The Science of Love, Desire and Attachment

Time: 0.34

- Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,

Time: 2.02

where we discuss science and science-based tools

Time: 4.93

for everyday life.

Time: 5.763

[upbeat music]

Time: 9.27

I'm Andrew Huberman,

Time: 10.18

and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology

Time: 12.93

at Stanford School of Medicine.

Time: 14.81

Today, we are going to talk about the psychology

Time: 17

and the biology of desire, love and attachment.

Time: 21.13

Today happens to be Valentine's day, 2022.

Time: 24.63

However, the themes we are going to discuss

Time: 27.03

pertain to desire, love and attachment on any given day.

Time: 30.8

And indeed the mechanisms we are going to discuss

Time: 33.29

almost certainly were at play thousands of years ago,

Time: 36.13

hundreds of years ago, and no doubt will still be at play

Time: 39.5

in our minds, and in our bodies, and in our psychologies

Time: 42.42

for the decades, centuries and thousands of years to come.

Time: 45.74

Indeed today, I want to focus on core mechanisms

Time: 49.03

that lead individuals to seek out other individuals

Time: 52.33

with whom to mate with,

Time: 54.62

with whom to have children with or not,

Time: 57.14

with whom to enter short

Time: 58.65

or long-term relationships with,

Time: 60.67

and for perhaps to end those relationships

Time: 63.22

or to seek relationships on the side, so-called infidelity.

Time: 66.5

I'm certainly not going to encourage or discourage

Time: 68.4

any of these behaviors.

Time: 69.35

I'm simply going to cover the peer-reviewed scientific data

Time: 72.73

on all these aspects of desire, love, and attachment.

Time: 76.87

I'm going to discuss how our childhood attachment styles,

Time: 80.3

as they're called, influence our adult attachment styles.

Time: 84.78

Yes, you heard that right.

Time: 86.01

How we attached or did not attach to primary caregivers

Time: 90.7

in our childhood has much to do with how we attach

Time: 95.34

or fail to attach to romantic partners as adults,

Time: 99.08

because the same neural circuits,

Time: 101.95

the neurons and their connections in the brain and body

Time: 104.44

that underlie attachment between infant and caregiver,

Time: 107.77

between toddler and parent or other caregiver,

Time: 110.4

and during adolescence and in our teenage years

Time: 113.21

are repurposed for adult romantic attachments.

Time: 117.11

I know that might be a little eerie to think about,

Time: 119.27

but indeed that is true.

Time: 121.03

Now the fortunate thing is that regardless of our childhood

Time: 124.18

attachment styles and experiences, the neural circuits

Time: 127.84

for desire, love and attachment are quite plastic.

Time: 131.35

They are amenable to change in response to both

Time: 134.27

what we think and what we feel as well as what we do.

Time: 138.6

However, all three aspects that we're discussing today,

Time: 142.08

desire, love and attachment

Time: 144.09

are also strongly biologically driven.

Time: 147.03

We're going to talk about biological mechanisms

Time: 149.55

such as hormones,

Time: 151.11

biological mechanisms such as neurochemicals,

Time: 153.36

things like dopamine, oxytocin and serotonin,

Time: 156.31

and neural circuits, brain areas, and indeed areas

Time: 159.91

of the body that interact with the brain that control

Time: 162.93

whether or not we desire somebody or not,

Time: 165.18

whether or not we lose or increase our desire

Time: 167.41

for somebody over time, whether or not we fall in love,

Time: 171.01

what love means, and whether or not the relationships

Time: 174

we form continue to include the elements

Time: 176.8

of desire and love over time or not.

Time: 179.71

In order to illustrate just how powerfully our biology

Time: 182.15

can shape our perception of the attractiveness

Time: 184.09

of other people, I want to share with you the results

Time: 186.49

of a couple of studies.

Time: 187.91

Both studies explore

Time: 189.29

how people rate other people's attractiveness.

Time: 192.19

And in both studies, the major variable is that women

Time: 196.29

are at different stages of their menstrual cycle.

Time: 198.87

Now in the first study, men are rating the attractiveness

Time: 202.48

of women according to the smell of those women.

Time: 205.41

Now they're not smelling them directly.

Time: 206.81

They're smelling clothing that women wore for a couple

Time: 209.87

of days at different phases of their menstrual cycle.

Time: 212.9

And what they find is that men will rate the odors of women

Time: 216.31

as most attractive if those women wore

Time: 219.4

those shirts, that clothing,

Time: 221.8

in the pre-ovulatory phase of their cycle, okay?

Time: 224.81

So this is not to say that men do not find women attractive

Time: 228.54

at other stages of their cycle.

Time: 229.801

It is to say that men find women's odors

Time: 233.47

particularly attractive, if those odors were worn by women

Time: 237.54

that are in the pre-ovulatory phase

Time: 239.72

of their menstrual cycle, okay?

Time: 242.21

Now, there was also a study that was done where women

Time: 245.85

at different stages of their menstrual cycle

Time: 247.099

are rating the odors of men.

Time: 249.97

And a similar but mirror symmetric result was found

Time: 253.42

such that women who are in the pre-ovulatory phase

Time: 256.29

of their menstrual cycle will rate men's odors

Time: 259.53

as more attractive than at other stages of their cycle.

Time: 263.45

So the simple way to put this is that there seems

Time: 265.81

to be something special about the pre-ovulatory phase

Time: 268.71

of a woman's menstrual cycle that makes men rate them

Time: 271.52

as more attractive during that time and women rate men

Time: 275.26

as more attractive during that particular time as well.

Time: 278.51

So this is a bidirectional effect.

Time: 281.41

The way that the second study was done,

Time: 283.66

where women are rating men was not just to smell the odors

Time: 287.46

of those men on t-shirts, they did that,

Time: 290.2

but they correlated that with whether or not

Time: 292.37

the shirts were worn by men

Time: 293.69

that were particularly physically symmetrical.

Time: 296.66

They actually had these men divided into groups.

Time: 298.67

It was more of a continuum rather,

Time: 300.29

rated according to body symmetry and face symmetry.

Time: 303.55

And women preferred more symmetrical men

Time: 307.07

when they were doing the preference test

Time: 309.81

during the pre-ovulatory phase of their cycle.

Time: 312.44

So again, the point is that that pre-ovulatory

Time: 313.828

phase of the cycle seems to create

Time: 316.01

a bidirectional mutual attractiveness.

Time: 319.41

Now, also extremely interesting is that this effect

Time: 322.01

does really seem to have something to do with ovulation.

Time: 325.24

Because in both studies,

Time: 327.44

they had women that were taking oral contraception or not.

Time: 330.37

And what they found was if a woman is taking

Time: 333.18

oral contraception, it prevented that peak and perceived

Time: 336.55

attractiveness by the men, meaning men no longer perceived

Time: 341.43

a woman to be more attractive

Time: 343.18

at a particular phase of their cycle.

Time: 345.31

And also women taking oral contraception no longer preferred

Time: 349.38

the odors of more symmetrical men

Time: 351.73

during the pre-ovulatory phase of their cycle.

Time: 354.19

Now, I want to make sure that it's especially clear

Time: 357.17

that it is not the case that oral contraception reduced

Time: 361.43

the perception of a woman as attractive,

Time: 363.52

that did not happen in these studies.

Time: 365.33

It reduced the further increase in a male's perception

Time: 369.53

of her as attractive.

Time: 370.58

And if women took oral contraception,

Time: 373.23

it prevented them from preferring more symmetrical men

Time: 378.12

based on the odors of those men.

Time: 380.15

Now I realize there are a lot of variables here.

Time: 381.68

We've got odors, we've got symmetry,

Time: 382.783

we've got menstrual cycle, pre-ovulatory, non pre-ovulatory.

Time: 386.24

And we have whether or not people

Time: 388.21

are taking contraception or not.

Time: 389.71

But the basic finding is that depending on where women are

Time: 392.68

in their menstrual cycle, influences both men's perception

Time: 395.81

of them as attractive and their perception of men

Time: 399.45

as attractive and oral contraception eliminates that effect.

Time: 403.39

So I share with you those data to illustrate that

Time: 406.35

we often think that somebody is attractive or not based on,

Time: 410.5

I don't know, how they look,

Time: 412.39

their skin, their hair, et cetera,

Time: 414.53

but it also illustrates that their odor is a powerful cue

Time: 418.4

for some people more than others.

Time: 419.81

Some of us tend to be more factory driven than others.

Time: 422.03

Although if you watched the "Huberman Lab Podcast" episode

Time: 424.76

that I did with Professor David Buss

Time: 426.75

from the University of Texas, Austin,

Time: 428.55

who's a luminary in the field of evolutionary psychology

Time: 432.38

and has studied mate choice

Time: 434.01

and mate selection bias over decades,

Time: 436.44

he's really one of the founders of that field,

Time: 438.95

he emphasized findings that odor for many people

Time: 443.22

is a maker or a deal breaker.

Time: 446.29

Meaning, there are some people that even if somebody has

Time: 449.56

all the characteristics that they're looking for in terms

Time: 452.17

of kindness, and attractiveness, and values,

Time: 454.56

and other features that would and should be

Time: 457.08

of very high priority in selecting a mate,

Time: 459.49

that if someone does not like the way that person smells,

Time: 463.33

their innate body odor independent of colognes,

Time: 465.842

and perfumes, and soaps, et cetera,

Time: 468.5

that that's often a complete and total deal breaker.

Time: 471.51

I'm sure there are some of you that can relate to that.

Time: 473.25

And there's some of you perhaps for which that is

Time: 475.76

not the case, and you can't even imagine that being

Time: 477.9

such a powerful variable.

Time: 479.095

And yet the data suggested indeed it is a powerful variable

Time: 483.46

for many people out there.

Time: 484.88

Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast

Time: 487.35

is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.

Time: 490.13

It is however, part of my desire and effort

Time: 492.43

to bring zero cost to consumer information about science

Time: 495.027

and science-related tools to the general public.

Time: 497.8

In keeping with that theme,

Time: 498.87

I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.

Time: 501.58

Our first sponsor is Thesis.

Time: 503.51

Thesis makes custom nootropics.

Time: 505.84

Now nootropics is not a word that I'm usually a fan of,

Time: 508.99

because nootropic means smart drug.

Time: 511.42

And there are a lot of different aspects

Time: 512.582

to being smart or to intelligence.

Time: 515.04

There's the ability to focus.

Time: 516.44

There's the ability to task switch.

Time: 518.51

There's the ability to be creative.

Time: 520.72

And each of those different forms of intelligence

Time: 522.98

or expressions of intelligence involves

Time: 524.94

different brain circuits, different neurochemicals,

Time: 527.22

and indeed different hormones in the body.

Time: 530.37

So I think it's rather naive to think that

Time: 532.38

there could be one smart drug or nootropic.

Time: 534.85

Thesis understands this.

Time: 535.969

And so they've developed custom nootropics that are tailored

Time: 539.24

to the specific goals that you might have

Time: 541.21

in terms of cognitive work, or physical exercise,

Time: 544.29

or skill learning of any kind.

Time: 546.06

And they tailor them to the individual.

Time: 548.46

They only use the highest quality ingredients,

Time: 550.86

many of which I've talked about here on the podcast.

Time: 553.05

Things like alpha GPC, which I personally use,

Time: 555.3

things like phosphatidylserine,

Time: 556.754

which I also use on occasion.

Time: 559.01

However, they've taken different combinations

Time: 561.16

of those ingredients and put them into different

Time: 563.7

formulations designed for particular goals or endpoints.

Time: 567.36

For instance, I have Thesis nootropics

Time: 569.68

that are for motivation, others that are for clarity,

Time: 573.93

for mental clarity, others that I take pre-workout,

Time: 577.07

others that I take post-workout, and so on and so forth.

Time: 580.41

Now, in addition to that Thesis understands that

Time: 583.32

not every ingredient is terrific for everybody.

Time: 586.21

In fact, one particular substance that I've talked about

Time: 588.71

on the podcast, Ginkgo biloba is very useful

Time: 592.25

for a lot of people.

Time: 593.17

However, I don't tolerate it well.

Time: 594.61

I get vicious headaches from Ginkgo biloba.

Time: 596.71

So none of the formulations that they've made

Time: 598.27

for me include Ginkgo biloba.

Time: 600.55

So again, these are custom nootropics that are tailored

Time: 603.08

for your particular goals,

Time: 604.53

and where the ingredients really match

Time: 606.51

your particular needs,

Time: 607.67

and don't include things that aren't going to work for you,

Time: 610.42

or that create things like headaches.

Time: 612.63

So the personalization and the targeted effects

Time: 615.04

of the nootropics is really where or the power comes from.

Time: 617.23

I've been using Thesis nootropics

Time: 619.07

for close to six months now.

Time: 620.45

And I can confidently say that their nootropics

Time: 622.77

have been a total game changer for me.

Time: 624.75

My go-to formula is the motivation formula.

Time: 628.16

If I'm going to be training, or working out,

Time: 629.99

or I need extra energy,

Time: 631.12

and I use the clarity formula nowadays

Time: 633.25

for any kind of writing or creative work.

Time: 635.37

Those are the two that I've mainly been taking lately.

Time: 637.89

If you want to try your own personalized

Time: 639.176

nootropic starter kit, you can go online

Time: 641.99

to takethesis.com/huberman.

Time: 644.68

You'll take a three minute quiz and Thesis will send you

Time: 646.96

four different formulas to try in your first month.

Time: 649.55

You'll have the ability to try those different blends

Time: 651.8

across that month, discover which nootropics

Time: 653.95

work best for you, your unique brain chemistry in genetics.

Time: 657.39

You can have a consult with them,

Time: 659.17

and then they will give you the best nootropics for you.

Time: 662.14

Again, that's takethesis.com/huberman

Time: 664.63

and use the code Huberman at checkout

Time: 666.53

to gain 10% off your first box.

Time: 668.86

Today's episode is also brought to us by Athletic Greens.

Time: 671.89

Now called AG1.

Time: 673.49

I've been taking AG1 since 2012.

Time: 675.89

So I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast.

Time: 678.17

The reason I started taking AG1

Time: 679.61

and the reason I still take AG1 once or twice a day

Time: 682.68

is that it covers all of my vitamin mineral probiotic needs.

Time: 686.1

Probiotics are essential because they support what's called

Time: 689.07

a healthy gut microbiome.

Time: 690.62

The gut microbiome is vital for things like

Time: 692.92

metabolism, hormone function,

Time: 694.55

and also we now know our brain function,

Time: 697

things like focus and memory and our general immune system.

Time: 700.04

With AG1, I get the probiotics I need,

Time: 702.17

I get the vitamins and minerals that I need

Time: 703.79

to cover any nutritional gaps, if I'm not eating optimally.

Time: 707.22

And even if I am eating optimally, AG1 can further support

Time: 710.95

metabolism, hormone function, et cetera.

Time: 712.68

In fact, whenever people ask me,

Time: 714.57

what's the one supplement that I should take

Time: 716.52

if I can only take one supplement?

Time: 717.99

I always say AG1.

Time: 719.256

I take mine early in the day.

Time: 721.32

I mix it with water and some lemon or lime juice.

Time: 723.41

I love the way it tastes.

Time: 724.26

And I'll take it again later in the day,

Time: 725.91

typically in the late afternoon.

Time: 727.87

If you'd like to try Athletic Greens,

Time: 729.3

you can go to athleticgreens.com/huberman

Time: 731.88

and claim a special offer.

Time: 733.49

They're giving you five free travel packs

Time: 735.32

that make it really easy to mix up AG1

Time: 737.17

while you're on the road, in the car,

Time: 738.43

on the plane, et cetera, and a year supply of vitamin D3 K2.

Time: 742.89

There's a ton of data now supporting the fact that

Time: 745.71

vitamin D3 is critical and that most of us don't get enough

Time: 748.91

vitamin D3 even if we're getting ample sunlight.

Time: 751.68

Vitamin D three is important from metabolism,

Time: 753.51

hormone function, brain function,

Time: 754.81

and many other aspects of our biology.

Time: 757.22

So again, if you go to athleticgreens.com/huberman,

Time: 760.29

you can get a special offer of the Athletic Greens,

Time: 762.42

five free travel packs and the year supply of vitamin D3 K2.

Time: 766.37

Today's episode is also brought to us by Inside Tracker.

Time: 769.66

Inside Tracker is a personalized nutrition platform

Time: 771.927

that analyzes data from your blood and DNA

Time: 775.22

to help you better understand your body

Time: 776.79

and help you reach your health goals.

Time: 778.93

I've long been a believer

Time: 780.09

in getting regular blood work done.

Time: 781.84

For the simple reason that many of the factors

Time: 784.24

that influence your immediate and long-term health

Time: 786.56

can only be discovered from a quality blood test.

Time: 789.65

And nowadays with the advent of modern DNA tests,

Time: 792.56

you can also get information for instance about

Time: 794.7

how your biological age compares to your chronological age,

Time: 798.04

which of course is a vital measurement.

Time: 800.82

Now, one of the major issues with blood tests and DNA tests

Time: 803.81

out there is that people get the information back

Time: 806.4

that lipid marker of one type, it might be high or low,

Time: 810.75

or that a hormone of another type might be high or low,

Time: 813.51

but they don't give you any for information

Time: 814.532

about what to do with that information.

Time: 816.7

Inside Tracker makes that all very easy to navigate.

Time: 819.76

Once you get your results back,

Time: 821.3

you can click on any of those results.

Time: 823.31

And Inside Tracker will immediately show you things

Time: 825.74

that you can do, for instance, with your nutrition,

Time: 828.01

or supplementation, or lifestyle factors

Time: 830.47

to help you bring those numbers into the ranges

Time: 832.54

that are appropriate for you.

Time: 834.38

So it's immensely powerful, not just in terms

Time: 836.51

of the measurements, but also it provides some directives

Time: 839.27

that can help bring those measurements into the ranges

Time: 841.67

that are best for your immediate and long-term health.

