.55

and you're going to see every little detail

Time: 2211.84

and as you play each one of those frames,

Time: 2213.81

it's going to look like it moved slower, okay?

Time: 2217.01

Whatever, so the kind of jump shot in basketball

Time: 2219.59

that's done slowly, any slow motion

Time: 2222.1

is the consequence of higher frame rate.

Time: 2224.84

So dopamine and norepinephrine increase frame rate,

Time: 2229.38

and as a consequence, they tend to lead us

Time: 2232.47

to overestimate the amount of time that's passed.

Time: 2236.64

Conversely, the neuromodulator serotonin causes people

Time: 2241.14

to underestimate the amount of time that's passed.

Time: 2244.6

So they've done these experiments.

Time: 2246.27

They actually have done these experiments using,

Time: 2248.65

in humans, with drugs that increase serotonin.

Time: 2250.87

They've also done them with cannabis

Time: 2252.66

which increases serotonin among other things

Time: 2254.84

including the cannabinoid receptor activation.

Time: 2259

And when people have elevated levels of 5-HT

Time: 2263.26

or whether or not they've ingested cannabis,

Time: 2265.01

they tend to underestimate how much time has passed.

Time: 2267.57

You do the equivalent experiment.

Time: 2268.7

You tell people that they have to guess

Time: 2271.1

or tell you when five minutes, for instance, has passed.

Time: 2274.41

Just use five minutes as an example this time.

Time: 2276.54

And generally they will miss the five-minute mark.

Time: 2279.51

They will think, they'll let six minutes pass

Time: 2281.93

and they'll think it was five minutes

Time: 2283.42

when they've underestimated how much time has passed.

Time: 2286.19

And that's because serotonin

Time: 2288.86

and some of the related molecules in the brain

Time: 2291.25

tend to lead to slower frame rates, right?

Time: 2295.36

They take the frame rate in the example I used before

Time: 2297.92

from 4,000 frames per second down

Time: 2299.62

to, say, 20 frames per second.

Time: 2302.34

So this is very interesting.

Time: 2305.31

It's interesting in terms of how pharmacology can be used

Time: 2308.43

to adjust time perception but it's also interesting

Time: 2311.21

in the context of that circadian rhythm.

Time: 2314.29

There's some emerging evidence

Time: 2315.91

that throughout the 24-hour cycle,

Time: 2318.41

there are robust changes in the amount of dopamine,

Time: 2321.03

norepinephrine, and serotonin that are present

Time: 2323.72

in the brain and bloodstream and body

Time: 2325.72

depending on time of day within the circadian cycle.

Time: 2330.72

Now, I'm not talking about during sleep.

Time: 2333.74

During sleep, there are definitely variations

Time: 2335.67

in things like dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin.

Time: 2337.75

I talked about that in the episodes on sleep.

Time: 2340.09

Here I'm just talking about the role of these molecules

Time: 2343.41

in time perception during wakefulness.

Time: 2345.95

So much of the evidence points to the fact

Time: 2349.14

that in the first half of the day,

Time: 2351.06

approximate first half of the day,

Time: 2352.98

dopamine and norepinephrine are elevated in the brain,

Time: 2355.63

body, and bloodstream much more than is serotonin

Time: 2359.94

and that in the second half of the day

Time: 2361.47

and in particular towards evening and nighttime,

Time: 2363.13

serotonin levels are going up.

Time: 2365.54

I think that's fairly well-established now.

Time: 2367.84

What that means based on what we just discussed

Time: 2370.47

about the role of dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin

Time: 2372.81

in setting the frame rate of time perception

Time: 2376.3

is that our perception of the passage of time

Time: 2378.98

will be very different in the early part of the day

Time: 2381.39

and in the latter half of the day.

Time: 2383.4

And there's starting to be some evidence

Time: 2385.01

to support this, that early in the day,

Time: 2388.02

people tend to overestimate how much time has passed

Time: 2391.11

and later in the day,

Time: 2391.943

they tend to underestimate how much time has passed.

Time: 2394.04

And this is independent of taking any kind of substance

Time: 2396.24

that would increase or decrease dopamine or serotonin.

Time: 2399.25

Now, this is important in terms

Time: 2401.32

of how one thinks about structuring their day,

Time: 2403.19

because I know many people are thinking

Time: 2404.52

about the various tasks that they need to do

Time: 2407.05

throughout their day.

Time: 2408.8

Many, or I should say all of the literature,

Time: 2412.01

at least that I can find on productivity

Time: 2414.67

and things of that sort point to the idea

Time: 2416.87

that we should be doing the hardest task,

Time: 2418.9

the thing that we want to do the least

Time: 2420.5

or the most important task early in the day

Time: 2423.49

as a kind of a psychological tool for getting it done

Time: 2426.83

and feeling as if we accomplished something

Time: 2428.72

and I think that's an excellent protocol, frankly,

Time: 2431.8

but I'm not sure it's an excellent protocol

Time: 2433.57

because of the way that we sense accomplishment

Time: 2437.38

or at least it's not only an excellent protocol

Time: 2439.81

because of the way that we sense accomplishment.

Time: 2442.07

Another reason to move something that's very hard

Time: 2444.93

into the early part of the day

Time: 2446.93

is that if indeed the dopaminergic

Time: 2450.247

and noradrenergic circuits are more active at that time,

Time: 2454.62

we are actually in a better position cognitively

Time: 2457.93

to parse that hard problem because of the way

Time: 2460.67

that we are able to fine-slice our perception of time

Time: 2464.76

and fine-slice all the perceptual events outside us.

Time: 2467.89

So what I'm really saying is that early in the day,

Time: 2470.01

you are a much more high resolution camera,

Time: 2473.1

so to speak, than you are later in the day.

Time: 2475.7

Now, different types of tasks and different types of things

Time: 2478.88

require different frame rates

Time: 2481.04

or different ways of perceiving time,

Time: 2483.15

and indeed, this also lends itself to a tool

Time: 2486.01

whereby for activities that involve

Time: 2489.51

more kind of creative thinking that aren't as constrained

Time: 2492.96

by particular answers or outcomes

Time: 2495.5

and in which we need to kind of blend different aspects

Time: 2499.16

of our memory, different aspects of task utilization,

Time: 2503.75

in other words, for creative works, for brainstorming,

Time: 2507.04

for things that are a bit more fluid, so to speak,

Time: 2509.94

the more serotonergic second half of the day,

Time: 2513.45

and because of the way the serotonergic second half

Time: 2515.78

of the day lends itself to our time perception,

Time: 2518.47

may actually be more beneficial for those sorts of tasks.