Time: 844.22

If you'd like to try Inside Tracker,

Time: 845.65

you can visit insidetracker.com/huberman

Time: 848.2

to get 20% off any of Inside Tracker's plans.

Time: 850.94

Just use the code Huberman at checkout.

Time: 853.21

Let's talk about desire, love, and attachment.

Time: 855.45

And of course, these are topics

Time: 857.081

that grab tremendous interest.

Time: 858.79

So it's worth us defining our terms a little bit

Time: 861.8

before going any further.

Time: 863.86

Of course, we can have many different kinds of loves.

Time: 866.93

There's romantic love, there's love of family,

Time: 869.99

so-called familial love, there's love of pets.

Time: 872.64

We can even love objects,

Time: 874.27

where we can feel as if we love objects.

Time: 876.31

We can love certain activities.

Time: 877.71

We can have friends that we love and so on and so forth.

Time: 880.26

The word love is used to encompass

Time: 881.83

a lot of different types of relationships.

Time: 884.45

Today, we are mainly going to be focused on romantic love

Time: 887.9

and the neural mechanisms of romantic love.

Time: 891.21

I want to acknowledge here at the outset

Time: 892.78

that most of the studies of romantic love

Time: 895.13

have focused on monogamous heterosexual love.

Time: 898.86

And also when we talk about studies focused on desire,

Time: 902.3

and attractiveness, and attachment, that's also the case.

Time: 905.29

And that simply reflects the general bias of the literature

Time: 908.27

over the last 50 to 100 years.

Time: 910.66

It does, of course, not rule out that similar

Time: 913.21

or different mechanisms could be at play

Time: 915.43

in non-monogamous relationships,

Time: 917.57

in homosexual relationships,

Time: 919.6

or in relationships of any kind or variation.

Time: 922.93

It's also worth us defining our terms around desire.

Time: 926.42

It can mean lust, it can mean the desire

Time: 929.37

for long-term partnership.

Time: 930.95

So we need to define our terms and throughout I will do

Time: 933.52

my best to very carefully define what I mean by desire,

Time: 937.35

what I mean by love, and what I mean by attachment.

Time: 941.89

The formal study of love, and desire, and attachment

Time: 946.35

goes back to the early 1900s.

Time: 949.9

One of the classic studies on this is entitled

Time: 952.607

"Love and Desire."

Time: 953.66

It was published in 1912 and really focused

Time: 956.57

on two opposing themes within romance.

Time: 960.95

One is love which in that paper was really meant to include

Time: 966.2

attachment and dependence or interdependence

Time: 969.44

between individuals, right?

Time: 971.66

And the other end of the spectrum being desire,

Time: 974.36

or the sexual desire for another.

Time: 977.23

And romance was meant to encapsulate both those things,

Time: 980.58

love and desire.

Time: 982.33

And for much of the 1900s, it was thought that love

Time: 986.43

and desire were on opposing ends or in a push pull.

Time: 990.29

And it was the dynamic push and pull between love and desire

Time: 995.3

that one could define romance.

Time: 998.01

And that actually led to much of what's out there

Time: 1000.36

in the psychological literature.

Time: 1002.23

Today, we are going to explore some neurobiological studies,

Time: 1006.1

some studies of the endocrine system,

Time: 1008.27

meaning the hormone system

Time: 1009.47

that actually support that general model.

Time: 1011.37

And I'll point you toward what I think is a very useful book

Time: 1014.908

in thinking about how relationships can both form and last

Time: 1019.51

over long periods of time and how those relationships

Time: 1023.23

can include both desire and interdependence.

Time: 1027.93

I'll also talk about some studies that have really focused

Time: 1030.58

on why relationships succeed and why they fail,

Time: 1034.17

and how that relates to whether or not there is sufficient

Time: 1037.32

amounts of attachment and desire.

Time: 1039.45

So today we're going to talk about the science

Time: 1040.78

and indeed you'll also get some tools.

Time: 1042.74

Those tools should be useful to you, whether or not

Time: 1044.4

you happen to be in a relationship or not,

Time: 1047.19

whether or not you're seeking a relationship or not.

Time: 1049.35

I'd like to begin with an anecdote.

Time: 1050.97

And this is not an anecdote

Time: 1052.35

about my relationship history,

Time: 1054.345

it's an anecdote about my scientific history.

Time: 1058.12

When I started graduate school,

Time: 1059.5

the chairman of the department I was in at the time

Time: 1061.98

said to me, "Most PhDs last longer than most marriages."

Time: 1067.16

And indeed he was right.

Time: 1068.66

And also most marriages in this country end in divorce.

Time: 1072.48

I think it's about 50% with a slight skew toward more ending

Time: 1076.72

in divorce than persist until death do them part.

Time: 1081.43

But nonetheless, it's about half,

Time: 1083.77

and most marriages end before the eight-year period is up.

Time: 1087.83

Most PhDs take anywhere from four to nine years.

Time: 1091.7

So there was a bit of a smearing of averages there,

Time: 1093.88

but the point he was trying to make

Time: 1095.18

really landed home for me.

Time: 1097.22

It did not scare me out of relationships

Time: 1100.3

nor did it scare me out of a PhD, obviously.

Time: 1103.08

What it did illustrate was that there's something

Time: 1106.01

about our attachment machinery

Time: 1108.7

that can be very, very compelling,

Time: 1110.7

such that people take on tremendous levels of commitment.

Time: 1113.97

I have to imagine that most people enter marriages

Time: 1116.34

assuming that they're going to stay in those marriages.

Time: 1117.612

I don't think most people enter marriages thinking

Time: 1120.32

they're going to get divorced.

Time: 1121.51

But that if 50% of those commitments end in divorce,

Time: 1126.16

there must also be mechanisms

Time: 1127.9

by which our attachments can break.

Time: 1130.74

And today we're going to talk about both the forming

Time: 1132.86

of attachments and the breaking of attachments,

Time: 1135.36

what can prevent those breaks in attachments,

Time: 1137.68

and indeed what can lead to re-attachments.

Time: 1141.04

There are biological mechanisms to desire, love

Time: 1145.94

and attachment, that's abundantly clear.

Time: 1148.77

Now there's a robust and very large literature

Time: 1152.58

in animal models.

Time: 1153.89

What I mean by that are field studies and laboratory studies

Time: 1157.38

in primates of different kinds,

Time: 1159.08

such as macaque monkeys or bonobos.

Time: 1162.28

People have looked at these sorts of things,

Time: 1163.97

believe it or not, in ducks, in laboratory mice,

Time: 1166.8

in different types of birds, et cetera.

Time: 1169.51

And if you look at that literature, you can essentially find

Time: 1172.04

biological examples in the animal kingdom for just about

Time: 1176.18

any behavior that you can easily map to human behavior.

Time: 1179.86

So for instance, there's a species of animal called

Time: 1182.8

the prairie vole.

Time: 1183.83

In one portion of the United States,

Time: 1186.35

this prairie vole species is monogamous.

Time: 1188.43

They only mate with one other prairie vole,

Time: 1191.61

only raise the young with one other prairie vole

Time: 1193.57

for their entire life.

Time: 1195.51

And in another region of the United States,

Time: 1198.157

the same species of animal, prairie vole,

Time: 1201.12

will mate with many individuals, they're non-monogamous.

Time: 1204.09

And the major difference, at least as far as we know

Time: 1206.42

between the prairie voles in one location

Time: 1208.43

and another location is the levels of a molecule

Time: 1211.96

called vasopressin in the brain and body.

Time: 1213.94

Vasopressin is present in humans.

Time: 1216.01

It has numerous biological roles.

Time: 1218.41

It's responsible, for instance, for controlling

Time: 1220.6

the amount of urine that you excrete,

Time: 1222.44

the amount of water that you retain,

Time: 1224.14

and for sexual desire, as well as mate seeking.

Time: 1229.78

Levels of vasopressin in prairie voles

Time: 1231.73

are strongly determinant of whether or not a prairie vole

Time: 1235.46

is going to be a monogamous or non-monogamous.

Time: 1238.34

Why do I raise this?

Time: 1239.25

Well, I raise this because the literature on prairie voles

Time: 1243.56

is quite beautiful and has been discussed quite a lot

Time: 1247.03

in the popular press.

Time: 1247.863

You can look it up with an easily, just Web Edge search.

Time: 1251.01

You'll find lots of information about this,

Time: 1252.55

lots of news articles about this,

Time: 1254.04

and lots of interpretations as to how vasopressin might

Time: 1257.12

be involved in similar or different mechanisms in humans.

Time: 1260.65

Now, I don't have a problem

Time: 1261.956

with mapping animal studies to humans.

Time: 1264.33

I think there's certainly a place for that.

Time: 1265.59

But if we just lean back and look at the giant mass

Time: 1270.39

of studies in animals and their mating behavior,

Time: 1273.707

and their mate selection behavior,

Time: 1275.48

you can essentially find examples of anything.

Time: 1277.87

You can find examples of polygamy,

Time: 1280.05

you can find examples of cheating, of infidelity,

Time: 1283.45

you can find examples of all sorts of different behaviors

Time: 1286.23

that in your own mind, you can map to human behavior.

Time: 1289.31

But it's really hard to make the leap from animal models

Time: 1292.6

to humans in any kind of direct way.

Time: 1294.57

And so thankfully there's been tremendous work done

Time: 1297.9

in the last mainly 20 years or so,

Time: 1300.28

looking at human mate selection,

Time: 1302.5

human desire, human love and human attachment.

Time: 1305.47

So we're mainly going to focus on those studies today,

Time: 1307.47

and where appropriate, we will map those findings back

Time: 1311.22

to the findings in animals to see if there's some universal

Time: 1314.24

truths or some universal principles

Time: 1316.8

about how the neural circuits

Time: 1318.31

and biological mechanisms work.

Time: 1320.03

But by and large, we're going to focus on human studies today.

Time: 1322.34

So unless I say otherwise, the data that I'm referring

Time: 1324.69

to today are entirely from human beings.

Time: 1327.21

So let's talk about attachment and attachment styles.

Time: 1330.24

And this will offer you the opportunity

Time: 1331.9

to answer some important questions for yourself,

Time: 1335.17

such as what is my, meaning,

Time: 1337.95

your attachment style in relationship?

Time: 1341.86

One of the most robust findings in the field of psychology

Time: 1345.38

is this notion of attachment styles.

Time: 1347.72

And this was something that was discovered

Time: 1349.66

through a beautiful set of studies that were done

Time: 1351.92

by Mary Ainsworth in the 1980s,

Time: 1354.78

in which she developed a laboratory condition

Time: 1357.28

called the strange situation task.

Time: 1359.93

Now, the strange situation task has been studied

Time: 1362.56

over and over again in different cultures,

Time: 1365.15

in different locations throughout the world.

Time: 1367.4

And in preparing for this episode,

Time: 1369.62

I actually spoke to three different psychologists.

Time: 1371.44

I spoke to a psychoanalyst,

Time: 1372.75

I spoke to a cognitive behavioral psychologist,

Time: 1374.6

and I actually spoke to a psychiatrist, excuse me,

Time: 1376.39

not a psychologist, but a psychiatrist with a medical degree

Time: 1379.16

and asked, is the strange situation task

Time: 1382

and the various attachment styles that emerge

Time: 1384.91

from that task, are those still considered valid?

Time: 1388.29

And indeed all three of them said if ever there was

Time: 1390.63

a literature in psychology that is absolutely tamped down

Time: 1395.12

and has a firm basis in both data and real world principles

Time: 1400.17

and real world examples,

Time: 1402

it's this notion of attachment styles.

Time: 1405.55

So what is the strange situation task?

Time: 1407.6

The strange situation task involves a parent,

Time: 1410.19

typically a mother in the studies that were done,

Time: 1412.78

but a parent or other caregiver bringing their child,

Time: 1416.93

their actual child into a laboratory.

Time: 1419.56

And there's a room with a stranger and the mother enters

Time: 1424.19

the room with the child and there are some toys in the room.

Time: 1428.07

And typically the mother and the stranger will talk.

Time: 1431.44

Obviously the stranger is part of the experiment,

Time: 1433.28

is not just some random person off the street.

Time: 1435.65

And the child is allowed to move about the room.

Time: 1438.18

They can observe the mother interacting

Time: 1440.19

with the other person or not,

Time: 1441.404

they can play with toys or not,

Time: 1443.51

but then at some point the mother leaves.

Time: 1446.82

And then at some point later designated by the experimenter,

Time: 1449.55

the mother comes back.

Time: 1451.54

And what is measured in these studies is both how the child,

Time: 1457.2

the toddler reacts to the mother leaving,

Time: 1461.01

and how the child reacts to the mother returning

Time: 1464.05

at the end of the experiment.

Time: 1465.97

And oftentimes this will have two or three different phases

Time: 1469.72

where the mother will bring the child in then leave,

Time: 1472.9

then come back in and leave.

Time: 1475.49

There are also studies in which the behavior of the child

Time: 1479.29

with the stranger is also examined.

Time: 1482.12

So there are a lot of variations of this,

Time: 1483.58

but the basic findings are that toddlers, children

Time: 1489.61

fall into four different categories of attachment style.

Time: 1494.79

And that these attachment styles can predict many features

Time: 1498.3

of adolescent, teen, young adult, and even adult

Time: 1502.03

attachment styles, not in strange situations

Time: 1505.17

of the sort that I just ascribed,

Time: 1506.69

but in romantic attachments.

Time: 1510.32

I should mention also that attachment style is plastic,

Time: 1514.61

meaning it can change across the lifespan.

Time: 1517.28

So as I describe the results, I describe the different

Time: 1519.74

attachment styles that are out there,

Time: 1522.56

and if any of those resonate with you or bring to mind

Time: 1527.32

certain people in your life, please do not assume that

Time: 1530.38

those attachment styles are rigid and fixed

Time: 1533.26

for the entire lifespan.

Time: 1534.28

There are also terrific data that indicate that

Time: 1537.27

through specific processes, both psychological

Time: 1540.41

and some biological adjustments

Time: 1543.53

that people can change their attachment style,

Time: 1546.03

and that indeed people who have different attachment styles

Time: 1548.62

can change the attachment styles of others.

Time: 1550.92

But just to make very clear what the results

Time: 1552.89

of the study were, I want to review what the four different

Time: 1555.61

attachment styles are, and typically people fall

Time: 1557.84

into one group or another but not several.

Time: 1561.01

So the four patterns of attachment that were revealed

Time: 1563.59

by these studies, again, were revealed by examining

Time: 1567.5

the behavior of the child in response to the mother leaving

Time: 1570.77

and the mother returning, and the child's response

Time: 1573.83

to the stranger that is in the room with them.

Time: 1576.72

The first style is the so-called secure attachment style.

Time: 1581.12

In the nomenclature of this kind of study,

Time: 1583.61

these are the so-called B babies,

Time: 1585.13

as in the letter B, bulldog, B.

Time: 1587.69

Not for bulldogs, but just to designate this category.

Time: 1591.01

The secure attachment style is one in which the child

Time: 1595.83

will engage with the stranger, with the experimenter

Time: 1599.08

while the parent is present in the room.

Time: 1602.33

But that when the parent, typically it's a mother,

Time: 1604.51

but when the parent or other caregiver leaves,

Time: 1607.89

the child does get visibly upset.

Time: 1610

They might whine, they might cry,

Time: 1611.47

they might even tantrum a bit.

Time: 1613.49

However, when the caregiver, meaning the mother, or father,

Time: 1617.23

or other caregiver returns, the child visibly expresses

Time: 1621.5

happiness that the caregiver has returned, okay?

Time: 1624.03

So that's the hallmark of the secure attachment style.

Time: 1627.73

And again, this is all pre-verbal.

Time: 1629.243

This is happening long before the child can express

Time: 1631.73

how they feel with words.

Time: 1633.85

And the interpretation of this is that the secure child

Time: 1636.78

feels confident that the caregiver is available

Time: 1639.87

and will be responsive to their needs

Time: 1642.36

and their communications.

Time: 1643.68

So that when the child whines and, or is distressed,

Time: 1648.16

the parent doesn't come right back into the room.

Time: 1649.9

But at some point they do and they seem to have a sense

Time: 1652.83

of trust that if the parent or caregiver leaves

Time: 1655.57

that the parent will come back,

Time: 1657.28

and that they're happy that they do.

Time: 1659.84

These children are also very good at exploring

Time: 1662.86

novel environments after the parent is gone,

Time: 1665.33

and while the parent is there.

Time: 1666.59

And almost always, when the parent is there,

Time: 1669.59

they will explore more broadly,

Time: 1670.841

literally in space, they'll venture out further

Time: 1674.08

than they will when the parent is gone.

Time: 1676.84

They also tend to engage with the caregiver in a way

Time: 1679.57

that's not immediately and completely trusting,

Time: 1681.88

but that over time seems to evolve from one

Time: 1684.61

in which they're kind of suspicious of this person

Time: 1686.37

to one in which they're at least somewhat trusting, okay?

Time: 1689.89

So these were the general contours

Time: 1691.98

of the secure attachment style.

Time: 1693.82

And fortunately, nowadays there are physiological studies

Time: 1696.6

measuring things like heart rate and breathing

Time: 1698.68

and other measures that correlate with the subjective

Time: 1702.44

assessment of what these children are feeling.

Time: 1705.08

Okay, so first category is secure attached.

Time: 1707.18

The second category is a so-called anxious-avoidant

Time: 1710.81

or insecurely attached, which are the category A babies.

Time: 1716.2

The children with anxious-avoidant insecure

Time: 1718.11

attachment patterns generally tend to avoid

Time: 1721.19

or ignore the caregiver, all right?

Time: 1723.59

Meaning the parent, and show very little emotion

Time: 1727.68

when the parent leaves or returns.

Time: 1731.26

So this is the reason they call them avoidant

Time: 1733.99

or anxious-avoidant and kind of insecure.