Time: 2521.55

And I'll put a reference to a couple of the studies

Time: 2523.75

that point to this idea

Time: 2525.29

that in these higher dopaminergic states,

Time: 2528.38

we are better at doing certain sorts of tasks

Time: 2531.63

and in these more serotonergic states,

Time: 2533.57

we're better at doing other sorts of tasks

Time: 2535.84

and how the dopamine tends to be earlier in the day

Time: 2538.96

and the serotonin later in the day, so to speak.

Time: 2541.24

These are broad, I'm painting with broad strokes here,

Time: 2543.84

but I think these lend themselves

Time: 2545.29

to some really excellent tools

Time: 2546.77

because I think we all understand the value

Time: 2549.07

of doing something that's hard

Time: 2550.31

or challenging early in the day,

Time: 2551.79

but we should ask ourselves hard or challenging how?

Time: 2554.62

What does that task actually really require

Time: 2557

in terms of time perception?

Time: 2558.65

Some people might appreciate some examples

Time: 2560.27

of how this might work.

Time: 2561.4

Basically what I'm saying is if you are doing work

Time: 2563.81

that involves adhering to some rigid rules,

Time: 2567.04

so math or a recipe or execution of musical scales

Time: 2571.82

or physical skills or accounting

Time: 2575.45

or something that requires a lot of precision

Time: 2577.6

where there's a right and wrong answer and it's hard,

Time: 2581

I would suggest that you do that

Time: 2582.14

in the early part of the day because of the way

Time: 2584.17

that dopamine and norepinephrine impact time perception.

Time: 2588.43

You are literally better at slicing up time,

Time: 2591.71

you are a higher resolution brain during those times,

Time: 2594.827

and so that's going to lend itself better to events

Time: 2598.84

and demands that require high resolution,

Time: 2600.79

whereas in the afternoon

Time: 2602.65

in this more what I'm calling serotonergic state,

Time: 2605.22

that's when you're going to be better at brainstorming

Time: 2607.4

and creative works where there's some flexibility

Time: 2610.17

in terms of how you're batching time and perceiving time

Time: 2613.26

and there isn't so much rigid oversight

Time: 2616.25

of a right or wrong answer.

Time: 2617.94

And as an aside to support what I said

Time: 2620.31

but also to take us back

Time: 2622.12

to this critical role of the circadian rhythm,

Time: 2625.1

there is a lot of evidence

Time: 2626.89

that when one's sleep is disrupted,

Time: 2629.28

when sleep is either too short or is fragmented

Time: 2632.05

or is not of high enough quality for enough days,

Time: 2635.33

one of the first things to happen

Time: 2637.41

is that there is a dysregulation

Time: 2639.72

of these dopaminergic, noradrenergic,

Time: 2642.28

and serotonergic states throughout the day.

Time: 2644.05

They get kind of mish-mashed up.

Time: 2647.79

It's not that they're a total mess

Time: 2649.5

but they aren't as cleanly defined.

Time: 2652.33

And I think this is one of the reasons why

Time: 2654.02

when we haven't slept well or we haven't slept enough,

Time: 2657.12

we tend to feel a little off, like we can't concentrate.

Time: 2659.73

Part of that lack of concentration is due to other things

Time: 2662.37

but part of that concentration could be due to the fact

Time: 2664.29

that our sense of the passage of time is disrupted.

Time: 2667.23

So there seems to be some value in keeping

Time: 2669.47

the dopaminergic noradrenergic state kind of limited

Time: 2673.01

to the early part of the day

Time: 2674.26

and this serotonergic state, as we're calling it,

Time: 2676.143

kind of pushed towards the second half of the day.

Time: 2678.88

Now, there is a version of how dopamine and norepinephrine

Time: 2681.86

can impact our perception of the passage of time

Time: 2684.49

in ways that can be very disruptive or even maladaptive

Time: 2689.02

and the best example that I'm aware of is trauma.

Time: 2693.01

Many people who have been in car accidents

Time: 2695.68

or who have experienced some other form of major trauma

Time: 2700.11

do what's called overclocking.

Time: 2702.692

Overclocking is when levels of dopamine and norepinephrine

Time: 2707.5

increase so much during a particular event,

Time: 2711.5

our level of alertness is increased so much

Time: 2713.93

during a given event that we fine-slice.

Time: 2718.12

In other words, the frame rate is increased so much so

Time: 2721.89

that we perceive things as happening in ultra slow motion.

Time: 2726.01

Now, that might not seem like a bad thing overall

Time: 2730.15

but the problem with overclocking is the way

Time: 2733.83

in which that information gets stamped down

Time: 2736.01

into the memory system.

Time: 2737.7

So the memory system, which involves areas of the brain

Time: 2740.79

like the hippocampus but also the neocortex,

Time: 2743.58

is basically a space-time recorder.

Time: 2746.46

What do I mean by space-time recorder?

Time: 2748.16

Well, your nervous system, of course,

Time: 2750.45

is housed in the darkness of your skull.

Time: 2753.01

It doesn't have a whole lot of information

Time: 2754.49

about the outside world except light coming in

Time: 2756.35

through the eyes and whatever happens to hit our ears

Time: 2758.608

in terms of sound waves and skin and so forth

Time: 2762.32

so it has to take all those neural signals

Time: 2764.26

and it has to create a record of what happened.

Time: 2767.44

Now, it doesn't create a record of everything that happened

Time: 2769.67

but car accidents and trauma

Time: 2771.99

and things of that sort oftentimes are stamped down

Time: 2774.51

into our record of what happened.

Time: 2777.33

And what gets stamped down,

Time: 2779.75

what we actually mean by the phrase stamped down

Time: 2782.28

is that the precise firing of the sequence of neurons

Time: 2785.8

that reflected some events,

Time: 2788.06

so let's say I'm in a car accident,

Time: 2789.94

certain neurons are firing because of the flipping

Time: 2792.29

of the car or their screams or there's blood,

Time: 2795.36

or, you know, things of that sort,

Time: 2797.42

all of that neural activity gets repeated

Time: 2802.27

in the hippocampus and then the sequence

Time: 2805.58

of the firing of those neurons is also remembered.

Time: 2807.91

So it's not just that neuron one, two, three, four

Time: 2810.81

fired in that sequence.

Time: 2811.93

It's also that neuron one, two, three, four

Time: 2813.97

fired at a particular rate.

Time: 2815.46

So it would be one, two, three, four

Time: 2816.75

during the actual event and then the memory is stored

Time: 2819.55

as firing of those neurons as one, two, three, four, right?

Time: 2822.41

If during the event, it was one, two, three, four

Time: 2825.07

at that rate, the storage of the memory

Time: 2827.86

is not going to be one,

Time: 2830.76

two, three, four, okay?

Time: 2832.53

In other words, there's both a space code, as we say,

Time: 2835.7

meaning the particular neurons that fire is important,

Time: 2838.67

and there's a rate code, how quickly those neurons fire

Time: 2842.24

or the relative firing, the timing of the firing

Time: 2844.92

of those neurons is also part of the memory.

Time: 2846.91

This affords our memory system tremendous flexibility.

Time: 2849.33

What it means is that you can take the same set of neurons

Time: 2851.8

in the hippocampus and stamp down many, many more memories

Time: 2855.61

because all you have to do is use a match

Time: 2859.276

of the different rates of the different neurons

Time: 2861.85

that were firing in order to set that code, right?

Time: 2864.593

Otherwise, if you needed a different set of neurons

Time: 2866.9

for every memory, you'd need an enormous hippocampus.

Time: 2869.04

You'd need an enormous head.

Time: 2870.21

So I think you get the basic idea.

Time: 2872.511

Overclocking is a case in which the frame rate is so high

Time: 2876.98

that a memory gets stamped down

Time: 2879.18

and people have a very hard time shaking that memory

Time: 2881.367

and the emotions associated with that memory.

Time: 2883.91

And it's not the topic of today's conversation

Time: 2886.09

but we will cover trauma in a future episode in detail,

Time: 2889.12

but many of the treatments for trauma,

Time: 2892.31

EMDR, nowadays there's a lot of excitement also

Time: 2894.87

about ketamine therapies, exposure therapies,

Time: 2898.67

you know, like cognitive behavioral therapies

Time: 2900.79

involve not just trying to reduce the amount of emotion

Time: 2903.93

associated with a memory, but also a deliberate speeding up

Time: 2908.36

or slowing down of that memory.

Time: 2910.4

In other words, trying to allow the person

Time: 2912.91

who experienced the trauma to take control

Time: 2914.81

of the rate of the experience in their memory,

Time: 2918.24

not just whether or not the memory happened at all.

Time: 2921.09

In fact, you know, one of the first things

Time: 2923.41

that trauma victims learn

Time: 2924.69

is that they aren't going to forget what happened.

Time: 2927.37

What's eventually going to happen ideally

Time: 2929.5

with good treatment is that the emotional weight

Time: 2932.67

of the experience will eventually be divorced

Time: 2936.08

from the memory of the experience.

Time: 2938.02

And that's done again by trying to reduce

Time: 2940.4

the amount of emotional activation

Time: 2942.27

during the recall of that experience

Time: 2943.93

and one of the best ways to do that

Time: 2945.64

is to alter the rate of the memory playback.

Time: 2950.06

In other words, taking that firing of neurons

Time: 2952.06

that might've been one, two, three, four,

Time: 2954.29

again, it would be much more complicated,

Time: 2955.92

but one, two, three, four for the car crash

Time: 2957.75

and getting the memory to play back at a rate

Time: 2960.4

of one, two, three, four,

Time: 2963.17

or even one, two, three, four,

Time: 2965.83

one, two, three, four.

Time: 2967.4

In other words, allowing the person

Time: 2969.95

or instructing the person to take control

Time: 2972.68

of the rate of the playback, and in that way,

Time: 2974.89

there seems to be still yet unknown mechanism

Time: 2977.61

by which people can uncouple some of the emotional weight

Time: 2981.73

that's associated with that memory.

Time: 2983.52

So overclocking is a kind of extreme example

Time: 2988.44

of where the dopaminergic and the neurogenetic system

Time: 2991.15

is ramped up so high that people have this,

Time: 2994.32

unfortunately, what seems like indelible mark

Time: 2996.7

in their brain of a particular event.

Time: 2998.43

But again, trauma treatment is designed

Time: 3000.03

to uncouple the emotional load of that event.

Time: 3002.59

Some of you are probably saying why dopamine during trauma?

Time: 3005.93

I thought dopamine was the feel-good molecule.

Time: 3008.34

Well, in reality,

Time: 3010.5

dopamine is not necessarily a molecule of reward.

Time: 3013.17

It's a molecule of motivation, pursuit, and drive.

Time: 3016.11

And because of the close relationship

Time: 3017.83

between dopamine and norepinephrine,

Time: 3019.58

oftentimes they are co-released.

Time: 3021.59

So whether or not dopamine is released during car crashes

Time: 3025.05

or other forms of trauma, we don't know

Time: 3027.18

but what we do know is that both the dopamine system

Time: 3029.447

and noradrenergic system, when we say noradrenergic,

Time: 3032.25

we mean norepinephrine, those systems are greatly increased

Time: 3035.24

anytime there's a heightened state of arousal.

Time: 3037.64

And arousal can have negative valence,

Time: 3039.93

like meaning associated with an event that we really hate,

Time: 3043.2

that we would prefer not to be involved in,

Time: 3045.11

or can positive valence, but dopamine and norepinephrine

Time: 3048.69

are kind of the common hallmark

Time: 3050.73

of all things of elevated arousal.

Time: 3053.46

And so that's why we see evidence for dopamine

Time: 3056.44

being associated with these changes in time perception

Time: 3059.24

both for positive events and for negative events.

Time: 3063.06

There's a very interesting relationship between arousal,

Time: 3066.65

dopamine, time perception, and blinking,

Time: 3069.98

and this is all supported by a really interesting paper,

Time: 3073.41

first author, Terhune is the last name, T-E-R-H-U-N-E.