Time: 1736.42

There isn't this happiness or joy that the parent is back.

Time: 1739.39

They don't seem to express that.

Time: 1741.31

They do not exhibit distress on separation.

Time: 1744.57

And they generally tend to have some tendency to approach

Time: 1749.09

the caregiver when they return,

Time: 1751.48

but there doesn't seem to be a general expression of joy.

Time: 1754.31

And again, physiological measures support that as well.

Time: 1756.96

Things like changes in heart rate tend to be less dramatic

Time: 1760.73

in the anxious-avoidant insecure attachment style

Time: 1764.25

than in the secure attachment style.

Time: 1765.685

Okay, so that's the second one.

Time: 1768.75

The third category is the so-called

Time: 1770.7

anxious-ambivalent/resistant-insecure category.

Time: 1775.43

Okay, I didn't name these categories.

Time: 1776.75

So you have to blame others in this one instance.

Time: 1779.86

For everything else blame me, but in this instance,

Time: 1781.88

you have to blame the psychologists

Time: 1783.147

that named this category.

Time: 1784.91

The anxious-ambivalent/resistant-insecure category

Time: 1788.7

also called the C babies,

Time: 1791.04

for the letter C just as a categorization.

Time: 1794.05

The anxious-ambivalent, resistant-insecure toddlers, really,

Time: 1798.81

show distress even before separation from their mother

Time: 1801.97

or other caregiver.

Time: 1803.19

And they tend to be very clingy and difficult to comfort

Time: 1806.45

when the caregiver returns, okay?

Time: 1809.08

So they're distressed even before the mother leaves the room

Time: 1812.21

and they tend to be very clingy and really hard to calm down

Time: 1815.43

when the mother returns.

Time: 1818.25

They tend to show either what seems to be resentment

Time: 1821.59

in response to the parent's absence.

Time: 1823.48

We don't really know what they're feeling.

Time: 1824.95

Or some helpless passivity.

Time: 1826.97

And there's actually subcategorizations

Time: 1828.27

that the psychologists have come up with, C one subtypes

Time: 1831.18

and C two subtypes.

Time: 1832.013

We don't have to get bogged down in that.

Time: 1833.59

But just know that there isn't one absolute measure

Time: 1837

that says, oh, well, this person

Time: 1839.22

is anxious-ambivalent, resistant-insecure.

Time: 1841.66

They could be somewhat passive, or they could be,

Time: 1845.53

seem somewhat angry at the caregiver.

Time: 1847.77

But the basic idea is that before and after the separation,

Time: 1852.81

they are clingy and difficult to comfort.

Time: 1855.28

They just can't seem to calm themselves down,

Time: 1856.97

and physiological measures of heart rate and hormone

Time: 1859.56

measurements such as cortisol also support that statement.

Time: 1862.84

And the third category of attachment style is the so-called

Time: 1866.7

disorganized or disoriented or D for the letter D babies.

Time: 1871.16

This is a categorization that was added later

Time: 1873.75

to this strange situation task.

Time: 1875.62

That is a real hallmark of developmental psychology studies.

Time: 1879.41

It was developed by Mary Ainsworth, graduate student,

Time: 1883.32

Mary Maine, who I actually had the great fortune of taking

Time: 1886.11

a course from and learning from when I was a graduate

Time: 1887.97

student at Berkeley many years ago.

Time: 1890.69

And this fourth categorization was controversial

Time: 1892.82

for a while, but now is generally accepted.

Time: 1895.75

The key feature of the disorganized, disoriented category

Time: 1900.17

is that the toddlers tend to be tense

Time: 1903.48

and they tend to encompass a lot of odd physical postures.

Time: 1907.61

They tend to hunch their shoulders.

Time: 1909.97

They'll put their hands behind their neck.

Time: 1911.72

They'll cock their head to the side.

Time: 1914.24

For those of you listening,

Time: 1915.15

I'm doing this on the video version.

Time: 1916.75

It's not where you don't have to go see that.

Time: 1919.02

But for those of you that are watching this on video,

Time: 1921.48

they tend to constrain their body size a bit

Time: 1924.34

and going to odd postures that they normally wouldn't

Time: 1928.04

do anywhere else.

Time: 1930.41

So this is why it's called the disorganized

Time: 1932.53

or disoriented category.

Time: 1935.06

It seems like these children just don't really know

Time: 1937.19

how to react to a separation.

Time: 1939.13

And they just start to manifest behaviors and emotional

Time: 1942.38

tones that aren't observed in other situations.

Time: 1945.71

Okay, so we've got our four categories.

Time: 1947.83

I'll try and use the shortest possible names

Time: 1949.55

for each category.

Time: 1950.383

We've got category one, which is securely attached.

Time: 1952.67

We've got category two, which is insecurely attached,

Time: 1955.76

also sometimes called anxious-avoidant.

Time: 1958.17

Then we've got category three,

Time: 1959.56

which is the resistant-insecure category,

Time: 1962.75

which is anxious-ambivalent.

Time: 1964.09

And then there's this fourth category,

Time: 1965.87

the disorganized, disoriented category

Time: 1968.45

where they're so-called D babies.

Time: 1970.95

Now, what's interesting about this from the perspective

Time: 1974.73

of desire, love and attachment is that the categorizations

Time: 1979.94

of children into one of these four different categories

Time: 1983.52

as toddlers is strongly predictive of their attachment style

Time: 1988.31

in romantic partnerships later in life.

Time: 1991.31

Which is to me both amazing and surprising

Time: 1994.93

and not surprising all at the same time.

Time: 1997.1

Amazing because it means that, first of all,

Time: 2000.92

we are relatively hardwired for attachment.

Time: 2003.72

I think that that's incredible and beautiful,

Time: 2006.68

that we have designated neurons, nerve cells

Time: 2009.7

and hormonal systems that are there to ensure

Time: 2012.65

that we have some response to a caregiver being there,

Time: 2017.09

or not being there, or returning, or leaving,

Time: 2020.21

but also that the same neural circuitries, the same hormonal

Time: 2023.86

responses are at least in some way repurposed

Time: 2027.44

for entirely different types of attachments later in life.

Time: 2031.3

So when we hear the psychologist talk about how we formed

Time: 2036.29

a template early in life based on experiences

Time: 2038.8

that were even pre-verbal before we had language,

Time: 2041.55

and those templates are superimposed on our relationships,

Time: 2045.33

or we should say, our later relationships are superimposed

Time: 2048.12

on those templates, there really is a basis for that.

Time: 2050.69

We now have neuroimaging studies to support, for instance,

Time: 2054.07

the work of Allan Schore from UCLA, showing that

Time: 2057.92

when a mother and child interact, either through very

Time: 2061.87

soothing interactions, like bottle feeding,

Time: 2064.08

or breastfeeding, or singing to one's baby,

Time: 2066.93

or putting them to sleep, that the brain of the child

Time: 2070.32

and the brain of the mother

Time: 2071.61

are entering a coordinated state of relaxation.

Time: 2074.68

And it's not one direction, mother to child,

Time: 2076.96

the child is also calming the mother.

Time: 2080.45

Typically these studies were done with mothers again,

Time: 2082.38

sometimes with fathers, but typically with mothers.

Time: 2085.15

And in addition to that, when the mother or other caregiver

Time: 2089.02

acts very excited and raises their voice,

Time: 2091.93

or puts a lilt in their voice, or widens their eyes,

Time: 2094.82

that the child will do the same.

Time: 2096.116

And again, there's a bidirectional interaction

Time: 2099.11

in that case of excitement.

Time: 2101.15

And there's the release of neurochemicals like dopamine

Time: 2103.96

into the bloodstream, whereas in the relaxation scenario

Time: 2106.64

and the soothing scenario, there's we know the release

Time: 2109.56

of things like serotonin and oxytocin.

Time: 2111.85

So the neural systems for attachment and the neural systems

Time: 2116.88

for what we call autonomic arousal for being alert and calm

Time: 2121.46

don't act in a vacuum.

Time: 2123.74

They are tethered to other people in our environment.

Time: 2126.42

And of course we know this, right?

Time: 2127.93

We sometimes hear the statement,

Time: 2129.17

no one can make you feel anything.

Time: 2130.78

I've always had a little bit of a problem

Time: 2132

with that statement.

Time: 2134.02

I don't think I'm contradicting anyone in particular,

Time: 2136.89

but you hear that a lot, no one can make you feel anything.

Time: 2139.43

Indeed they can, right?

Time: 2140.62

A physical injury can make you feel something.

Time: 2143.27

If somebody says something that you very much like,

Time: 2145.49

it can make you feel something.

Time: 2146.387

And if somebody says something that you very much dislike,

Time: 2149.33

it will make you feel something.

Time: 2150.61

So the idea that no one can make us feel anything

Time: 2152.97

isn't actually true.

Time: 2154.09

Our nervous system is tethered

Time: 2156.4

to the nervous systems of others.

Time: 2157.959

And that is true from the very earliest stages of our lives.

Time: 2161.56

And in this case, we're talking about how our templates

Time: 2164.34

for attachment in romantic relationships, how we find them,

Time: 2168.11

how we maintain them, and indeed how we break them

Time: 2170.4

and reform them is based on a template that was established

Time: 2174.33

through an entirely different set of priorities,

Time: 2176.61

which was how we feel safe and secure in novel environments,

Time: 2180.39

depending on whether or not our primary caregiver

Time: 2182.26

is there or not.

Time: 2183.27

Neuroimaging supports that.

Time: 2184.93

When I say neuroimaging, I mean, brain scan support that,

Time: 2187.46

measures of hormones in the body and brain support that,

Time: 2190.14

measures of neurochemicals support that.

Time: 2192.06

There's simply no way around this truth,

Time: 2194.29

that we have a set of roadmaps in our mind that are reused

Time: 2198.39

for entirely different purposes later in life.

Time: 2201.21

That is vitally important to understand.

Time: 2203.77

Because if one is successful in forming

Time: 2207.76

romantic attachments, maintaining them, et cetera, or not,

Time: 2212.86

does in fact reflect the earlier templates

Time: 2215.88

that you've created.

Time: 2217.13

But as I've mentioned before, the good news is

Time: 2219.54

that these templates can shift over time.

Time: 2221.66

And one of the more powerful ways to shift those templates

Time: 2224.21

over time is purely by the knowledge that they exist,

Time: 2227.99

and the understanding that those templates are malleable.

Time: 2231.92

They can change through the process of neuroplasticity.

Time: 2235.64

Again, neuroplasticity is just a rewiring

Time: 2237.59

of nerve connection.

Time: 2239.03

That is very much present in childhood,

Time: 2241.76

but also very much present in adulthood.

Time: 2243.95

So if you're somebody who you think falls into category

Time: 2247.21

one, two, three, or four, or you know somebody,

Time: 2249.89

or involved with somebody who falls into category

Time: 2251.96

one, two, three, and four,

Time: 2254.73

the mere knowledge of that can be very useful.

Time: 2257.15

But you might ask, well, what do I do with that knowledge?

Time: 2259.16

Well, fortunately, both psychologists and biologists

Time: 2261.41

have started to leverage that knowledge toward establishing

Time: 2264.83

better, more secure bonds in adult romantic relationships.

Time: 2268.32

And there's a book that has really tapped into this.

Time: 2271.27

I think it's the first book that has really addressed

Time: 2273.03

this head on.

Time: 2273.91

And that book comes from two Columbia professors.

Time: 2278.75

And title of the book is "Attached: The New Science of Adult

Time: 2283.83

Attachment and How It Can Help You Find and Keep Love."

Time: 2287.09

The authors of this book are Amir Levine and Rachel Heller.

Time: 2291.22

Again, both of them are skilled academics and researchers

Time: 2295.07

who have really taken the literature that I described

Time: 2297.7

on the strange situation task

Time: 2299.08

and mapped it to adult attachment styles.

Time: 2301.12

And also they've mapped out ways that they've observed

Time: 2304.92

in their clinical practice.

Time: 2306.28

And that is laboratory supported for, for instance,

Time: 2310.52

people that have an anxious-ambivalent

Time: 2312.58

or what we were calling insecure attachment style,

Time: 2314.85

or for people that fall

Time: 2316.47

into the disorganized or disoriented attachment style,

Time: 2319.83

how they can modify that attachment style in or out

Time: 2323.3

of relationships in order to establish what I think

Time: 2326.7

everybody wants, which is secure attachment.

Time: 2328.91

Why does everybody want that?

Time: 2330.13

Well, secure attachment allows people to be both

Time: 2332.93

in relationship, or if they choose to be on their own,

Time: 2336.19

or to be in relationship but physically separated

Time: 2338.77

from somebody else, or even emotionally separated

Time: 2340.77

from somebody else and maintain

Time: 2342.565

what we call a stable autonomic equilibrium,

Time: 2345.55

the ability to remain calm, clearheaded.

Time: 2348.32

You might not like what's happening,

Time: 2350.01

but you're able to navigate that with some sense of clarity

Time: 2352.7

and not excessive discomfort.

Time: 2354.77

So, is there a goal in all of this stuff

Time: 2357.28

about love, desire, and attachment?

Time: 2359.33

Indeed, there is.

Time: 2360.61

The secure attachment style is the one that leads

Time: 2364.5

to the most stable and predictable long-term relationships.

Time: 2368.14

Put differently, babies, toddlers, adolescents, teens,

Time: 2372.73

and young adults that have a secure attachment style

Time: 2375.35

are more likely to find and form long-term relationships

Time: 2378.52

than are people in the other categories.

Time: 2380.82

But people in other categories can learn and eventually

Time: 2384.56

migrate into the secure attachment style.

Time: 2387.69

And I think that book "Attached..."

Time: 2389.96

I have no affiliation to the authors or the book itself,

Time: 2392.58

I should just mention that.

Time: 2393.617

"Attached: The New Science of Adult Attachment

Time: 2395.82

and How It Can Help You Find and Keep Love,"

Time: 2397.53

really it sounds very pop psychology, yes,

Time: 2400.77

but it is really grounded

Time: 2402.04

in the research psychology literature.

Time: 2403.88

And there's also some interesting biology there.

Time: 2406.95

Another point to make about attachment styles

Time: 2409.81

is that it is possible.

Time: 2412.35

And some of you may be familiar with circumstances

Time: 2414.94

whereby people who are securely attached,

Time: 2418.12

either because they grew up in an environment

Time: 2419.93

where secure attachment was cultivated,

Time: 2422.11

or because they developed that on their own,

Time: 2424.61

can also migrate out of the securely attached category

Time: 2429.01

into insecurely attached, or into avoidant types

Time: 2432.7

of attachment styles as teens, or as young adults,

Time: 2436.33

or as adults at any age or any stage of life,

Time: 2440.76

by virtue of being with somebody who has a different,

Time: 2443.8

perhaps less adaptive attachment style, right?

Time: 2447.31

What this means is that if you have or you develop

Time: 2450.44

a secure attachment style, it's vitally important

Time: 2453.76

to protect that attachment style because it is possible

Time: 2456.69

to become anxiously attached,

Time: 2457.776

even if you grew up in a stable attachment framework.

Time: 2461.21

And again, this can happen at any stage.

Time: 2463.7

So if you're interested in attachment styles

Time: 2465.51

and how they influence adult romantic attachments,

Time: 2468.7

and certainly if you are a parent, I would encourage you

Time: 2472.05

to check out the book, "Attached."

Time: 2474.89

Again, it's quite good and I think that it offers a number

Time: 2477.46

of actionable tools to both form

Time: 2480.087

and hold onto secure attachment styles.

Time: 2482.5

So I mentioned that the neural circuits for child-parent

Time: 2485.82

or child-caregiver attachment are repurposed

Time: 2489.49

for romantic attachment later in life.

Time: 2492.46

But what are these neural circuits?

Time: 2494.83

What do they do?

Time: 2495.77

I mean, it's so attractive, if you will,

Time: 2499.15

to think about a brain area that controls love,

Time: 2501.7

or a brain area that controls desire,

Time: 2503.78

or a brain area that controls attachment,

Time: 2505.39

but it simply doesn't work that way.

Time: 2507.25

As I've talked about before on this podcast

Time: 2509.52

and I will say again and again because it will persist

Time: 2511.8

to be true long after I'm gone,

Time: 2513.9

is that no one brain area can give rise to anything

Time: 2517.91

as complex as desire, or love, or attachment.

Time: 2521.07

Instead, there are multiple brain areas that

Time: 2523.64

through their coordinated action create

Time: 2526.06

a song that we call desire, or a song that we call love,

Time: 2529.97

or a song that we call attachment.

Time: 2531.26

Not a literal song, although there are songs

Time: 2533.82

about desire, love, and attachment, of course, many songs,

Time: 2536.38

some good, some not so good.

Time: 2539.15

But rather different brain areas being active in different

Time: 2542.75

sequences and with different intensities can make us feel

Time: 2546.27

as if we are in the mode that we call desire,

Time: 2549.56

or in the mode of love, or in the mode of attachment.

Time: 2552.6

But beneath all of that is this element

Time: 2556.41

of autonomic arousal.

Time: 2557.92

And I want to focus on autonomic arousal

Time: 2560.45

just for a bit longer,

Time: 2561.5

because it really is one of the three core elements

Time: 2565.27

by which we form and maintain loving attachments,

Time: 2568.67

and by which we break loving attachments.

Time: 2572.36

The autonomic nervous system

Time: 2574.01

as the name suggests is automatic.

Time: 2577.1

In fact, that's what autonomic means.

Time: 2579.08

Now it's actually the case that we can control

Time: 2581.86

our autonomic nervous system to some degree or another.

Time: 2584.19

But the autonomic nervous system controls things like

Time: 2586.8

digestion, our breathing, whether or not we're conscious

Time: 2590.47

of that breathing or not.

Time: 2591.91

It controls things like how alert we are or sleepy we are.