Time: 3076.78

It's published in "Current Biology."

Time: 3078.41

Cell Press journal. Excellent journal.

Time: 3080.29

The title of the paper is

Time: 3081.123

"Time Dilates After Spontaneous Blinking."

Time: 3084.13

So heightened states of arousal are associated

Time: 3085.94

with heightened levels of dopamine.

Time: 3087.25

You now know that dopamine leads

Time: 3089.22

to a kind of fine-slicing of time

Time: 3092.46

and one of the ways that we fine-slice time is by blinking.

Time: 3096.28

You know, we think of blinking as just a thing

Time: 3097.81

to lubricate our eyes or to limit

Time: 3099.76

the amount of light coming into our eyes,

Time: 3101.49

but it's a shutter on our experience.

Time: 3104.07

So much of the information coming into the brain

Time: 3106.47

through our eyes impacts our attention.

Time: 3108.02

I've said it before on this podcast,

Time: 3109.54

that cognitive attention follows visual attention,

Time: 3111.8

at least for sighted individuals.

Time: 3113.73

Well, it turns out that dopamine

Time: 3116.61

and increases in dopamine are associated

Time: 3118.71

with increases in spontaneous blink rate.

Time: 3121.35

So the more aroused we are, the more awake we are,

Time: 3125.05

there are a number of effects.

Time: 3125.883

Pupils dilate, heart rate increases, et cetera,

Time: 3128.1

but also blink rate increases.

Time: 3130.02

And every time we blink, this study cleanly shows,

Time: 3134.37

we shift our perception of time, leading to,

Time: 3137.03

as I mentioned before, overestimations of time.

Time: 3140.32

So it seems as though in some way,

Time: 3142.55

blink rate is actually related to frame rate.

Time: 3145.44

So this is very, very interesting

Time: 3147.7

and the way that you could think about leveraging this

Time: 3150.19

would be if you wanted to actually slow down

Time: 3154

your perception of time, you would blink less.

Time: 3156.91

And if you want to speed up your perception of time,

Time: 3159.5

you would blink more.

Time: 3160.45

Now, you'd have to think of a scenario

Time: 3162.22

in which that would be useful to you.

Time: 3163.9

Obviously if you're going to blink,

Time: 3165.13

you're going to miss things as well.

Time: 3166.95

But I think it's a very interesting parameter

Time: 3169.8

of our visual attention as it relates to time perception,

Time: 3173.18

because what it really speaks to

Time: 3174.75

is that these neuromodulators like dopamine

Time: 3176.72

or serotonin that adjust frame rate,

Time: 3178.83

they're not doing it through some magical mechanism.

Time: 3181.34

In fact, there's no single brain area

Time: 3183.53

that we can say controls time perception.

Time: 3186.18

I haven't said today, oh, you know, it's the striatum.

Time: 3188.92

Well, it involves the striatum but I'm not going to say,

Time: 3191.72

for instance, oh, it's the cerebellum.

Time: 3193.37

The cerebellum is definitely involved

Time: 3194.99

in timing of movement, something for a future podcast,

Time: 3198.65

but time perception is what we call

Time: 3200.9

a distributed phenomenon.

Time: 3202.4

It's a network of areas in the brain working together.

Time: 3205.45

But dopamine and the way that it relates

Time: 3208.12

to the shuttering of your eyes

Time: 3210.98

seems to be controlling the frame rate on your experience.

Time: 3214.41

Numerous times on this podcast,

Time: 3215.84

I've talked about cold exposure

Time: 3217.52

and nowadays there's a lot of interest

Time: 3219.27

in things like cold showers, ice baths,

Time: 3222.43

immersion in cold water tanks

Time: 3225.18

and lakes and oceans and things of that sort.

Time: 3227.92

There are a lot of different positive effects

Time: 3229.81

of cold exposure provided it's done properly.

Time: 3232.55

It can lead to increases in metabolism, brown fat stores,

Time: 3235.19

which are the good fat stores that you want.

Time: 3236.84

They're sort of like a furnace

Time: 3238.53

that allow you to heat yourself up,

Time: 3239.74

stay warm in cold environments, to reduce inflammation,

Time: 3243.78

to increase resilience and so forth.

Time: 3246.97

There's a study published

Time: 3247.88

in the "European Journal of Physiology"

Time: 3249.34

showing that cold exposure can increase

Time: 3252.43

our baseline levels of dopamine robustly, 2.5x,

Time: 3255.837

and it's a long-lasting increase in dopamine

Time: 3258.1

and appears to be a healthy one,

Time: 3259.34

meaning it doesn't seem to be addictive.

Time: 3260.98

I'm sure there are some people out there addicted

Time: 3262.57

to ice baths, but, you know, when you think about the range

Time: 3264.97

of dopamine-inducing behaviors that are addictive,

Time: 3267.45

it seems to be more on the health-promoting side.

Time: 3270.78

What's interesting is that because cold water exposure

Time: 3273.67

increases dopamine, it will also change

Time: 3275.94

your perception of time and if you've ever done

Time: 3277.62

one of these cold water exposures, you've experienced this.

Time: 3279.997

You've experienced getting in and feeling like, wow,

Time: 3283.4

making it three minutes is a really, really long time,

Time: 3285.93

and you are fine-slicing time.

Time: 3287.62

Your frame rate is going up.

Time: 3289.05

Part of that, just at a kind of a coarse level

Time: 3292.44

is you're thinking, this is painful.

Time: 3293.75

I don't like this. I want to get out, right?

Time: 3296.45

But part of it is also that your dopamine levels

Time: 3298.93

are going up very quickly and therefore your perception

Time: 3302.32

of that discomfort is also being fine-sliced.

Time: 3305.21

And so you could leverage a tool, for instance,

Time: 3307.85

where you try and entrain your thinking

Time: 3310.08

to something other than your immediate experience, right?

Time: 3313.02

This is a kind of a controversy, if you will,

Time: 3315.8

in the cold exposure world.

Time: 3318.45

The question is do you try and lean into the experience

Time: 3321.39

and really feel it, or do you try and distract yourself,

Time: 3324.64

you know, sing a song or count off,

Time: 3326.55

you know, from one to a hundred.

Time: 3330.56

Just know that whatever tactic you use

Time: 3332.69

to get through the cold exposure

Time: 3334.97

that the dopamine level that's now increased

Time: 3337.77

in your system is going to cause you to fine-slice

Time: 3341.32

or experience that at slow motion.

Time: 3344.1

So a minute is going to seem like a lot longer

Time: 3346.36

than a minute in reality.

Time: 3349.01

So you could, for instance,

Time: 3351.42

decide to pay attention to some external cue.

Time: 3355.1

Maybe it's a metronome that ticks once every 10 seconds.

Time: 3359.56

You could decide to think about something else.

Time: 3362.36

You could decide to sing a song in your head

Time: 3364.36

or sing a song out loud.

Time: 3365.81

All of that will divorce you from the sensation

Time: 3368.38

that you experience somewhat,

Time: 3369.56

but more so it will divorce you

Time: 3371.88

from the perception of your experience as governed

Time: 3375.45

by that dopamine increase in frame rate.

Time: 3377.69

If that isn't clear, just know this.

Time: 3379.84

When you're in the ice bath, your dopamine levels are high.

Time: 3381.98

When your dopamine levels are high,

Time: 3383.72

your experience of the discomfort

Time: 3385.09

of that ice bath is at higher resolution.

Time: 3388.26

Now, up until now, I've been talking about how dopamine,

Time: 3391.22

and to some extent, serotonin

Time: 3393.78

can differentially impact your perception of how fast

Time: 3397.34

or how slowly things are happening in the moment.

Time: 3401.33

But remember, we have prospective time,

Time: 3403.86

we have our experience of time in the moment,

Time: 3406.15

and we have retrospective time

Time: 3408.41

and there are beautiful studies that have showed

Time: 3411.21

that the dopaminergic state changes the way

Time: 3416.09

not just that we experience things now,

Time: 3418.42

but that it changes the way in which we remember things

Time: 3422.39

in the past and the rate at which those things occurred.

Time: 3425.6

And those are in opposite direction.

Time: 3428.13

So to make this very simple,

Time: 3430.74

if something that you experience is fun or varied,

Time: 3435.56

meaning it has a lot of different components in it

Time: 3438.64

and is, in other words, is associated

Time: 3440.7

with an increase in dopamine in your brain,

Time: 3444.24

you will experience that as going by very fast.

Time: 3449.36

Now, this is different than the ice bath

Time: 3450.81

which I just said you experienced as going by very slowly,

Time: 3453.03

but here I'm talking about something that's fun

Time: 3454.97

and varied that you really like

Time: 3456.7

and you feel like it goes by very, very fast.

Time: 3458.83

Imagine an amazing day for a kid at an amusement park.

Time: 3462.33

They can do a ton of things. It's all new.

Time: 3464.32

They're very excited and they'll feel

Time: 3466.21

like it goes by very fast,

Time: 3468.41

but later they will remember that experience

Time: 3472.28

as being very long,

Time: 3474.61

that it was a long day full of many, many events.

Time: 3477.59

And so there's this paradoxical relationship

Time: 3480.38

between how we perceive fun, exciting, varied events

Time: 3483.47

in the present and how we remember them in the past.

Time: 3486.25

For those of you who've gone on vacation,

Time: 3488.37

if you've had an amazing day on vacation, it'll seem like,

Time: 3490.87

or an amazing vacation overall,

Time: 3492.31

it will seem like it goes by very fast.

Time: 3494.23

The last day of vacation, you sort of go, whoa,

Time: 3495.85

it went by so fast 'cause there's so much happening.

Time: 3498.72

But in memory six to eight months later, you remember, wow,

Time: 3502.84

that just went, you know, that was a long, long thing.

Time: 3506.98

We had this. Then we had that.

Time: 3508.35

Then we did this. Then we had that.

Time: 3509.61

It tends to spool out in a longer memory

Time: 3512.74

than the actual experience.

Time: 3514.77

Conversely, if you are bored with something

Time: 3519.03

or it's something you really don't like,

Time: 3520.32

it's going to seem like it takes a long time

Time: 3523.72

to go through that experience in the moment,

Time: 3526.21

but retroactively, looking back,

Time: 3528.09

it will seem like that moment was very short.

Time: 3530.3

So the other day I was waiting in the waiting room

Time: 3532.98

for the dentist, it was pretty boring.

Time: 3534.83

I was just kind of sitting there.

Time: 3535.98

There wasn't much going on.

Time: 3537.78

And it did seem like it was going on an awfully long time,

Time: 3540.09

but indeed, looking back, it just seems like, okay,

Time: 3542.05

I sat in that room, not much happened.

Time: 3544.03

And so it seems like a very short time bin.

Time: 3545.67

This seems to be an efficiency

Time: 3547.42

of how the brain stores information,

Time: 3550.62

dopamine being associated, of course,

Time: 3552.36

with fun and varied experiences

Time: 3554.41

and low dopamine being associated

Time: 3556.23

with kind of empty, boring,

Time: 3558.51

or what at the time seem like long experiences.

Time: 3562.06

And this whole thing has been stamped down

Time: 3564.47

into the scientific literature by those earlier experiments

Time: 3567.65

where they take human beings and isolate them

Time: 3569.51

in certain environments.

Time: 3570.67

You know, take away all their clocks and watches and cues

Time: 3573.41

and about what time of day it is

Time: 3575.46

and what time of night it is and allow people to have a life

Time: 3579.01

where they can either read and work and do things

Time: 3582.22

or where they have very little to do.

Time: 3584.16

When people are isolated in very boring environments

Time: 3587.57

and they don't have access to time cues, time dilates.

Time: 3591.45

They tend to assume that time has gone on very, very long.

Time: 3595.91

And so the reason I bring this up is we aren't just driven

Time: 3599.24

by these circadian clocks and these circannual clocks

Time: 3601.72

and these ultradian clocks.

Time: 3603.74

We are driven by these timers that vary

Time: 3607.7

depending on our level of excitement

Time: 3610.69

and they vary depending on our level of excitement

Time: 3613.52

because of these neuromodulators, dopamine and serotonin.