Time: 2595.97

And the autonomic nervous system,

Time: 2598.12

as I just briefly described earlier,

Time: 2599.96

is really something that we come into the world with.

Time: 2602.73

It's hardwired, all the elements are there.

Time: 2605.47

But through interactions with our parent,

Time: 2608.46

either soothing interactions, or fun, playful interactions,

Time: 2612.78

or dare I say, scary interactions,

Time: 2615.52

our autonomic nervous system gets tuned up.

Time: 2618.74

Meaning, we each develop a tendency to either be more alert

Time: 2622.687

and anxious, or more calm, or a balance of alert and calm.

Time: 2627.07

Now, of course, this changes across each day

Time: 2629.06

and depending how tired we are late in the day.

Time: 2630.77

If we've been awake for a while,

Time: 2631.68

we tend to get sleepy early in the day.

Time: 2633.36

If we're very rested,

Time: 2634.2

we tend to wake up and feel very alert.

Time: 2636.29

So the way to think about the autonomic nervous system

Time: 2637.96

is it's kind of a seesaw.

Time: 2639.52

We go back and forth between being very alert,

Time: 2642.17

we can be alert and calm, or we can be very, very alert,

Time: 2644.8

we can be in a state of panic,

Time: 2646.26

we can be fast asleep so we can be extremely calm,

Time: 2649.25

or we can just be sleepy, semi calm but still also alert.

Time: 2654.9

So think about it like a seesaw.

Time: 2657.03

And that seesaw has a hinge,

Time: 2659.97

and that hinge defines how tight or loose that seesaw is,

Time: 2663.27

how readily it can tilt back and forth.

Time: 2666.14

Our autonomic tone is how tight that hinge is.

Time: 2670.16

And there are biological mechanisms to explain this,

Time: 2672.18

but here I just want to stay with the analogy

Time: 2673.8

of the seesaw for now.

Time: 2676.72

The interactions between child and caregiver early in life

Time: 2681.91

take the child and the caregiver

Time: 2684.29

from one end of the seesaw to the other.

Time: 2687.06

From being very alert in a state of play, for instance,

Time: 2690.17

to being nursed and being very soothed until we go to sleep.

Time: 2693.49

And of course, we each have a seesaw,

Time: 2695.98

the parent and the child has a seesaw,

Time: 2697.117

and they're interacting.

Time: 2698.2

What do I mean by that?

Time: 2699.106

Well, there are beautiful studies, and beautiful

Time: 2703

not in the sense that they focused on a pleasant topic,

Time: 2705.28

but beautiful because they were done so beautifully well,

Time: 2708.43

that looked at, for instance, the response of mothers

Time: 2712.52

and their physiologies and the response of children

Time: 2715.08

and their physiologies during the bombing of cities

Time: 2718.5

during World War II.

Time: 2719.56

So an unpleasant situation.

Time: 2721.54

But what was revealed during the course of these studies

Time: 2725.14

was that if the mothers were very stressed

Time: 2728.75

during an onslaught of bombing of the city,

Time: 2731.58

the children's physiologies tended to be stressed also,

Time: 2734.93

and persisted in being stressed long after that stressful

Time: 2738.81

episode was done.

Time: 2741.6

They actually followed that these children

Time: 2743.69

well out from many decades afterwards.

Time: 2747.4

Conversely, if the parent, and in this case, again,

Time: 2750.85

it was mothers that were explored in these studies,

Time: 2753.73

had turned this whole business of going

Time: 2755.81

into the bomb shelters into somewhat of a game, right?

Time: 2759.13

Taking it seriously, but essentially telling the children,

Time: 2761.627

"Okay, it's time to go,"

Time: 2762.46

but not expressing much stress or distress,

Time: 2765.86

the children also didn't develop much stress,

Time: 2767.156

or distress, or trauma.

Time: 2769.4

Now there were exceptions to this, of course,

Time: 2770.98

but in general, that was the rule

Time: 2772.162

that the autonomic nervous systems of children tend to mimic

Time: 2775.62

the autonomic nervous systems of the primary caregiver.

Time: 2778.8

And the mechanisms by which this occurs has been explored.

Time: 2782.63

And again I just refer to the beautiful work of Allan Schore

Time: 2785.62

at University of California, Los Angeles.

Time: 2788.14

And again, his name is Schore, spelled S-C-H-O-R-E.

Time: 2792.387

I'm looking down briefly at the floor here

Time: 2793.94

because I'll just reach for the book.

Time: 2796.63

He has a wonderful book called "Right Brain Psychotherapy."

Time: 2799.73

It's a little bit technical,

Time: 2800.76

but if you're interested in some of the studies,

Time: 2803.21

this book "Right Brain Psychotherapy" details how everything

Time: 2806.29

from nursing of children to playtime behavior,

Time: 2810.32

to strange situation type task behavior that we talked

Time: 2813.26

about before which of course occurs when children

Time: 2815.31

get dropped off at daycare, or nursery school,

Time: 2817.177

or with babysitters, et cetera.

Time: 2819.6

And indeed all types of caregiver-child interactions

Time: 2824.08

tune up that autonomic nervous system,

Time: 2826.91

so that the child ends up with an autonomic nervous system

Time: 2830.22

that either tends to lean more towards alert and anxious,

Time: 2834.72

or can be very alert but calm,

Time: 2836.58

or can be very calm and hard to budge.

Time: 2838.44

Again, it's the tightness of that hinge

Time: 2840.82

that really underlies these attachment styles

Time: 2843.77

that we were talking about earlier,

Time: 2845.27

and not on this episode of the "Huberman Lab Podcast,"

Time: 2848.03

but on many other previous previous episodes,

Time: 2849.62

such as the master stress episode,

Time: 2851.73

or some of the optimized health episodes.

Time: 2854.67

You can find these if you want at hubermanlab.com.

Time: 2856.97

A lot of the tools and techniques that are recommended there

Time: 2860.24

have to do with readjusting the autonomic nervous system

Time: 2863.89

in deliberate ways as an adult.

Time: 2865.265

Again, I won't go into the specific tools,

Time: 2868.61

but for instance, the physiological sigh.

Time: 2872.66

This tool that I've talked about extensively of two inhales

Time: 2876.28

through the nose, as deeply as you can on the first one,

Time: 2878.89

sneaking in a little bit more air on the second one,

Time: 2880.17

and then a long exhale through the mouth

Time: 2882.91

is a way of adjusting that autonomic seesaw.

Time: 2885.68

It tends to make us more calm.

Time: 2887.34

It activates what we call the parasympathetic arm

Time: 2890.47

of the autonomic nervous system, which is just fancy nerd

Time: 2892.97

speak for it's a quick way to calm yourself down, right?

Time: 2896.29

Things like ice baths, or cold showers, or cold immersion,

Time: 2900.68

or deliberate hyperventilation by contrast,

Time: 2904.61

or ways in which we can deliberately increase the level

Time: 2908.28

of our so-called sympathetic arm of our autonomic nervous

Time: 2910.967

system to make ourselves more alert.

Time: 2912.87

Why would you want to do that?

Time: 2913.91

Well, you can do that to be more alert,

Time: 2915.84

to be more awake, if you like,

Time: 2917.28

or as a form of self-induced stress inoculation

Time: 2920.5

to be able to tolerate higher levels of adrenaline

Time: 2923.77

by making it a practice that you self-direct.

Time: 2927.39

The reason those tools are out there is because many of us,

Time: 2931.51

for whatever reason, we don't have to blame anyone,

Time: 2934.38

but because of our childhood templates,

Time: 2936.88

because of things that happened and didn't happen

Time: 2938.77

in terms of our interactions with caregivers,

Time: 2940.76

have autonomic nervous systems that are tilted to one side

Time: 2943.37

or the other more than we would like.

Time: 2945.4

Or in which the hinge that I'm talking about in this analogy

Time: 2950.32

is too loose or that is too tight.

Time: 2953.03

And we're stuck in a mode of anxiousness,

Time: 2955.01

or stuck in a mode of lack of energy.

Time: 2957.18

That's what those tools are really about.

Time: 2959.56

But at a deeper level, the autonomic nervous system

Time: 2964.66

is really the system that governs how we will react

Time: 2967.9

in response to a romantic partner being present or leaving.

Time: 2973.41

And I don't necessarily mean leaving the relationship

Time: 2975.68

entirely, although it could mean that, right?

Time: 2977.79

We know people, I'm sure you know people that upon the end

Time: 2981.21

of a relationship that they wanted very much,

Time: 2983.47

are absolutely crushed.

Time: 2984.99

And actually in researching this episode there,

Time: 2987.242

I discovered there's an extensive literature finding

Time: 2990.22

that the feelings that one has after a breakup are very much

Time: 2994.82

like a clinical depression in many cases.

Time: 2997.36

But there are individuals that can look at a breakup

Time: 2999.84

as a transient event that they don't interpret

Time: 3003.33

as going to mean so much for all aspects of their life

Time: 3007.05

or reshaping their view of themselves, why?

Time: 3010.24

Well, we have different levels of autonomic function.

Time: 3014.16

And depending on where our seesaw is, if you will,

Time: 3017.8

some of us become extremely distraught and can't recalibrate

Time: 3021.35

ourselves, can't adjust ourselves down from stress to calm,

Time: 3025.39

or can't take ourselves from exhausted to more alert

Time: 3028.74

if we need to do that on our own.

Time: 3030.46

And so that's why tools for doing that exist.

Time: 3032.84

But attachment itself is about

Time: 3037.21

where our autonomic nervous system resides.

Time: 3039.73

So if I were to offer a set of tools around these topics

Time: 3042.81

of desire, love and attachment, I would say, first of all,

Time: 3045.55

you might want to think about whether or not you fall

Time: 3047.15

into the secure, insecure, or other attachment styles.

Time: 3051.38

Second, I think it is vitally important for all of us,

Time: 3055.38

but certainly for people that are in relationships

Time: 3058

or seeking relationships to be able to at least have some

Time: 3061.89

recognition of where our autonomic nervous system tends

Time: 3065.85

to reside both in terms of when we are with somebody

Time: 3068.94

and when they leave.

Time: 3070.43

When we are apart for long periods of time,

Time: 3072.35

can we calm ourselves? Can we self-soothe?

Time: 3074.9

Or are we very much dependent on the presence of another

Time: 3077.99

in order to feel soothed?

Time: 3079.51

Now, I absolutely want to emphasize

Time: 3081.96

that there is nothing wrong.

Time: 3083.39

In fact, there's everything right with feeling great

Time: 3086.18

in the presence of somebody else.

Time: 3087.58

That is actually a hallmark

Time: 3088.893

of strong and quality attachments.

Time: 3092.1

These days, we hear the term codependent a lot.

Time: 3094.9

This was a, I believe, the term was first coined

Time: 3097.19

by Pia Mellody.

Time: 3098.26

And it actually does occupy an important role in the world

Time: 3102.98

of trauma, trauma healing, so-called trauma bonding,

Time: 3106.03

topics of another episode.

Time: 3107.21

Actually, did an episode on fear and trauma.

Time: 3108.92

And we will do one all about trauma bonding

Time: 3110.67

with an expert at some point in the future.

Time: 3112.94

But codependence and codependency, the term can sometimes

Time: 3118.06

be misinterpreted as any dependence on another isn't good.

Time: 3123.55

Interdependence, a healthy interdependence,

Time: 3125.97

of course is good.

Time: 3127.09

It is the hallmark of healthy child-parent relations,

Time: 3129.75

sibling relations and romantic relations.

Time: 3132.53

But a key element of healthy interdependence is that,

Time: 3136.51

yes, our autonomic nervous system is adjusted

Time: 3139.21

by the presence of another, but that also that we can adjust

Time: 3142.5

our own autonomic nervous system,

Time: 3144.06

even in the absence of that person.

Time: 3146.41

That if the person goes away temporarily or permanently,

Time: 3150.17

that we can still regulate our own autonomic nervous system,

Time: 3153.63

both from states of stress to states of calm,

Time: 3155.95

both from states of exhaustion to states of more alertness.

Time: 3160.35

And of course, we all need sleep

Time: 3161.66

to go from exhaustion to alertness.

Time: 3163.85

But what I'm referring to here is the ability to regulate

Time: 3166.88

when distraught, or regulate when fatigued,

Time: 3171.54

or feeling depressed.

Time: 3172.87

And that is and is all about the autonomic nervous system.

Time: 3177.03

So as we talk about attachment styles,

Time: 3178.72

and we talk about infant and toddler

Time: 3180.91

and adult attachment styles,

Time: 3181.843

what we are really talking about is a complex set

Time: 3184.79

of neural circuitries.

Time: 3185.8

And one of those neural circuitries, which is absolutely

Time: 3188.61

crucial is this autonomic nervous system.

Time: 3191.07

So if the autonomic nervous system is one key component

Time: 3194.82

of desire, love and attachment, what are the other two?

Time: 3198.88

And what I'm going to tell you next is largely the pioneering

Time: 3202.64

work of Helen Fisher, who is really an anthropologist,

Time: 3206.69

who's become a bit of a neuroscientist and has collaborated

Time: 3209.65

with neuroscientists to establish brain areas

Time: 3212.72

and neural circuits that are associated

Time: 3214.33

with different aspects of attachment, love and desire.

Time: 3217.02

I think the first really high quality study

Time: 3219.69

of neural circuits associated with these themes

Time: 3222.74

was a paper published in 2005 in a very fine anatomical

Time: 3227.51

journal, perhaps the best neural anatomical journal,

Time: 3230.47

which is the Journal of Comparative Neurology.

Time: 3233.24

The Journal of Comparative Neurology has been around

Time: 3235.24

for more than 100 years and is considered the archival

Time: 3239.34

location for placing really high quality anatomy.

Time: 3242.79

They have tremendously high standards.

Time: 3244.61

And the study that I'm referring to is entitled,

Time: 3247.277

"Romantic Love: An fMRI,"

Time: 3249.5

meaning functional magnetic resonance imaging,

Time: 3251.867

"Study of a Neural Mechanism for Mate Choice."

Time: 3254.9

And Dr. Fisher is a author on this paper

Time: 3259.13

as is Arthur Aron, and Lucy Brown.

Time: 3261.99

So all very fine researchers.

Time: 3264.01

And this study as well as several other studies

Time: 3267.58

using magnetic resonance imaging,

Time: 3270.68

things like EEG, neuro anatomical tracing, et cetera,

Time: 3273.76

have identified a large number of brain areas

Time: 3275.69

that are associated with different aspects

Time: 3277.14

of desire, love, and attachment.

Time: 3278.254

And I'll just throw out a few names of those brain areas

Time: 3280.67

and what they control.

Time: 3281.63

And then I'll tell you how those anchor

Time: 3283.9

to the other two categories of neural circuits

Time: 3286.43

essential for desire, love, and attachment.

Time: 3288.57

So not surprisingly the dopamine system in the brain

Time: 3293.38

is associated with desire, love, and attachment,

Time: 3295.92

and mainly with desire, although to some extent, love.

Time: 3298.76

Dopamine is a neurochemical sometimes

Time: 3300.47

associated with reward.

Time: 3301.669

But as some of you have heard me say before,

Time: 3305.16

it is mainly a molecule of motivation, craving and pursuit.

Time: 3309.66

And that motivation, craving and pursuit that relates

Time: 3312.48

to dopamine is not unique to attachment, or love, or sex,

Time: 3315.95

or mating, et cetera.

Time: 3317.07

It is a universal generic currency in the brain

Time: 3320.87

for pursuing something.

Time: 3322.34

Food when you're hungry, a mate when you want one,

Time: 3325.35

to mate when you want to, warmth when you're cold,

Time: 3329.01

et cetera, et cetera, okay?

Time: 3330.26

So it's not for one specific purpose.

Time: 3331.602

But the brain areas associated with dopamine involve,

Time: 3336.01

for instance, the ventral tegmental area,

Time: 3338.57

the substantia nigra, areas of that sort, the basal ganglia.

Time: 3342.91

You don't need to know these names,

Time: 3344.06

just understand that these are networks of neurons

Time: 3346.58

that tend to have put the person, you into a state

Time: 3349.95

of forward action and pursuit and craving and motivation.

Time: 3354.01

They are not about being quiescent, relaxed, et cetera.

Time: 3357.18

The neural circuits for quiescence and relaxation

Time: 3361.78

are most associated with love and attachment,

Time: 3364.76

not surprisingly, and they're the neurochemical

Time: 3367.17

serotonin, and to some extent, oxytocin

Time: 3369.83

are the predominant neurochemicals involved.

Time: 3372.31

And those are released from brain areas,

Time: 3373.538

such as the raphe nucleus in the back of the brain.

Time: 3376.51

You may have heard that the majority of serotonin

Time: 3378.71

in your body is made in your gut, and indeed, that's true,

Time: 3382.11

but I hate to break it to you, the serotonin in your gut

Time: 3385.64

is not responsible for your feelings of love and attachment,

Time: 3388.79

at least not to a high degree.

Time: 3390.15

That's mainly going to be the reflection of neurons

Time: 3392.81

in your brain that make serotonin.

Time: 3395.16

And there are other areas of the brain that make serotonin

Time: 3397.7

as well and oxytocin as well,

Time: 3399.99

but they tend to be associated with the warmth and calm,

Time: 3403.22

and the soothing that we feel in the presence of another.

Time: 3406.58

And again, these are not strictly divided circuits.

Time: 3409.95

We can have dopamine and serotonin present in our brain

Time: 3412.087

and body at the same time to equal or different degrees.

Time: 3416.05

And we return in a little bit to what happens when levels

Time: 3419.07

of dopamine are very high and levels of serotonin are low

Time: 3421.59

and vice versa and so on,

Time: 3422.842

including in states of neurochemically modified states,

Time: 3428.38

as it were in, when we talk about things like MDMA,

Time: 3431.69

so-called ecstasy.