Time: 3616.13

So the way I like to think about it

Time: 3617.66

is that you have two clocks, two stopwatches.

Time: 3620.78

One is a dopaminergic stopwatch

Time: 3623.24

that fine-slices really closely.

Time: 3625.24

It's like, counts off milliseconds and it's grabbing a movie

Time: 3628.03

of your experience at very high resolution.

Time: 3630.27

And in the other hand, you have a stopwatch

Time: 3633.28

that's gathering big time bins,

Time: 3634.9

big ticks along the, you know,

Time: 3637.47

the hand is moving at bigger intervals,

Time: 3639.86

you know, marking off time.

Time: 3641.48

And depending on whether or not you're excited

Time: 3644.58

or whether or not you're bored,

Time: 3645.73

you're using different stopwatches on time

Time: 3648.79

and therefore you're perceiving your experience differently.

Time: 3652.03

One very interesting aspect to the way

Time: 3654.04

that neuromodulators like dopamine and novelty interact

Time: 3658.75

with time perception and memory

Time: 3660.78

is how we perceive our relationship to places and people.

Time: 3666.77

So really interesting literature showing

Time: 3668.76

that the more novel experiences we have in a place,

Time: 3672.88

the more we feel we know that place, obviously,

Time: 3676.59

but the longer we feel we've been there.

Time: 3679.46

So here's the kind of gedanken or thought experiment

Time: 3681.88

that illustrates what's in the literature.

Time: 3684.32

Let's say I were to move to New York City.

Time: 3685.94

I happen to really like New York City.

Time: 3687.23

I've never lived there, but let's say I lived there.

Time: 3689.59

I lived in a given apartment for a year

Time: 3692.36

and I would have a number of different experiences

Time: 3694.42

in this mental experiment.

Time: 3695.71

Let's say I had 100 different exciting and new experiences.

Time: 3702.01

I would at the end of that year feel as if I lived there

Time: 3705.49

a certain period of time, one year.

Time: 3706.94

I would actually know I lived there one year.

Time: 3708.59

If however I lived in three different places

Time: 3710.58

in New York City and I met three times as many people

Time: 3713.3

and I had three times as many novel experiences,

Time: 3716.29

I would actually feel as if I had been there much longer

Time: 3719.84

than had I only lived in one location.

Time: 3722.01

This is also true for social interactions.

Time: 3724.62

When we move to multiple

Time: 3727.49

or several novel environments with somebody else,

Time: 3731.2

we tend to feel as if we know that person much better

Time: 3733.52

and that they know us much better.

Time: 3734.91

Now, of course, we get the opportunity to interact

Time: 3736.79

with those people in different contexts

Time: 3738.23

and so indeed we do get the opportunity to see them,

Time: 3741.19

for instance, at the coffee shop, how they order coffee.

Time: 3744.14

You maybe go to a sports event, how they act there.

Time: 3747.36

Maybe how they interact with your family.

Time: 3749.15

You're getting a sense of them in different contexts.

Time: 3752.07

That's certainly playing a role,

Time: 3754.72

but it seems as if the more novelty you experience

Time: 3757.8

with somebody, not only the more familiar they are to you,

Time: 3761.08

but the more time you feel you've spent with them,

Time: 3763.45

even though the total amount of time

Time: 3765.05

can be exactly the same.

Time: 3766.56

And so that's a very interesting aspect

Time: 3768.65

of how our perception of time and these neuromodulators

Time: 3771.21

and novelty can shape the way not just that we perceive

Time: 3775.08

a given event in our world, but how we relate

Time: 3777.93

to a place or relate to a person.

Time: 3779.97

So we've talked a lot about the different neurochemicals

Time: 3782.89

and how those neurochemicals can influence

Time: 3784.77

our perception of time.

Time: 3786.06

We haven't talked a lot about the neural circuits

Time: 3788.15

and the various areas of the brain that underlie this.

Time: 3791.26

I do want to touch on that

Time: 3792.36

by highlighting a really wonderful study.

Time: 3795.35

This was a study published in "Neuron,"

Time: 3796.99

also a Cell Press journal, excellent journal.

Time: 3799.47

The title of the paper is "Behavioral, Physiological,

Time: 3802.43

and Neural Signatures of Surprise

Time: 3804.8

During Naturalistic Sports Viewing."

Time: 3808.06

This experiment is really cool.

Time: 3809.12

They did brain imaging on individuals

Time: 3811.32

who are watching basketball games.

Time: 3813.84

These were basketball games

Time: 3815.2

that actually took place that were recorded

Time: 3817.83

and the subjects watching these basketball games

Time: 3821.47

in some cases, not all, had some interest

Time: 3823.87

in who would win or lose, and in some cases, not all,

Time: 3826.81

the subjects in these studies had some prior knowledge

Time: 3829.78

of which team they thought was better,

Time: 3831.81

which team was likely to win or not likely to win.

Time: 3835.71

The basic findings of the study

Time: 3837.03

were that they could measure surprise

Time: 3841.53

by the release of dopamine in two areas of the brain,

Time: 3845.43

part of what are called, is called, excuse me,

Time: 3847.47

the mesolimbic reward pathway.

Time: 3849.33

So the two areas of the brain that are important here

Time: 3850.9

are the nucleus accumbens and the VTA,

Time: 3853.38

the ventral tegmental area.

Time: 3854.62

These are areas that release dopamine

Time: 3857.52

as kind of a token of reward

Time: 3859.36

any time something is surprising

Time: 3861.66

or a positive expectation is met, okay?

Time: 3865.27

So if I predict that my team dribbling down court

Time: 3868.65

is going to score on this drive

Time: 3870.45

and they get the ball in the basket,

Time: 3871.98

a little bit of dopamine is released.

Time: 3873.3

These two brain areas light up

Time: 3875.24

in the functional imaging so-called FSRI,

Time: 3878.382

functional magnetic resonance imaging

Time: 3880.3

that they used in this study.

Time: 3883.2

What's really interesting about this study

Time: 3884.88

is not just that dopamine was released any time

Time: 3888.64

that something the subject wanted to see happened, right,

Time: 3892.12

any time they wanted to see their team score, they scored,

Time: 3894.8

but also during surprise.

Time: 3896.67

So if they thought for instance,

Time: 3898.79

and they would hit a button to predict

Time: 3901.1

that their team was going to score in this particular drive

Time: 3903.41

and they didn't, well, then dopamine could also be released

Time: 3907.17

in response to that surprise.

Time: 3909.06

So this speaks again to dopamine being something

Time: 3911.33

that's important not just for positive events,

Time: 3914.1

but for unexpected events.

Time: 3916.11

Now, that's all very interesting and speaks to the fact

Time: 3919.12

that dopamine is a kind of flexible currency in the brain.

Time: 3922.46

It's doled out, if you will, or released

Time: 3925.57

when something that one hopes will happen happens

Time: 3929.09

and it's released when there's a surprise,

Time: 3931.9

even if it's a kind of a negative surprise.

Time: 3934.01

It's not something that the subject wanted to happen.

Time: 3937.26

But the more interesting thing

Time: 3939.09

is how that relates to time perception.

Time: 3942.25

What they found was regardless of what caused

Time: 3945.06

the dopamine release, the frequency of dopamine release

Time: 3949.37

predicted how the subjects parsed the time bins

Time: 3953.48

of the game they were watching.

Time: 3955.72

What do I mean by that?

Time: 3956.7

Well, when you watch a basketball game

Time: 3958.37

or you watch anything, children playing or talking

Time: 3961.05

to your spouse or whatever, you're batching time.

Time: 3964.78

How are you batching time?

Time: 3966.55

Well, you could batch a meal by the, I don't know,

Time: 3969.62

the appetizer, the main course, and the dessert,

Time: 3972.24

but it turns out that's not what you're doing.

Time: 3974.68

You're batching time according

Time: 3976.89

to the frequency of dopamine pulses,

Time: 3979.48

the frequency of dopamine release.

Time: 3981.25

And that's what they saw in this study.

Time: 3983.06

If they evaluated people's perceptions

Time: 3985.28

of the passage of time,

Time: 3986.55

what they found is that that matched

Time: 3988.31

not whether or not the, you know,

Time: 3990.29

it was a particular time point in the game,

Time: 3992.59

not whether or not their team was going down court

Time: 3994.94

or running back up court to play defense,

Time: 3997.73

but the dopamine released served as markers

Time: 4002.36

which would predict the frame rate

Time: 4004.27

of their perception of the experience.

Time: 4006

And if that sounds complicated, what I mean is how often

Time: 4009.1

and when you release dopamine is actually setting

Time: 4012.34

the frame rate on the entire perception of everything,

Time: 4015.84

not just for positive events or negative events.

Time: 4018.79

So what this means is as you're going through life,

Time: 4022.83

dopamine and the release of dopamine is saying that's over

Time: 4027.98

and now you're in a new phase of your life

Time: 4030.14

even if it's very short, right?

Time: 4031.62

So if I get up in the morning

Time: 4033.127

and I really need a cup of coffee, as you probably all know,

Time: 4036.03

I wait 90 minutes to 120 minutes before I drink my coffee,

Time: 4040.03

but then I get my coffee

Time: 4040.98

and surely there's a dopamine hit there, I promise you,

Time: 4043.97

I actually am starting to carve up my day

Time: 4047.96

according to dopamine hits.

Time: 4050.83

Consciously or subconsciously,

Time: 4054.5

I'm actually carving up my experience

Time: 4057.04

according to when I'm getting dopamine throughout my day.

Time: 4059.87

This governance over our perception of time

Time: 4063.47

that dopamine has points to a very clear,

Time: 4067.08

very actionable, and very powerful tool.

Time: 4070.55

And that is a tool that many people have talked about before

Time: 4073.35

which are habits.

Time: 4075.97

People have discussed habits in a variety of contexts

Time: 4078.73

but in the context of dopamine reward

Time: 4083.67

and time perception, what this means

Time: 4086.56

is that placing specific habitual routines

Time: 4090.85

at particular intervals throughout your day

Time: 4094.54

is a very, not just convenient,

Time: 4096.94

but a very good way to incorporate the dopamine system

Time: 4101

so that you divide your day into a series

Time: 4103.35

of what I would call functional units.

Time: 4105.15

What would this look like?

Time: 4106.22

It would mean waking up and having one specific habit

Time: 4108.56

that you always engage in that causes a release of dopamine.

Time: 4114.15

You can say, well, great.

Time: 4115.69

That'll make me feel good. And I would agree.

Time: 4118.32

Dopamine released generally makes us feel motivated

Time: 4121.32

But it would have an additional effect of marking

Time: 4123.67

that time of day as the beginning of a particular time bin.

Time: 4127.98

Then inserting another habit, perhaps the beginning of,

Time: 4130.99

I don't know, your breakfast or something,

Time: 4133.06

but recognizing that that's a habit

Time: 4134.81

and being fairly habitual, you don't have to be, you know,

Time: 4137.82

obsessively precise about the timing,

Time: 4140.1

but that regular sequencing of things

Time: 4144.5

is going to lead not just to dopamine release

Time: 4147.09

as it relates to reward and motivation and feeling good,

Time: 4150.01

but it actually becomes the way

Time: 4151.62

in which we carve up our entire experience of our day.

Time: 4155.32

And this is almost a circular argument.

Time: 4157.93

You could say, well, of course, you know,

Time: 4159.12

I do one thing, then I do the next,

Time: 4160.58

then I do the next, and that's how I perceive my day.

Time: 4163.69

That's my day. It's my list.

Time: 4165.33

It's my to-do list, et cetera.

Time: 4167.24

But what I'm saying is that on the basis of this study,

Time: 4170.04

I should mention the first author, his last name is Antony.

Time: 4173.15

It was Antony et al. It was published in 2020.

Time: 4177.11

The study on basketball viewing, what it points to

Time: 4179.95

is that by engaging in specific habits

Time: 4182.83

that we know we can perform well,

Time: 4185.07

we are actually setting the frame rate on our day.

Time: 4188.37

And so I think there will soon come a time

Time: 4190.59

where human beings are not just thinking of, okay,

Time: 4192.94

my morning routine and my afternoon routine,

Time: 4195.65

I think that can be useful, and in fact,

Time: 4197.73

I used or mentioned a structure

Time: 4199.7

of that sort earlier in the episode,

Time: 4201.56

but rather thinking about what's actually going on

Time: 4204.95

at the level of our biology,

Time: 4207.26

which is that dopamine is marking time.