Time: 3433.48

But in the meantime, I want to just discuss

Time: 3436.95

the two neural circuits that use dopamine,

Time: 3440.53

that use serotonin and oxytocin,

Time: 3442.78

and that collaborate with the autonomic nervous system

Time: 3446.13

to drive what we call desire, love and attachment.

Time: 3449.35

And the three circuits are autonomic nervous system,

Time: 3453.77

we talked about that one.

Time: 3454.947

Then there's the nervous system components

Time: 3457.92

or the neural circuits for empathy, for being able to see

Time: 3462.4

and respond to indeed match the emotional tone

Time: 3466.74

or the autonomic tone of another.

Time: 3469.5

And then there's the third category,

Time: 3471.23

and this might surprise some of you,

Time: 3472.35

it certainly surprised me,

Time: 3473.8

but the data point to the fact that the third neural circuit

Time: 3476.89

that's very important for establishing bonds

Time: 3479.86

is one associated with positive delusions.

Time: 3482.78

So given that the neural circuits for empathy

Time: 3484.092

are absolutely crucial for falling in love

Time: 3487.48

and maintaining stable attachments,

Time: 3490.33

I'd like to talk about those neural circuits

Time: 3491.407

and what they are.

Time: 3493.29

Now, often when we hear empathy, we think,

Time: 3495.61

oh, empathy is really about listening to and really

Time: 3499.75

understanding what somebody else is feeling.

Time: 3501.63

Maybe even feeling what they're feeling.

Time: 3503.97

And indeed that's the case, but what do we mean by that?

Time: 3507.13

Right? What is it to feel what another feels?

Time: 3509.85

Well, what it means is that their seesaw

Time: 3513.18

is driving your seesaw,

Time: 3516.04

or your seesaw is somehow driving their seesaw.

Time: 3519.72

That there's a match in terms of the tilt of those seesaws.

Time: 3522.65

Now it doesn't have to be an exact match, right?

Time: 3524.65

If someone that you really he care about

Time: 3525.695

is very, very stressed, you could also become very stressed.

Time: 3530.34

That's a form of empathic matching,

Time: 3532.93

and there are indeed neural circuits for that.

Time: 3534.74

I'll describe what those neural circuits are in a moment.

Time: 3537.21

But sometimes the best role for us to take is actually one

Time: 3540.47

in which we are calm when the person that we care about

Time: 3543.86

or that we are romantically involved

Time: 3545.26

with is very, very anxious.

Time: 3547.08

And in a few minutes, I'll talk about how matching

Time: 3550.26

of emotional tone can be good or bad

Time: 3553.08

for the stability of a relationship,

Time: 3556.21

and complementarity of autonomic matching

Time: 3559.93

can be good or bad.

Time: 3561.37

In other words, sometimes it's beneficial for a relationship

Time: 3565.35

to go into the same state as the other.

Time: 3567.96

And sometimes it's more beneficial for us to not go

Time: 3570.81

into the same state as the other.

Time: 3573.7

But the important feature here is that when we talk about

Time: 3577.47

emotional matching or empathy or going into the same state

Time: 3580.33

or not going into the same state, what we are really talking

Time: 3582.68

about is whether or not the autonomic seesaw

Time: 3587.5

of one individual is driving the autonomic seesaw

Time: 3590.41

of the other individual.

Time: 3592.21

And this is a vital principle for how we fall in love

Time: 3596.52

and form attachments.

Time: 3597.56

And it's actually part of the desire

Time: 3599.44

and mating process itself.

Time: 3602.69

I would go so far as to say that one of the prerequisites

Time: 3607.78

to the propagation and expansion of our species

Time: 3611.89

is this notion of autonomic regulation.

Time: 3615.48

And to some extent, matching of autonomic nervous systems.

Time: 3618.76

Let me explain what I mean.

Time: 3620.93

Last I checked, the only way that new humans can be created

Time: 3624.7

is by way of sperm meeting egg, either in body or in dish,

Time: 3629.84

but sperm meets egg and then typically nine months later,

Time: 3633.98

we have a human baby.

Time: 3636.51

The process of bringing sperm to egg, right?

Time: 3640.54

Mating behavior, sex behavior in humans

Time: 3643.1

is one of autonomic regulation.

Time: 3646.69

And what I mean by that is the process of finding a mate,

Time: 3654.18

and in this case, I mean, actually someone to mate with,

Time: 3656.15

typically, well, scenarios vary, typically,

Time: 3659.08

is one of elevated autonomic arousal,

Time: 3663.12

meaning increased activation

Time: 3664.92

of the so-called sympathetic nervous system.

Time: 3667.09

This is related to dopamine release

Time: 3669.17

and it's related to epinephrine release.

Time: 3671.36

There has to be a pursuit or at least there has to be

Time: 3675.03

a mobilization to arrive in the same location

Time: 3678.36

whereby one can mate, right?

Time: 3680.38

That almost always is the case.

Time: 3683.68

However, the sexual arousal itself

Time: 3689.24

in both males and females

Time: 3691.83

is actually driven primarily by the parasympathetic arm

Time: 3696.9

of the autonomic nervous system.

Time: 3699.01

So while pursuit is one of alertness and sympathetic drive,

Time: 3704.18

as we say, again, sympathy is not really

Time: 3707.1

what's at play here,

Time: 3708.45

the word simple means together,

Time: 3710.02

and the activation of the autonomic nervous system

Time: 3712.53

toward more alert state

Time: 3713.82

is because of a sympathetic nervous system,

Time: 3715.343

meaning the co-activation together of many neurons

Time: 3718.55

in the brain and spinal cord.

Time: 3720.36

But then the actual physiological arousal state

Time: 3724.147

that we call sexual arousal is predominantly

Time: 3727.082

parasympathetically driven, okay?

Time: 3730.01

To be quite direct about this,

Time: 3731.43

if the sympathetic nervous system activation is too high,

Time: 3734.36

the sexual arousal response cannot happen in either males

Time: 3737.7

or in females, it's inhibited.

Time: 3740.16

However, the orgasm and ejaculation response,

Time: 3745.03

which if you think about it is required for sperm

Time: 3748.34

to meet egg is sympathetic driven.

Time: 3754.5

And then after orgasm and ejaculation,

Time: 3757.41

the parasympathetic nervousness system kicks back in,

Time: 3760.09

and there's a calming and relaxation.

Time: 3762.37

So the arc of mating involves sympathetic arousal, okay?

Time: 3768.76

Not sympathy, but alertness and arousal for pursuit,

Time: 3772.27

then a tilt of the seesaw at least to some degree

Time: 3775.23

for arousal of the sort that we typically hear

Time: 3778.05

of sexual arousal.

Time: 3780.13

Then at the point of orgasm and ejaculation is back

Time: 3784.65

to a sympathetic response.

Time: 3786.23

And how can I say that? How do I know that?

Time: 3788.7

The sympathetic nervous system meaning neurons

Time: 3791.08

within the sympathetic arm of the autonomic nervous system

Time: 3793.54

are what drive ejaculation and orgasm.

Time: 3797.43

And then afterward there's a return

Time: 3800.84

to increased parasympathetic activation.

Time: 3804.05

And we don't know for sure why that happens,

Time: 3806.64

but it's thought that in species that pair bond,

Time: 3810.77

humans generally pair bond, not always,

Time: 3813.32

there the return to more parasympathetic activation

Time: 3816.95

after orgasm and ejaculation is thought to increase

Time: 3819.87

the exchange of pheromonal orders, odors, excuse me,

Time: 3823.15

and to increase pillow talk and pair bonding

Time: 3825.66

of different kinds, okay?

Time: 3826.892

So that's the seesaw going back and forth is actually built

Time: 3832.54

into the process by which our entire species propagates.

Time: 3837.14

So in some ways, every human is required to navigate

Time: 3842.45

that process if they want their offspring to persist.

Time: 3845.32

And of course, nowadays there are technologies like

Time: 3847.82

in vitro fertilization, intrauterine insemination,

Time: 3852.62

there are a variety of ways that technology has allowed

Time: 3855.17

people to circumvent the actual physical mating process

Time: 3857.9

in the way that I described.

Time: 3859.18

But by and large, that's the way it it's done.

Time: 3861.81

And certainly that's the way it was done historically

Time: 3863.94

for if not tens of thousands

Time: 3865.73

or hundreds of thousands of years.

Time: 3867.79

That process is also what happens

Time: 3870.95

in all mammalian species that mate.

Time: 3873.79

Okay, so I'm overlooking an entire literature

Time: 3876.5

of animal studies that classic studies of this were done

Time: 3880.87

by two individuals, I'll just briefly mention them

Time: 3882.79

in case you want to look at the literature.

Time: 3885.01

There's a guy at the Rockefeller University named

Time: 3887.75

Donald Pfaff, P-F-A-F-F, who has done beautiful studies

Time: 3892.42

identifying the neural circuitry,

Time: 3893.85

what's called the lordosis response.

Time: 3896.07

Unlike in humans, the mating behavior of animals

Time: 3899.61

is rather stereotyped in terms of the positions

Time: 3901.84

that they occupy.

Time: 3902.75

And the lordosis response is a U-shaping

Time: 3905.49

or a bending up of the hindquarters of typically of rodents,

Time: 3908.9

but of other animals as well.

Time: 3910.43

The the male mounting is almost always from behind

Time: 3913.67

except in some species of primates.

Time: 3915.57

And that lordosis response is only going to occur

Time: 3919.55

during particular phases of the estrous cycle.

Time: 3922.36

The estrous cycle is the analog to the menstrual cycle,

Time: 3927.07

but it's not 28 days, it's four days or some other duration

Time: 3930.43

in other animals, depending on the animal.

Time: 3932.76

The lordosis response is strongly regulated by odors,

Time: 3936.05

by contact and is estrogen and testosterone controlled.

Time: 3940.17

And then the male portion of the mating sequence in animals,

Time: 3945.33

the mounting and thrusting and ejaculation as they're called

Time: 3947.79

or mounting, thrusting, intromission and ejaculation.

Time: 3950.01

Those are the four scientific categories

Time: 3951.78

that have been described,

Time: 3953.8

that's presence in rodents and also in dogs

Time: 3956.25

where it was primarily studied by Frank Beach,

Time: 3958.46

who was at University of California, Berkeley

Time: 3961.71

for a long time.

Time: 3962.58

And the entire literature around the neural circuitry

Time: 3966.02

for sexual mating behavior in animals largely stemmed

Time: 3970.82

from the work of Donald Pfaff and Frank Beach

Time: 3973.55

and their scientific offspring, not their actual offspring.

Time: 3977.36

You can look at that literature, if you like.

Time: 3979.65

There have been human neuroimaging studies of the process

Time: 3983.73

that I described a few minutes ago, believe it or not,

Time: 3986.38

of people in brain scanners, not necessarily mating

Time: 3990.58

with other people, but going through that arc of arousal,

Time: 3996.31

sympathetic activation during orgasm or ejaculation

Time: 3999

and then the post-ejaculatory or orgasmic phase

Time: 4002.46

in both men and women.

Time: 4003.71

And the brain areas associated with those

Time: 4006.63

have all been mapped out now.

Time: 4009.53

The spinal cord areas that control things like erection,

Time: 4014.48

vaginal lubrication, ejaculation and orgasm,

Time: 4017.12

those have also been mapped out.

Time: 4018.68

And this has all been explored from the perspective

Time: 4020.9

of both basic science, just to get an understanding

Time: 4023.48

of how our species has sexual interactions and reproduces,

Time: 4027.83

but also from the perspective of, for instance,

Time: 4030.41

trying to repair sexual function after spinal cord injury,

Time: 4034.81

which is a prominent concern for a lot of people,

Time: 4038.11

depending on where they have their injury,

Time: 4039.78

but in the number of people that have spinal cord injuries.

Time: 4042.87

So this is both vital biological and clinical data.

Time: 4048.9

The neural circuits for everything that I just described

Time: 4052.51

reside in the autonomic nervous system and are coordinated

Time: 4055.67

with the neural circuits that are associated with empathy.

Time: 4058.37

The neural circuits for empathy, again, there are many,

Time: 4061.44

but mainly two structures that you should know about,

Time: 4063.63

the prefrontal cortex,

Time: 4064.69

which is how we perceive things outside of us

Time: 4067.47

and make decisions on the basis of those perceptions,

Time: 4069.92

how we organize those decisions,

Time: 4072.08

and an area of the brain called the insula, I-N-S-U-L-A.

Time: 4075.53

The insula is a really interesting brain area that allows us

Time: 4080.3

to interocept, to pay attention to what's going on inside

Time: 4083.28

our body and to split some of our attention, to exterocept.

Time: 4087.2

And the mating dance, whether or not it's the dinner

Time: 4091.14

and date portion of the mating dance,

Time: 4092.388

or the actual physical dance part of the main dance,

Time: 4095.06

or actual mating and sexual behavior, kissing or otherwise,

Time: 4100.26

that is a coordinated activity of two bodies,

Time: 4105.04

typically it's two.

Time: 4106.1

I realize sometimes it's more, sometimes it's only one,

Time: 4108.32

but typically it's two bodies at least in the framework

Time: 4111.61

we're using here.

Time: 4113.29

That coordinated dance is one

Time: 4115.31

in which the autonomic nervous system of one individual

Time: 4119.37

in general is coordinating with the autonomic nervous system

Time: 4121.75

of the other individual.

Time: 4122.94

And the insula is essentially splitting one's attention

Time: 4127.43

between how we feel ourselves, how our body feels,

Time: 4132.2

what we're thinking with the thinking

Time: 4134.51

and the bodily sensations of the other.

Time: 4137.34

And that can be communicated, obviously, through words,

Time: 4140.38

it can be communicated through sounds,

Time: 4142.33

it can be communicated through touch,

Time: 4143.576

and it can be communicated through a number

Time: 4145.86

of more subtle cues like pupil size or whether or not,

Time: 4150.34

certainly in cases where we recognize the person

Time: 4152.39

and we know their responses, their autonomic responses

Time: 4155.64

under different conditions.

Time: 4156.473

We can assess, is the person comfortable?

Time: 4158.91

Are they uncomfortable?

Time: 4160.068

Are they more focused on me or on themselves?

Time: 4163.21

This is the coordinated silent dance that

Time: 4165.91

if we look out in neurobiological terms,

Time: 4168.63

we can really see is all about the autonomic nervous system,

Time: 4172.82

whether or not it's time to tip the seesaw to one side

Time: 4177.16

or the other, depending on whether or not the other person's

Time: 4179.65

seesaw is tilted higher or lower than the other.

Time: 4182.45

Okay, so we have the autonomic nervous system,

Time: 4184.26

and then we have this thing that we're calling empathy,

Time: 4186.72

which is really about autonomic matching.

Time: 4189.45

And again, the insula and the prefrontal cortex

Time: 4192.03

are neural circuits that are crucial for autonomic matching,

Time: 4195.74

because they allow us to say, what's out there,

Time: 4198.48

and do I want to match to it or not? Okay?

Time: 4201.48

And then the third category of neural circuit

Time: 4204.44

that Helen Fisher and others have found to be important

Time: 4206.92

for desire, love and attachment is the neural circuit

Time: 4210.16

associated with self-delusion.

Time: 4212.32

What do we mean by that?

Time: 4213.693

Well, first of all, self-delusion implies a kind of cynicism

Time: 4220.46

about love and attachment.

Time: 4221.67

And I think it was George Bernard Shaw that said,

Time: 4224.617

"Love is really about overestimating

Time: 4226.9

the differences between individuals."

Time: 4229.24

Actually, when I hear that, and as I say it,

Time: 4230.535

I really don't like that quote.

Time: 4232.87

I have no bone to pick with George Bernard Shaw,

Time: 4234.93

but what it suggests and I think what he meant was that,

Time: 4240.34

in love and attachment, we tend to put so much value

Time: 4243.85

in the other that we forget that many of the processes

Time: 4246.96

that are going on in our brain and body

Time: 4249.15

actually could be evoked by many other people too.

Time: 4251.76

But I think it somewhat overlooks the enormous power

Time: 4255.06

of attachment and the ways in which somebody's smell,

Time: 4258.4

somebody's voice, somebody's everything,

Time: 4261.86

or somebody's particular thing or things can really

Time: 4265.3

become so vital for our autonomic nervous system

Time: 4268.38

to feel soothed and to feel elated, et cetera.

Time: 4271.02

So I think that while the quote is accurate,

Time: 4275.68

in the one sense, I think it does overlook

Time: 4277.643

the neural circuits for attachment

Time: 4279.7

and just how deeply wired those can become for us.

Time: 4283.18

So I will balance that quote with an enormous number

Time: 4287.29

of other quotes that I won't mention, but that you can find

Time: 4290.23

out there that really point to how incredible the person is

Time: 4295.583

that one tends to be attached to that there's really

Time: 4298.1

only one or several people that could ever exist

Time: 4302.09

that could evoke those feelings from us.

Time: 4303.64

And of course you can read your Neruda poetry,

Time: 4307.47

and you can find these things all over the place,

Time: 4309.09

in music, and poetry, and writing.

Time: 4311.44

So for every cynical quote about these neural circuits

Time: 4315.04

being generic and could be activated by anybody,

Time: 4317.99

I think you'll find an ample number of opposing quotes

Time: 4322.6

that these neural circuits can only be activated

Time: 4324.93

by that special someone, or that particular person,

Time: 4327.82

or maybe in just a small set of those people.

Time: 4330.06

So what of delusion?

Time: 4331.475

Well, the work of Helen Fisher and others has really pointed

Time: 4336.78

to the fact that desire, love and attachment

Time: 4340.24

are three separate phases

Time: 4342.53

of what we call romantic relationships.

Time: 4345.42

That typically, not always, but typically desire tends

Time: 4349.15

to come first or falls in the early phase.

Time: 4352.33

That the process of romantic/sexual interactions,

Time: 4356.9

it doesn't necessarily have to be sex itself,

Time: 4358.76

but certainly something that involves intimacy

Time: 4362.14

of some kind, right?

Time: 4364.18

And generally touch of some kind,

Time: 4366.94

eventually transitions into what we call love,

Time: 4369.28

which eventually transitions into what we call attachment.

Time: 4373.07

And I should just mention touch because touch

Time: 4376.57

is a fundamental aspect of this whole process.

Time: 4379.64

There's an article, a research article, I should say,

Time: 4383.34

the title of it is,

Time: 4384.173

"Relationship-specific Encoding of Social Touch

Time: 4386.64

in Somatosensory and Insular Cortices."

Time: 4390.1

Cortices being cortex.

Time: 4392.11

Cortex's plural, cortices.

Time: 4393.98

And again, there's our friend the insula.

Time: 4396.13

So this is a study that explored what brain areas

Time: 4398.93

and what body areas are activated by specific forms

Time: 4403.62

of attachment and social touch.

Time: 4405.78

And what they found not surprisingly is that the areas

Time: 4409.19

of the brain that are associated with touch,

Time: 4410.56

the somatosensory areas, but more interestingly,

Time: 4413.63

the insular cortex are strongly activated by touch.

Time: 4418.91

So touch in the amount of touching, and proximity,

Time: 4422.14

and skin contact, not surprisingly activates brain areas

Time: 4425.57

associated with somatosensory, touch,

Time: 4428.45

but also the insular cortex, which again,

Time: 4430.74

is this brain area that links the internal

Time: 4433.7

or feelings about what's going on inside us

Time: 4435.557

and at the surface of our skin with events external.

Time: 4438.91

And they found activation of a number of brain areas,

Time: 4442.03

the amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex, and so on and so on.

Time: 4445.66

That's not as essential as just understanding

Time: 4447.47

that the insular is the place in which we start to take

Time: 4451.25

our experience of our internal landscape,

Time: 4453.79

attach that to our perceptions of the external landscape,

Time: 4456.75

and then assign that a value or assign that

Time: 4460.25

some sort of interpretation.

Time: 4461.89

And positive delusion is predictive of long-term attachment.

Time: 4468.27

What do we mean by positive delusion?

Time: 4469.64

Positive delusion is the contradiction

Time: 4472.87

of that George Bernard Shaw quote.

Time: 4474.526

It's the belief that only this person

Time: 4478.63

can make me feel this way.

Time: 4480.209

This other person holds the capacity to make me feel

Time: 4484.62

this way physically or emotionally or both.

Time: 4487.75

And so, as we move from desire to love to attachment,

Time: 4492.83

our brain circuitry is literally getting tuned up such that

Time: 4496.36

that individual that we happen to be attached to,

Time: 4499.13

again, here thinking about monogamous relationships,

Time: 4501.52

but I guess for non-monogamous relationship individuals

Time: 4506.84

is and are the way in which our autonomic nervous system

Time: 4510.31

can be regulated.

Time: 4511.17

They actually get access to our control panel.

Time: 4514.24

So it's our autonomic nervous system empathy

Time: 4516.091

and this positive delusion.

Time: 4519.57

Now positive delusion is critical.

Time: 4521.142

If you look at the stability of relationships over time,

Time: 4525.87

something that's been extensively studied

Time: 4527.85

mainly by psychologists, but now also by neurobiologists,

Time: 4531.64

what you find is that there are some key features

Time: 4536.3

of interactions between individuals that predict

Time: 4539.502

that a relationship will last.

Time: 4541.56

And those are many,

Time: 4543.08

but mainly fall under this category of positive delusions.

Time: 4546.46

I'll return to those in what those exactly look like.

Time: 4548.98

But there are also just a handful of things that predict

Time: 4552.9

that a relationship will fail over time.

Time: 4555.35

This is largely the work of the Gottman's,

Time: 4558.21

is actually a husband and wife team

Time: 4559.79

up at the University of Washington in Seattle.

Time: 4563.25

The Gottman's have, run a laboratory

Time: 4566.24

in the department of psychology for a long time.

Time: 4568.05

They've also done a lot of public facing

Time: 4569.67

work around relationships.

Time: 4571.62

And they've talked about the various aspects

Time: 4575.63

of relationships and interactions between people

Time: 4577.91

that predict either staying together or breaking up.

Time: 4580.31

So much so that they've established a method

Time: 4584.08

by which they can look at video interactions between couples

Time: 4587.95

and with very high degree of certainty predict

Time: 4590.98

whether or not those couples will stay together

Time: 4592.72

or break up over time.

Time: 4594.02

And they've identified what they call

Time: 4595.51

the four horsemen of relationships.

Time: 4597.95

These are things that essentially,

Time: 4600.581

almost always predict that a couple will break up.

Time: 4605.18

And I think the current number on this is that Gottman

Time: 4609.47

can predict divorce with 94% accuracy,

Time: 4613.11

which if you think about is pretty remarkable.

Time: 4615.14

So even though these are purely psychological studies,

Time: 4617.94

I'm not aware of any analysis of underlying physiology,

Time: 4621.53

there are some things that they can observe between couples

Time: 4625.32

that can lead them to predict whether or not a couple

Time: 4628.02

will stay together or break up with 94% accuracy.

Time: 4631.42

So what are those things?

Time: 4632.8

Those four behaviors, what they call the Four Horsemen

Time: 4635.37

of the Apocalypse [laughs] for relationships

Time: 4639.791

are one, criticism, two, defensiveness, three, stonewalling,

Time: 4645.92

and four, contempt, with contempt being the most powerful

Time: 4650.57

predictor of breaking up.

Time: 4654.1

Criticism, of course, does not mean that there's no place

Time: 4658.45

for criticism in stable relationships.

Time: 4660.92

Of course, there is.

Time: 4662.37

Has to do with how frequent and how intensely

Time: 4664.67

that criticism is voiced.

Time: 4666.15

Defensiveness, of course, is defensiveness.

Time: 4669.4

We know as the lack of ability to hear another

Time: 4674.28

or to adopt their stance.

Time: 4675.56

So lack of empathy,

Time: 4677.02

I think is one way to interpret defensiveness.

Time: 4680.43

Stonewalling, which is actually another form

Time: 4683.26

of lack of empathy.

Time: 4684.14

It's a turning off of this neural circuit

Time: 4686.22

that's so critical for desire, love and attachment.

Time: 4689.23

The stonewalling essentially means the emotional response

Time: 4693.61

or the request of another is completely cut off.

Time: 4695.54

So it's, I don't think there's been brain imaging of this,

Time: 4698.84

but I think, we can reasonably imagine that it involves

Time: 4701.725

untethering your insular response from the other

Time: 4705.96

and what they're dealing with,

Time: 4706.95

and focusing your insular response,

Time: 4709.764

[giggles] no pun intended, on your own internal state

Time: 4713.31

or perhaps the state of someone else entirely.

Time: 4716.02

I'll talk about infidelity in a moment.

Time: 4717.43

And then contempt.

Time: 4718.6

And contempt has actually been referred

Time: 4720.149

to as the sulfuric acid of relationship.

Time: 4723.3

I didn't say that, but Gottman and colleagues have.

Time: 4725.87

That it is such a powerful predictor

Time: 4728.48

of divorce and breakups in the future.

Time: 4731.81

And contempt, of course, by definition is the feeling

Time: 4735.35

that a person or thing is beneath consideration,

Time: 4737.87

worthlessness or deserving scorn.

Time: 4740.85

And apparently they can identify this in the videos

Time: 4743.52

of couples having discussions and interacting

Time: 4746.17

by very elaborate eye rolls, by expressions of anger

Time: 4751.79

in one individual when their partner is actually expressing

Time: 4754.75

enthusiasm or excitement about something.

Time: 4756.99

It's the, oh yeah, you would say that,

Time: 4760.04

or deep seated resentment toward the other,

Time: 4763.5

so much so that it's apparent that one actively dislikes

Time: 4768.28

the other partner.

Time: 4770.37

So contempt, disregard for something that should be taken

Time: 4773.99

into account is the other way to think about it.

Time: 4776.21

That runs counter to all of the neural circuits,

Time: 4779.44

all three of the neural circuits

Time: 4780.73

that we talked about before.

Time: 4781.75

It certainly, it is the antithesis of empathy.

Time: 4785.34

It is anything but a positive delusion.

Time: 4787.55

It's really looking at the other individual,

Time: 4789.55

either accurately or inaccurately

Time: 4791.49

as somebody that you despise.

Time: 4794.03

And then it is an absolute inversion of the autonomic

Time: 4798.39

seesaw matching that I was talking about before.

Time: 4800.84

It's a dissociating of your seesaw from their seesaw.

Time: 4803.61

They're very excited about something,

Time: 4805.15

you're unexcited by it.

Time: 4806.94

In fact, it's an inversion of their seesaw

Time: 4809.16

where they're excited, you're down.

Time: 4812.75

They're down, you are up, okay?

Time: 4814.93

So it's basically an inversion of all of the neural circuits

Time: 4819.64

that Helen Fisher and others have identified as critical

Time: 4822.74

for desire, love, and attachment.

Time: 4824.33

And therefore it's not surprising that it is so strongly

Time: 4828.3

predictive of breakups,

Time: 4829.51

and in the case of married couples, of divorce.

Time: 4832.13

For those of you that are interested in the work

Time: 4833.541

of the Gottman's and similar work,

Time: 4837.04

they've written several popular books.

Time: 4839.22

They're fairly easy to find.

Time: 4840.86

We can link to one of those in the caption.

Time: 4844.02

But they've also developed

Time: 4845.01

some quite interesting online resources

Time: 4848.5

in their so-called Love Lab.

Time: 4850.5

I guess it's fortunate that they didn't call it the Hate Lab

Time: 4853.59

or the Breakup Lab,

Time: 4854.43

'cause they focused a lot on what predicts breakups.

Time: 4856.94

But they've also written extensively and researched

Time: 4860.32

extensively in peer-reviewed studies

Time: 4863.61

what makes people find appropriate partners for them,

Time: 4868.19

and to maintain those partnerships over time.

Time: 4872.47

So you can go, you can search for Love Lab,

Time: 4875.08

University of Washington, Gottman,

Time: 4877.1

or any number of their various books.

Time: 4879.37

I think you'll find some useful resources there.

Time: 4884.17

I want to shift back to the work of Helen Fisher.

Time: 4888.14

She's made some very interesting statements

Time: 4891.15

and some very interesting observations

Time: 4893.33

that at least to my mind map very well

Time: 4895.84

onto the knowledge of neural circuitry both in humans

Time: 4899.47

and in non-human primates and in other species.

Time: 4901.63

I realize that she's the only name in the game.

Time: 4905.93

But she's made some observations that I think are very,

Time: 4909.8

as we say, parsimonious, meaning they allow us to organize

Time: 4912.62

a lot of this stuff into some distinct frameworks.

Time: 4915.71

Now, she's also done some really beautiful studies

Time: 4918.472

that involve data from millions or even tens of millions

Time: 4922.32

of individuals on dating sites.

Time: 4924.02

So I'm going to share those with you in a moment.

Time: 4925.34

But before I do that, I just want to paraphrase Dr. Fisher,

Time: 4931.65

who said that sex drive or desire that the pursuit

Time: 4936.81

of someone to mate with, meaning to mate, the verb,

Time: 4939.92

not necessarily to find a mate, may be,

Time: 4945.37

she didn't say definitively, but may be a way to forage

Time: 4948.01

for potential love partners that the arc of this

Time: 4951.57

whole business is really the order that we're describing it,

Time: 4954.58

that it's desire, then love, and then attachment.

Time: 4959.41

And that oftentimes people can get confused.

Time: 4963.12

You may know some of these people,

Time: 4964.67

you may be one of these individuals who might confuse desire

Time: 4968.4

for attachment or might confuse love for attachment,

Time: 4973.83

but that there's a sequence of recruitment

Time: 4976.78

of these neural circuits that's established first

Time: 4979.64

from the pursuit of someone to mate with.

Time: 4982.44

And she's placed us in the context of more modern dating

Time: 4986.83

themes where depending on culture, people might explore

Time: 4992.64

several, maybe many, many individuals before,

Time: 4995.99

quote, unquote, settling down with somebody,

Time: 4998.77

at least for some period of time.

Time: 5001.1

I think that's an interesting framework

Time: 5003.48

because it circumvents a lot of the, frankly, unanswerable

Time: 5008.65

questions about whether or not humans were meant

Time: 5012.33

to be monogamous or whether or not they weren't.

Time: 5014.22

Those are conversations that hold cultural context,

Time: 5017.11

that hold all sorts of context,

Time: 5019.77

that really can't be addressed in a laboratory setting.

Time: 5022.75

But this idea that sex drive is a way to forage

Time: 5026.76

for potential love partners and that love is a litmus test

Time: 5031.3

for whether or not longer term or deeper attachments

Time: 5034.64

can and will form, is one that at least makes sense to me.

Time: 5038.38

Later in the episode, we'll talk about this notion

Time: 5040.446

of sex drive and desire.

Time: 5042.77

I'll actually talk about some tools that have very strong

Time: 5046.95

data really to support them in terms of things that people

Time: 5050.36

can do or take to increase libido, both men and women,

Time: 5054.57

because there's actually quite good data on that now.

Time: 5057.35

But in the meantime, I want to talk about some of the work

Time: 5060.49

that Dr. Fisher has done in terms

Time: 5062.33

of categorizing people into...

Time: 5065.26

Again, we have four groups.

Time: 5067.16

These are distinct from the A, B, C, and D

Time: 5070.3

attachment styles described earlier.

Time: 5072.22

Although as I describe them,

Time: 5073.66

you might be able to map them somewhat onto those.

Time: 5076.4

And these four groups are groups that were defined

Time: 5081.51

through her studies of people that were, or are,

Time: 5084.96

I don't know if they were or if they are still on match.com,

Time: 5089.6

but a very extensive dataset.

Time: 5091.64

So again, millions if not tens of millions of individuals.

Time: 5095.16

The number I heard her quote,

Time: 5096.67

and forgive me if this is not accurate,

Time: 5099.27

is that in upwards of 40 million individuals,

Time: 5104.37

in terms of whether or not their neurochemical

Time: 5106.85

and hormone systems are tuned

Time: 5108.81

toward particular types of behaviors.

Time: 5112.4

What do I mean by that?

Time: 5113.33

Well, both men and women, males and females

Time: 5117.24

have both testosterone and estrogen.

Time: 5119.3

Typically, again, these are averages, but typically

Time: 5122.03

men have more testosterone than they do estrogen,

Time: 5124.68

and they have more testosterone than do women,

Time: 5126.92

and less estrogen then do women.

Time: 5128.65

Typically women have more estrogen

Time: 5130.66

than they do testosterone, again, averages.

Time: 5134.32

And they have less testosterone than men,

Time: 5138.27

more estrogen than men, and so on and so forth.

Time: 5140.9

But both hormone systems are active

Time: 5142.95

in both sets of individuals.

Time: 5146.45

And of course, all humans, as far as we know,

Time: 5150.31

manufacture both dopamine and serotonin.

Time: 5153.08

Dopamine, as I mentioned earlier, has a number of effects

Time: 5156.58

in the brain and body.

Time: 5157.413

But one of the primary effects is that it places us

Time: 5159.44

into states of motivation and pursuit for various things.

Time: 5163.4

There is somewhat close relationship between the dopamine

Time: 5167.66

system and the testosterone system in the hypothalamus,

Time: 5171.8

this brain area above the roof of your mouth.

Time: 5173.36

And the pituitary gland, which is responsible

Time: 5175.61

for making hormones that make the ovaries and or testes,

Time: 5181.5

secrete testosterone or estrogen.

Time: 5183.194

So there's a lot of signaling that occurs such that dopamine

Time: 5187.567

and testosterone tend to operate as close cousins

Time: 5191.66

in a system of pursuit.

Time: 5193.4

And conversely, the serotonin system tends to on average

Time: 5197.35

collaborate with the estrogen system to impart certain

Time: 5201.25

physiological functions and behaviors.

Time: 5203.46

So these aren't hard and fast, or I guess better stated,

Time: 5208.41

these aren't strict black and white categorizations,

Time: 5211.45

but I think those general themes hold

Time: 5213.51

when you look at the animal and human data.

Time: 5216.52

So Dr. Fisher has taken some liberties, but I think they are

Time: 5221.99

what I would call logically, and scientifically,

Time: 5224.21

and neurobiologically grounded liberties

Time: 5226.79

in classifying individuals who are on these dating sites

Time: 5230.57

according to the types of things they report

Time: 5233.01

about themselves and the type of people they tend

Time: 5235.5

to match with on these dating sites,

Time: 5239.11

and created these four categories.

Time: 5242.62

The four categories are, she calls one,

Time: 5245.068

the dopamine category.

Time: 5247.77

So these are people who would have high dopamine.

Time: 5250.47

And again, that's just a name.

Time: 5254

It doesn't mean they have low anything else,

Time: 5256.81

but they are high on the dopamine scale.

Time: 5259.49

People who rate high on the dopamine scale tend to be

Time: 5263.44

what the scientists and psychologists call

Time: 5265.19

high sensation seeking, novel seeking.

Time: 5267.09

They like new things, they like spontaneity,

Time: 5269.84

they tend to be very adventurous.

Time: 5271.49

And I think that's largely true.

Time: 5272.94

If you look at the conditions

Time: 5275.1

where dopamine is super physiological,

Time: 5279.5

it's elevated beyond abnormal levels.

Time: 5282.71

Things like mania or when people take certain drugs

Time: 5286.5

of abuse, like cocaine or amphetamine

Time: 5288.69

that really raise dopamine levels up very, very high.

Time: 5292.55

For some period of time, they do tend to increase

Time: 5295.63

energy motivation and novelty seeking.

Time: 5298.09

And of course, drugs like amphetamine and cocaine

Time: 5300.1

have all sorts of deleterious effects

Time: 5302.16

that I don't need to go into here,

Time: 5303.21

but it's worth pointing out.