Time: 4209.91

Habits are a very clear way

Time: 4211.78

in which we can invoke dopamine release

Time: 4214.24

and therefore provide time markers, and what this means

Time: 4217.47

is that, for instance, during your morning,

Time: 4219.6

you might insert habit one and habit two

Time: 4222.33

at, say, I don't know, 8 AM and 10 AM,

Time: 4225.2

and in doing that, that marks an epoch,

Time: 4227.83

a little batch of time in your morning routine

Time: 4231.64

that's distinct from the second half of your morning.

Time: 4234.54

In other words, habits serve as flankers

Time: 4237.55

or markers for the passage of your day.

Time: 4240.91

Now, if that seems kind of hyper-neurotic

Time: 4242.77

or why would I want to structure my life like that,

Time: 4245.04

I would say that many people would do well

Time: 4247.59

to structure their life like that and to utilize habits

Time: 4250.44

not just for the sake of what you do during the habit,

Time: 4252.84

but because of the fact that the habits serve as a marker

Time: 4256.14

because of the way they can evoke dopamine release.

Time: 4258.98

And in doing that, you are able to segment your day

Time: 4262.55

into a bunch of smaller, if you want them to be smaller,

Time: 4265.33

or larger functional units.

Time: 4268.81

If anyone wants to experiment with this,

Time: 4270.89

the Huberman Lab Podcast puts out a newsletter.

Time: 4272.9

It's called the Neural Network Newsletter.

Time: 4274.42

You can sign up for it at hubermanlab.com.

Time: 4277.49

We put it out each month.

Time: 4278.49

You can see the previous newsletters.

Time: 4280.06

There's zero cost. We have our privacy statement there.

Time: 4282.4

We don't share your email or anything.

Time: 4284.12

And there you'll find the, you know,

Time: 4287.3

12 steps to improving sleep was the first one.

Time: 4290.05

There's another, the second newsletter

Time: 4291.71

was all about neuroplasticity

Time: 4293.09

and using scientific literature

Time: 4294.88

to improve learning and teaching, and in the next newsletter

Time: 4299.05

I intend to include an example protocol

Time: 4301.78

of how one could use habits and the relationship

Time: 4304.03

between habits and dopamine, dopamine and time perception,

Time: 4307.65

to structure your day according to performance

Time: 4310.23

of particular types of tasks.

Time: 4312.11

Today we covered a lot about time perception.

Time: 4314.24

We certainly didn't cover everything about time perception

Time: 4316.38

but we covered things like entrainment,

Time: 4317.97

the role of dopamine, habits and various routines

Time: 4321.21

that can adjust your sense of time

Time: 4323.44

for sake of particular goals.

Time: 4325.44

If you're interested in learning more about time perception,

Time: 4328.48

I'd like to point you to a really excellent book

Time: 4330.93

called "Your Brain Is a Time Machine:

Time: 4332.807

"The Neuroscience and Physics of Time."

Time: 4335.3

The book was written by Professor Dr. Dean Buonomano

Time: 4338.13

who's a professor at UCLA and a world expert

Time: 4340.88

in the neuroscience and physics of time.

Time: 4343.48

I do hope to get Dean on the podcast

Time: 4345.63

in the not too distant future.

Time: 4347.45

If you're learning from and are enjoying this podcast,

Time: 4349.97

please subscribe to our podcast channel on YouTube.

Time: 4352.59

It's simply Huberman Lab on YouTube.

Time: 4354.75

And there you can also leave us suggestions

Time: 4357.57

for future guests and topics

Time: 4358.99

and questions about the podcast episodes

Time: 4361.69

in the comments section on YouTube.

Time: 4363.82

In addition, please subscribe to our podcast

Time: 4366.07

on Apple and/or Spotify, and on Apple,

Time: 4368.42

you have the opportunity to leave us

Time: 4369.84

up to a five star review.

Time: 4372.07

You can also follow us on Instagram.

Time: 4374

On Instagram I do short neuroscience tutorials

Time: 4376.69

and tools and protocols.

Time: 4377.92

I cover recent papers,

Time: 4379.45

many of which are not included on the podcast.

Time: 4382.25

We also have a Patreon. It's patreon.com/andrewhuberman.

Time: 4386.7

And there you can support the podcast

Time: 4388.38

at any level that you like.

Time: 4390.12

Not so much today but on many previous episodes

Time: 4392.37

of the Huberman Lab Podcast, we discuss supplements,

Time: 4395.05

and while supplements aren't necessary for everybody,

Time: 4397.96

many people derive great benefit from supplements

Time: 4400.25

for sleep, for focus, and so forth.

Time: 4402.88

One issue with supplements, however, is that what's listed

Time: 4405.78

on the bottle of various supplements

Time: 4407.16

isn't always what's included in the bottle

Time: 4409.16

and the quality of ingredients varies tremendously

Time: 4411.75

across different supplement manufacturers.

Time: 4414.03

For that reason, we've partnered with Thorne,

Time: 4416.27

that's T-H-O-R-N-E,

Time: 4417.93

because Thorne supplements have the highest levels

Time: 4420.08

of stringency of any supplement company out there

Time: 4422.27

that we are aware of.

Time: 4423.41

They work with all the major sports teams.

Time: 4425.54

They work with the Mayo Clinic.

Time: 4426.66

And so we're delighted that we partnered with them.

Time: 4428.4

If you'd like to see the supplements that I take,

Time: 4430.3

you can go to Thorne, that's T-H-O-R-N-E, .com

Time: 4433.73

slash the letter U slash Huberman

Time: 4435.82

to see the supplements that I take

Time: 4437.18

and you can get 20% off any of those supplements.

Time: 4439.92

If you enter the Thorne site through that portal,

Time: 4442.27

you can also get 20% off any of the supplements

Time: 4444.96

that Thorne makes.

Time: 4446.2

Thank you for your time and attention today,

Time: 4448.2

and last but certainly not least,

Time: 4450.41

thank you for your interest in science.

Time: 4452.318

[bright guitar music]

Copyright © 2024. All rights reserved.