Time: 5304.9

But they don't tend to make people calm and relaxed

Time: 5307.3

and seek soothing interactions.

Time: 5310.73

Conversely, the group that Dr. Fisher calls

Time: 5314.16

the serotonin group tend to be more grounded

Time: 5320.24

in soothing activities, quiescent type activities.

Time: 5323.43

They actually tend to be, on average,

Time: 5325.61

they tend to like rules and follow rules,

Time: 5327.79

they tend to be homebodies, this sort of thing.

Time: 5330.22

They're really, you can imagine them this stable types,

Time: 5333.69

but they really like stability.

Time: 5335.01

They're not really into spontaneity as much,

Time: 5337.32

again, averages.

Time: 5338.35

And then she created two other categories,

Time: 5341.42

the testosterone category of high testosterone.

Time: 5344.63

This again, could be males or females.

Time: 5347.64

And then the estrogen category,

Time: 5350.81

again, could be males or females.

Time: 5352.11

And she gave these different names

Time: 5353.917

that I won't go into here.

Time: 5355.73

You can look up her work online,

Time: 5357.02

but she names like the director, and the follower,

Time: 5360.48

and things like that.

Time: 5361.313

But I don't really want to use those as much as I want to stick

Time: 5363.04

to the biological terms.

Time: 5364.66

So we have dopamine, serotonin, testosterone, and estrogen.

Time: 5368.47

Now that might seem like an unfair overgeneralization,

Time: 5373.64

but what's interesting is not necessarily the name

Time: 5376.89

or the neurochemical system, right?

Time: 5378.65

Those could have just been called category one, two, three

Time: 5381.35

and four for all that matters here.

Time: 5385.32

What is interesting is seeing how those different groups

Time: 5389.56

of individuals that she absolutely can categorize

Time: 5393.29

based on their self-reported preferences

Time: 5397.12

about behaviors and certain kinds of interactions.

Time: 5399.96

How those groups tend to pair up with people

Time: 5403.08

in the same or opposite categories.

Time: 5405.41

So what her studies reveal is that people that fall

Time: 5408.51

into the high sensation seeking, novelty seeking,

Time: 5411.1

spontaneity category, the one that she calls

Time: 5413.43

the high dopamine category tend to pair up with, at least

Time: 5417.64

in the short term, tend to pair up with people

Time: 5421.19

who are also in that dopaminergic category.

Time: 5423.76

So these would be people that would spontaneously

Time: 5426.99

take a trip or explore something new or new restaurant.

Time: 5430.03

They tend to be creative and explorative types.

Time: 5434.49

So that group on average tends to date and mate,

Time: 5440.01

and potentially form long-term

Time: 5442.37

relationships within category.

Time: 5444.47

Again, averages.

Time: 5446.04

Individuals that she placed into the serotonin group

Time: 5449.93

or what she hypothesized would be a high serotonin group,

Time: 5452.96

again, they didn't measure serotonin,

Time: 5454.388

but people that tend to place value on stability, on rules,

Time: 5459.68

on certain forms of traditional organization

Time: 5463.84

at home and in relationships,

Time: 5465.59

those people also tended to pair up with select date,

Time: 5471.46

we presume mate with and form stable relationships

Time: 5474.72

with people in the same category.

Time: 5477.28

Now, individuals in the other two categories,

Time: 5479.78

the high testosterone group,

Time: 5481.56

and again, testosterone wasn't measured,

Time: 5483.52

but she called it the high testosterone group,

Time: 5485.57

but these are people that tend to be very directive,

Time: 5490.87

they tend to know what they want and are comfortable telling

Time: 5493.33

other people what they want.

Time: 5495.57

And from them, these are individuals that in her studies

Time: 5500.93

and in other studies tend to be a little bit challenging,

Time: 5504.24

meaning they not necessarily challenging to be around,

Time: 5507.23

but they tend to challenge other people,

Time: 5509.49

push them in order to expand their boundaries,

Time: 5512.77

either for sake of the relationship or just in general.

Time: 5515.89

And the people they tend to push are the people that

Time: 5517.73

they pair up with, which are the people

Time: 5519.81

in the estrogen category, what she called high estrogen.

Time: 5522.34

Again, they didn't measure estrogen,

Time: 5523.84

but the people in the estrogen category were the ones

Time: 5527.63

that described themselves and their choices in life

Time: 5530.9

and their preferences as being nurturing.

Time: 5533.52

They actually seem to like it when someone else

Time: 5536.07

is making the major decisions, not every decision.

Time: 5539.8

They certainly like to be heard, of course,

Time: 5542.16

in terms of their preferences.

Time: 5543.47

But that those two types, what she called the testosterone

Time: 5547.107

and the estrogen type tend to pair up.

Time: 5549.83

So why are these categorizations

Time: 5551.357

and these averages interesting to me,

Time: 5553.63

at least interesting enough to convey to you?

Time: 5556.3

The reason they're interesting to me is,

Time: 5558.94

again, not because of their names,

Time: 5560.49

these molecules were not measured in these individuals,

Time: 5563.27

but that they once again bring us to the themes

Time: 5566.53

that we addressed before,

Time: 5568.17

which are the autonomic nervous system and whether or not

Time: 5571.55

it tends to be shifted more towards alertness in action

Time: 5574.9

or more towards a stable equilibrium or more towards calm,

Time: 5579.63

and whether or not individuals are selecting for people

Time: 5584.53

who have autonomic nervous systems that are more or less

Time: 5587.43

like theirs before they even meet, right?

Time: 5590.78

So again, going back to this seesaw analogy,

Time: 5593.96

it's almost like people who have the flat seesaw,

Time: 5596.69

alert but calm, but not extremely alert,

Time: 5599.65

not extremely overly calm in situations but in the middle,

Time: 5605.44

seem to be seeking out people that are also

Time: 5607.67

at that autonomic equilibrium.

Time: 5611.37

People in the, what she called the dopamine category,

Time: 5613.77

which are really can just be described

Time: 5615.12

as high sensation seeking, novelty seeking,

Time: 5617.35

they seem to want to pair with one another.

Time: 5619.07

So there's a selection for similar,

Time: 5622.57

in two of the groups, autonomic tone.

Time: 5625.73

I find that very interesting because in that decision

Time: 5630.41

or that preference for similar autonomic tone,

Time: 5633.43

it essentially eliminates a lot of the requirement

Time: 5638.291

for figuring out how to match

Time: 5641.99

one's autonomic nervous system to another.

Time: 5643.344

They simply find someone with a similar tendency, okay?

Time: 5647.61

Whereas in the other two groups that she called

Time: 5650.6

testosterone and estrogen, the director type

Time: 5652.78

and the nurturing somewhat follower type,

Time: 5656.27

there's an establishment of balance,

Time: 5659.02

but not in between two who individuals as a match,

Time: 5664.05

but rather on the whole in the relationship.

Time: 5666.95

One person is driving the novelty seeking

Time: 5669.46

in the course of decisions and actions,

Time: 5671

and the other person is essentially agreeing to those.

Time: 5673.85

Now, assuming that those decisions are good for both people.

Time: 5677.29

And I emphasize good for both people,

Time: 5679.25

because one of the themes that Dr. Fisher underscores,

Time: 5682.02

and I'd like to underscore here as well,

Time: 5684.07

is that it need not be the case that people pair up

Time: 5688.83

exactly according to these categorizations

Time: 5691.03

that I've described, dopamine with dopamine,

Time: 5693.74

serotonin with serotonin, testosterone with estrogen,

Time: 5696.197

and so on.

Time: 5697.76

What is important is that there be a recognition

Time: 5700.96

and a respect for the other types,

Time: 5703.79

or a recognition and a respect for the fact that

Time: 5706.58

both are of the same type.

Time: 5708.2

You could actually imagine, for instance, that two people

Time: 5710.82

of this high sensation seeking, novelty seeking

Time: 5714.06

could have a terrifically exciting relationship,

Time: 5716.56

but that it actually might be a relationship

Time: 5720.41

in which the financial stability isn't quite there,

Time: 5723.67

or in which the basic stability isn't there.

Time: 5727.05

You could imagine, for instance, a situation

Time: 5730.7

in which a relationship between two people

Time: 5732.94

of what she called the high serotonin preference

Time: 5737.22

would have a relationship that was actually dull,

Time: 5739.67

in which both of them found themselves bored at some point,

Time: 5742.72

or in which there wasn't enough of the dynamic tension

Time: 5747.48

that sometimes is required in order to keep this cycle

Time: 5751.04

of desire, love, and attachment going.

Time: 5752.58

Something that we will talk about in a moment.

Time: 5755.06

So the point here is not that one should necessarily pair up

Time: 5759.93

according to these arrangements that I described.

Time: 5762.94

The point is that on average, that's what tends to happen.

Time: 5766.63

And that through a recognition that these categorizations

Time: 5770.71

exist similar to the recognition that the type

Time: 5775.32

A, B, C, and D infant and toddler type attachments exist,

Time: 5779.97

that we can gain better self-awareness of who we are

Time: 5783.69

and how we tend to show up in romantic attachments,

Time: 5786.83

and thereby navigate healthier mate seeking,

Time: 5791.96

healthier breakups, if the case dictates it,

Time: 5796.42

and in some cases healthy long-term relationships

Time: 5799.38

by understanding that the other person can either be similar

Time: 5802.47

or complimentary to us.

Time: 5804.72

One is neither better than the other.

Time: 5807.1

It's simply the case that in all romantic attachments,

Time: 5810.72

from the initial inception of the romantic attachment,

Time: 5814.58

desire, love, and attachment,

Time: 5818.12

there is an autonomic coordination.

Time: 5821.06

And of course there's coordination of all sorts

Time: 5823.25

of other things like food, sex, and sleep, and finances,

Time: 5827.9

and where people are going to live, and many other features,

Time: 5830.66

but that at the core of all that is a seeking

Time: 5834.33

of either autonomic likeness or autonomic differences.

Time: 5839.11

And I think that recognition can be extremely valuable

Time: 5842.7

in thinking about tools to enter and maintain relationships.

Time: 5847.11

If one thinks about their autonomic nervous system,

Time: 5850.43

not simply as something that is driven by external people

Time: 5854.43

and events, but that we can actually gain some control over

Time: 5857.97

through techniques of the sort that I talked about earlier

Time: 5860.44

and on previous podcasts,

Time: 5861.88

but also generally, if we are able to adjust

Time: 5865.17

our autonomic nervous system in order to at least appreciate

Time: 5869.23

or get some empathy into what someone else is experiencing,

Time: 5873.16

then we gain actual cognitive empathy.

Time: 5876.42

And this episode isn't about empathy per se,

Time: 5880.13

but the theme keeps coming up again and again.

Time: 5881.92

And I think it's worth mentioning that when you talk

Time: 5884.33

to psychologists, whether or not they're psychoanalyst

Time: 5887.42

or from another source of training,

Time: 5890.01

what you find is that they don't talk about empathy

Time: 5892.35

as a general term, they will talk about emotional empathy,

Time: 5896.67

they'll talk about cognitive empathy.

Time: 5898.34

And what I'm talking about here today is that

Time: 5900.25

yet a third category that is very strongly determinant

Time: 5904.21

of relationship dynamics, and that's autonomic empathy.

Time: 5907.96

I'm a biologist, I'm not a psychologist.

Time: 5910.64

So I love mechanism.

Time: 5912.35

And fortunately there are studies that have been done

Time: 5915.7

recently using modern techniques

Time: 5917.88

to look at neural mechanisms of romantic attachment.

Time: 5921.63

I mentioned earlier some of the brain imaging studies

Time: 5924.81

that have been done on child and mother literally imaging

Time: 5929.76

the activity of neurons in the brain as child is nursing,

Time: 5932.84

or as a mother is soothing baby,

Time: 5935

and as you learned earlier, baby is soothing mother as well.

Time: 5939.45

Those are remarkable studies.

Time: 5940.42

You may have seen some of these pictures online.

Time: 5942.12

You can see the silhouette of the infant and mother,

Time: 5944.57

and their brains and even some of the brain activation

Time: 5947.37

patterns, really, really beautiful studies.

Time: 5949.63

Similar studies have been done in romantic couples

Time: 5953.93

with those couples, either touching one another,

Time: 5957.17

touching and kissing, or in clever, I think,

Time: 5960.72

control experiments of the person just touching a pillow,

Time: 5963.284

[laughs] or something, or kissing a pillow

Time: 5966.46

in order to try and create the most reasonable control

Time: 5970.51

for what are actually pretty

Time: 5971.96

complicated interpersonal dynamics

Time: 5973.436

to do in a brain imaging scanner.

Time: 5976.77

But some of the other studies that have been done recently

Time: 5980.07

involved so called EEG.

Time: 5981.27

So these are electrical recordings

Time: 5982.96

that are done noninvasively,

Time: 5984.93

putting a bunch of electrodes on the outside of the scalp.

Time: 5987.57

EEG is useful in that you can do it noninvasively,

Time: 5992.34

you can do it while people are moving and doing things,

Time: 5996.38

kissing, touching, et cetera.

Time: 5998.85

It doesn't allow one to image or to evaluate neural activity

Time: 6003.16

very deeply in the brain.

Time: 6004.64

So you can miss out on a lot of things.

Time: 6006.39

It's like looking at the wave structure on the ocean

Time: 6008.92

without actually looking into the depths of the ocean.

Time: 6012.32

So you can miss certain things,

Time: 6013.64

but if you see things generally you trust they're there,

Time: 6017.02

but you can't see what you don't see.

Time: 6018.66

Nonetheless, there's some studies

Time: 6021.09

that I'll just point you to and that form the segue

Time: 6024.66

for what I'm going to discuss in a moment,

Time: 6027.44

which is a study published in Scientific Reports in 2021,

Time: 6033.08

entitled "Investigating Real Life Emotions

Time: 6035.28

in Romantic Couples: a Mobile EEG Study."

Time: 6037.96

So this is, as the title suggests, having people wear

Time: 6041.88

these EEG caps of electrodes, get engage in very passionate,

Time: 6047.36

emotional kisses, emotional speech toward one another

Time: 6050.56

standing at different distances.

Time: 6051.71

So a lot of cool stuff that you can do that you really

Time: 6054.16

couldn't do in a brain scanner, because in a brain scanner,

Time: 6056.87

people have to be there usually in a bite bar,

Time: 6058.97

they're actually jaw flicked like this.

Time: 6060.43

I've been in one of these things.

Time: 6062.42

There's not a lot of moving around to be heard,

Time: 6063.85

at least not using the current technology.

Time: 6067.07

In any case, what they found was

Time: 6069.51

there is a shift in brain waves, brain states,

Time: 6074.23

things like alpha waves or just a particular frequency

Time: 6076.76

of brain waves in the neocortex, the outer shell

Time: 6079.54

of the brain just beneath the skull.

Time: 6082.19

And in people that are kissing or in people that are engaged

Time: 6086.68

in romantic speech, or I didn't actually hear what they said

Time: 6091.19

to one another, but what the couple seems exciting,

Time: 6094.54

romantic and arousing to them, they see more alpha wave

Time: 6098.16

activity compared to the control conditions.

Time: 6100.33

And there was some what we call lateralization,

Time: 6102.7

where the left hemisphere was more active than the right

Time: 6104.75

and so forth.

Time: 6106.78

And these studies are important because we know that

Time: 6110.43

the autonomic nervous systems of individuals tend

Time: 6113.45

to start to collaborate and actually synchronize

Time: 6118.03

at the level of heartbeats, at the level of breathing

Time: 6121.26

during romantic interactions of different kinds.

Time: 6123.6

But these studies are some of the first

Time: 6125.29

of their kind to start looking

Time: 6126.55

at neural synchronization between individuals.

Time: 6129.92

Now, the simple version of looking at this

Time: 6134.27

and the way I would've thought this would all go was,

Time: 6137.21

okay, two people start kissing, they start talking

Time: 6139.64

about what they find particularly romantic

Time: 6141.59

and arousing for them.

Time: 6142.423

And their brain waves will just match to one another.

Time: 6144.85

And that's really the basis of romantic attachment

Time: 6148.377

and romantic engagement in that sort of thing.

Time: 6152.3

But it turns out that the opposite is true.

Time: 6156.33

So a really nice study published in a really fine journal,

Time: 6160.85

Cerebral Cortex is a journal that I've known

Time: 6162.85

about for many years, they published strong anatomy,

Time: 6164.84

physiology and neuroimaging.

Time: 6166.78

There's a study that was published, first author, Kajimura,

Time: 6172.59

and this paper really points...

Time: 6174.6

Again, this is 2021.

Time: 6176.99

And the title of this paper is,

Time: 6178.627

"Brain Knows Who Is on the Same Wavelength:

Time: 6181.11

Resting-State Connectivity Can Predict Compatibility

Time: 6184.05

of a Female-Male Relationship."

Time: 6186.41

Now what this study did was a little bit different.

Time: 6188.84

They looked at the resting or default mode

Time: 6191.75

activity of the brain.

Time: 6192.89

So rather than evoked activity, as it's called,

Time: 6195.92

where people are kissing or are engaged in some activity,

Time: 6199.44

this was a neuroimaging study, not EEG, but fMRI,

Time: 6204.17

functional magnetic resonance imaging,

Time: 6205.83

which is similar to EEG in principle,

Time: 6209.19

but allows you to look deep into the brain.

Time: 6211.41

And it has a very good resolution in time and space.

Time: 6215.49

So fast events can be monitored and the precise location

Time: 6220

of those events can be monitored somewhat better than EEG.

Time: 6223.64

And there are exceptions to this.

Time: 6224.78

So for you EEGers out there.

Time: 6226.38

EEG don't come after me with electrodes.

Time: 6230.37

Just understand that fMRI gives you a fuller picture

Time: 6234.2

of what's going on.

Time: 6235.15

And what Kajimura et al found was that contrary

Time: 6239.55

to what your reflexive prediction might be,

Time: 6245.01

people tend to select people that have resting brain states

Time: 6250.08

that different than theirs,

Time: 6251.99

or sometimes they found that are actually opposite

Time: 6255.24

to their own resting brain state.

Time: 6257.31

And you might say, "Well, that doesn't make any sense.

Time: 6259.36

I thought this is all about autonomic coordination."

Time: 6262.29

But actually if we go back to Helen Fisher's categorizations

Time: 6265.702

of the dopamine types, the sensation seeking types,

Time: 6268.65

that is serotonin, the stable rule following types,

Time: 6273.21

testosterone and estrogen types,

Time: 6274.53

remember that the two categories that she called

Time: 6276.91

testosterone and estrogen type,

Time: 6278.18

the director and the follower, the nurturer,

Time: 6280.72

I guess it would be the more accurate way,

Time: 6282.18

the director and the nurturer,

Time: 6283.94

those tend to pair up across categories,

Time: 6286.75

not within category.

Time: 6288.46

And so I think what's really needed for this field

Time: 6291.46

which to my knowledge hasn't happened yet,

Time: 6293.7

is to really start to map the neuroanatomical

Time: 6298.057

and neurophysiological findings of, in this case,

Time: 6301.16

that resting brain state is in one form in one individual,

Time: 6306.79

and they tend to seek out people whose resting brain state

Time: 6309.29

is different than theirs, not similar.

Time: 6311.93

That needs to be mapped onto the more subjective

Time: 6315.53

psychological categorizations that Helen Fisher

Time: 6319.37

and indeed the Gottman's and others have created,

Time: 6322.02

that sort of the state of the field now.

Time: 6324.03

And I mentioned this not to confuse you,

Time: 6326.43

but to the contrary, to illustrate that it's not just

Time: 6330.06

about finding someone just like you,

Time: 6332.1

and it's not just about finding someone

Time: 6334.43

who's opposite to you.

Time: 6335.92

This is actually the reason that I decided to become

Time: 6338.03

a biologist at some point in my life, which is that

Time: 6341.62

we can find verbal sayings, and stories, and examples

Time: 6344.61

to support just about anything.

Time: 6347.22

This is not a knock on the field of psychology,

Time: 6349.18

as you can probably tell from today's episode.

Time: 6350.87

I have great respect for in reverence

Time: 6352.61

for the field of psychology, especially its collaboration

Time: 6355.99

with neuroscience and vice versa.

Time: 6358.1

But in the popular culture,

Time: 6360.75

we can find examples and sayings that support essentially

Time: 6364.04

anything as it relates to a relationship.

Time: 6366.4

For instance, I've heard and you've probably heard,

Time: 6368.9

absence makes the heart grow fonder.

Time: 6370.9

And indeed I've experienced that and I believe it's true.

Time: 6374.01

But I also have experienced and I believe to be true

Time: 6376.41

that out of sight out of mind also exists

Time: 6378.83

and that there will be a biological mechanism for that.

Time: 6381.69

The point here is that matching of same to same

Time: 6384.8

or same to different can both be effective in creating

Time: 6387.558

the desire, love, attachment process.

Time: 6392.88

It's a matter of, who is looking for same,

Time: 6396.16

and who is looking for different?

Time: 6397.39

And there, I think Dr. Fisher and the work

Time: 6400.15

of these neurophysiologist and brain imagers

Time: 6402.73

really does point in a direction whereby

Time: 6405.25

there is not one form of attachment

Time: 6408.96

that is going to be wholly above [indistinct]

Time: 6411.28

will predict good outcomes.

Time: 6413.12

There is not going to be a case in which opposites attract,

Time: 6416.55

and that's always the best rule to follow.

Time: 6418.153

Sometimes it will, sometimes it won't.

Time: 6421.32

There is also not the case that people tend

Time: 6423.32

to pair up with similar.

Time: 6425.34

Sometimes it will be the case, sometimes it won't.

Time: 6428.14

Now there are certain statistics

Time: 6429.673

that support that statement.

Time: 6431.36

For instance, people, on average,

Time: 6433.69

people pair up with individuals of similar educational

Time: 6437.32

background, income, and attractiveness.

Time: 6439.17

That is true on average, but it's not always the case.

Time: 6442.42

And again, a knowledge of and a respect for the different

Time: 6446.15

categorizations of attachment, the different categorizations

Time: 6448.786

of mate seeking described by Fisher and others,

Time: 6451.61

and the recognition that matching of autonomic nervous

Time: 6454.39

systems but also mismatching of resting state brain networks

Time: 6458.6

are all at play in driving what we are calling

Time: 6461.65

desire, love and attachment.

Time: 6463.49

So in keeping with the exploration of the fact that

Time: 6465.66

there's a saying, or a book, or a song, or an example

Time: 6469.32

of pretty much any relationship dynamic,

Time: 6472.32

I want to now talk about an article that came out a little

Time: 6475.5

over 10 years ago that talked about the universality of love

Time: 6481.41

and the ability to fall in love.

Time: 6482.85

So this would be very much in line

Time: 6484.16

with the George Bernard Shaw quote that I mentioned earlier

Time: 6486.61

that "Love is really overestimating

Time: 6489.13

the differences between individuals."

Time: 6490.67

And again, I should say that is not something

Time: 6492.16

that I personally believe,

Time: 6493.72

although maybe I'm just diluting myself.

Time: 6495.63

I like to think that the people that we fall in love

Time: 6498.38

with are really special for us,

Time: 6501.98

that they could not easily be replaced with anybody else.

Time: 6505.63

That's simply my stance.

Time: 6506.98

I'm not basing that

Time: 6507.87

on any hardcore neurobiological mechanism.

Time: 6511.61

But nonetheless, an article was published

Time: 6515.24

in The New York Times in 2015 that related

Time: 6518.39

to some psychological studies that were done,

Time: 6522.42

as well as some clinical work, as well as some,

Time: 6524.61

what I would call pop psychology or things that fall

Time: 6528.18

outside the domains of academic science.

Time: 6530.73

And the whole basis of this article was

Time: 6536.33

36 questions that lead to love.

Time: 6538.98

And it involved a listing out indeed of 36 questions,

Time: 6543.42

divided into set one, set two, and set three that progressed

Time: 6547.17

from somewhat ordinary questions about life experience

Time: 6551.39

and self-report to more,

Time: 6554.64

let's call them deep questions about people's values

Time: 6558.142

and things that are emotionally close to them.

Time: 6560.24

And I'll just give an example of a few of these.

Time: 6561.99

You can find this easily online by just putting

Time: 6564.69

into your search engine, 36 questions that lead to love.

Time: 6567.96

Some of the questions in set number one, where for instance,

Time: 6571.72

what would constitute a perfect day for you?

Time: 6575.4

For what in your life do you feel most grateful?

Time: 6577.12

Kind of standard questionnaire stuff.

Time: 6578.69

In set two, what is your most treasured memory?

Time: 6581.97

What is your most terrible memory?

Time: 6583.42

So these, as you can tell, are drilling a little bit deeper

Time: 6586.02

into one's personal experience and emotional system.

Time: 6589.27

And then set three, questions 25 through 36 are things,

Time: 6596.55

what is a very embarrassing moment in your life?

Time: 6600.28

When did you last cry in front of another person

Time: 6603.127

and by yourself?

Time: 6604.97

What is something that's too serious to be joked about?

Time: 6607.11

So it's going deeper into one's emotional system.

Time: 6611.01

And even questions like, of all the people in your family,

Time: 6613.78

whose death would you find most disturbing and why?

Time: 6616.69

So pretty, pretty heavy stuff there at the end.

Time: 6619.2

Now, the reason this article got so much traction,

Time: 6621.76

and the reason I'm bringing it up today is that

Time: 6623.85

there was a statement that was made in and around

Time: 6627.44

this article that if two people went on a date

Time: 6632.41

or simply sat down and asked each other these questions,

Time: 6636.52

and each answered these questions and the other was paying

Time: 6641.97

attention carefully and at some level emotionally responding

Time: 6647.24

or not out responding, but certainly paying attention

Time: 6648.683

to the answers of the other person,

Time: 6651.18

that by the end of that exchange, where one person asks

Time: 6655.8

36 questions and the other person answers all 36,

Time: 6658.61

and then the other person asks all 36 and the other person

Time: 6661.92

answers all 36, that they would fall in love, right?

Time: 6665.66

Which seems like a ridiculous thing.

Time: 6667.97

And yet it is the case that people who go

Time: 6671.95

through this exercise report feeling as if they know

Time: 6676.4

the other person quite well and feeling certain levels

Time: 6680.6

of attachment or even love and desire for the other person

Time: 6683.8

that they were, would not have predicted, excuse me,

Time: 6687.23

would not have predicted

Time: 6689.26

had they not gone through that process.

Time: 6691.22

So what's going on in this exchange of questions

Time: 6692.578

and answers of a progressively more emotional

Time: 6696.67

and deep level?

Time: 6698.58

Well, what I predict is going on is that inside

Time: 6701.31

of that exchange, people are creating a delusional story

Time: 6706.13

about the nature of the exchange being a reflection

Time: 6709.6

of some deeper attachment.

Time: 6711.28

And so even though people are just exchanging words,

Time: 6714.01

they're not physically touching, they are not, at least not

Time: 6717.77

at the point where they're running these kinds

Time: 6720.738

of questionnaire studies.

Time: 6723.51

They may touch afterwards for all I know,

Time: 6725.4

and probably did in some cases,

Time: 6726.734

but they're not exchanging life experience

Time: 6729.78

in an immediate way.

Time: 6731.35

They're not actually going off into the world

Time: 6733.19

and doing things together yet.

Time: 6735.06

They are simply exchanging narrative.

Time: 6737.84

But we know based on recent studies and I've covered

Time: 6741.76

this before on this podcast, but I'll mention again,

Time: 6744.19

there was a study published in Cell Reports,

Time: 6747.26

a Cell Press journal, excellent journal

Time: 6749.4

showing that when individuals listen to the same narrative,

Time: 6753.93

their heart rates tend to synchronize

Time: 6755.147

or at least follow a very similar pattern,

Time: 6757.8

even if they're not in the same room

Time: 6759.51

listening to a given narrative.

Time: 6761.15

Whereas in this case, people are facing one another

Time: 6764.21

listening to the narratives of each other.

Time: 6766.69

Certainly they are having autonomic responses.

Time: 6769.96

And it stands to reason that their autonomic nervous systems

Time: 6773.15

are synchronizing much in the same way that

Time: 6775.25

the Cell Report study found that people will synchronize

Time: 6778.41

their autonomic nervous systems to a shared,

Time: 6781.74

heard story from another.

Time: 6783.85

In other words, whether or not we hear a story,

Time: 6786.28

watch a movie, listen to a song,

Time: 6788.52

or exchange our own individual stories,

Time: 6791.33

our autonomic nervous systems have the potential

Time: 6793.6

to map onto one another.

Time: 6795.04

So I'm not all that surprised that people find

Time: 6798.86

that they fall in love, in quotes,

Time: 6801.87

after answering these questions to one another,

Time: 6804.24

because essentially the way these questions

Time: 6805.88

are laid out is they establish a narrative.

Time: 6808.98

They establish a very personal narrative

Time: 6810.93

and the other person is listening very closely.

Time: 6812.277

And we don't have physiological or brain imaging studies

Time: 6815.97

to support what I'm about to say,

Time: 6818.67

but the reasonable interpretation is that

Time: 6822.12

that's causing some autonomic synchronization.

Time: 6825.47

So if you want to try this on a date or even it's actually

Time: 6828.8

been hypothesized that this could be useful

Time: 6831.4

for existing couples, even if they already know the answers

Time: 6834.99

to some of these questions.

Time: 6836.05

And that doesn't surprise me either.

Time: 6837.78

I think the autonomic coordination is present

Time: 6839.85

during mating behavior,

Time: 6842.44

it's present during shared experience of the outside world,

Time: 6845.91

movies, concerts, watching one's children

Time: 6848.81

with somebody else, et cetera.

Time: 6850.031

And it's established by sharing one's own narrative

Time: 6853.45

of their own personal experience.

Time: 6855.17

So I don't want to seem overly reductionist.

Time: 6857.43

I'll never propose that all of our sensation, perception,

Time: 6861.53

action and experience in life boils down to us just being

Time: 6865.02

bags of chemicals and the action of those chemicals,

Time: 6868.06

or any aspect of our nervous system.

Time: 6869.253

And yet in looking across the psychological literature

Time: 6874.56

of development of attachment, in the psychological

Time: 6877.19

literature of adult and romantic attachment,

Time: 6880.09

and what makes and breaks those attachments,

Time: 6883.41

it's very clear to me.

Time: 6884.88

And I think courses through the literature multiple levels,

Time: 6888.4

that autonomic coordination is absolutely key

Time: 6892.41

for the establishment of desire, love and attachment.

Time: 6895.66

In fact, I talked earlier about how our actual conception

Time: 6898.58

is born out of autonomic coordination

Time: 6900.87

of one sort or another.

Time: 6902.6

So again, it doesn't necessarily mean that autonomic nervous

Time: 6906.5

systems always be synchronized in the case of the two

Time: 6909.46

categorizations that Fisher proposed

Time: 6911.82

of the director/testosterone type

Time: 6914.38

and the nurturing follower/estrogen type.

Time: 6920.22

It was actually the coordination but in opposite directions

Time: 6924.67

of individuals that fall into each of those categories

Time: 6927.33

that led to more stable attachments

Time: 6928.643

or the seeking out of those attachments, I should say.

Time: 6931.84

But nonetheless, it's, at least to my mind, very clear

Time: 6935.61

that autonomic coordination is a hallmark feature

Time: 6939.39

of desire, a hallmark feature of what we call love,

Time: 6944.25

and a hallmark feature of what we call attachment.

Time: 6947.11

And that the breaking of attachments were the failures

Time: 6951.06

of desire, the failures of love,

Time: 6953.95

and the failures of attachment over time

Time: 6956.74

in line with the work of Gottman and others.

Time: 6958.66

And in even just simply what's required for mating behavior

Time: 6962.57

is also reflected in the autonomic nervous system.

Time: 6965.7

But in that case, a failure to coordinate

Time: 6967.91

the autonomic nervous systems in some sort of concerted way.

Time: 6971.38

Any discussion about desire, love, and attachment

Time: 6974.24

would be incomplete

Time: 6975.96

if we didn't talk about the dreaded infidelity and cheating.

Time: 6980.42

Much has been made of infidelity and cheating

Time: 6982.36

and whether or not people who are higher on dopamine

Time: 6984.85

and sensation seeking tend to cheat more or less.

Time: 6987.41

Frankly, I don't think there's any solid evidence for that.

Time: 6991.75

I think there are a lot of examples that we can draw

Time: 6993.71

from in our own lives and in the lives of others

Time: 6996.83

that would generally support one or the other model,

Time: 6999.59

but I'm not aware of any decent physiological studies

Time: 7003.41

or psychological studies that really point to that.

Time: 7006.29

For instance, I would never say that the serotonergic

Time: 7010.06

phenotype as described by Fisher is less prone to cheat,

Time: 7014.8

or that the people who have an insecure attachment

Time: 7018.63

are more likely to cheat, et cetera, for instance.

Time: 7022.01

I don't think those correlations have been drawn

Time: 7024.45

in any kind of meaningful way yet.

Time: 7025.88

So I would be cautious about assigning them

Time: 7029.33

without that evidence.

Time: 7031.4

However, there are some interesting studies involving,

Time: 7034.64

again, neuroimaging and some subjective measures in humans,

Time: 7038.9

meaning asking them questions

Time: 7040.33

that they're good ways to tease out lies from truths

Time: 7043.81

in these sorts of studies and whether or not people tend

Time: 7048.28

to find their partner or others more or less attractive

Time: 7053.5

depending on how people feel about themselves.

Time: 7056.48

And I think this is a very interesting aspect

Time: 7059.47

to desire, love, and attachment for the following reason.

Time: 7063.92

You hear a lot out out there that in order

Time: 7067.14

to form a really strong relationship,

Time: 7069.95

you have to have a good relationship with yourself,

Time: 7071.842

or you have to love yourself.

Time: 7074.42

Or you often hear for instance that it's exactly when you're

Time: 7079.73

not looking for a relationship that you're going to find one.

Time: 7082.02

You hear this stuff, right?

Time: 7083.43

But none of that is really grounded in any studies.

Time: 7085.8

Again, that's like out of sight out of mind,

Time: 7087.041

or absence makes the heart grow fonder.

Time: 7089.36

There are many life examples to support those statements.

Time: 7093.5

And there are many life examples

Time: 7094.917

to support statements to the opposite.

Time: 7099.07

There's a particular study that I found.

Time: 7101.7

This was published in Frontiers in Psychology,

Time: 7103.95

but it's a experimental study that involves neuroimaging.

Time: 7108.02

The title of this study is "Manipulation of Self-Expansion

Time: 7111.78

Alters Responses to Attractive Alternative Partners."

Time: 7115.5

And I love the design of this study.

Time: 7117.19

What they did in this study is they took couples

Time: 7120.54

and they evaluated members of that relationship

Time: 7124.56

for what's called self-expansion.

Time: 7126.078

Now self-expansion is a metric that involves

Time: 7131.84

one's perception of self

Time: 7133.38

as seen through the relationship to the other.

Time: 7136.83

And this is something that was developed by,

Time: 7139.9

the authors are Aron and Aron.

Time: 7142.38

So they have the same last name.

Time: 7143.48

So I'm assuming this was either a sibling team

Time: 7145.95

or a somehow related team or a romantic couple team.

Time: 7150.21

A-R-O-N and A-R-O-N, Aron and Aron, in 1986,

Time: 7155.78

proposed this self-expansion model of close relationships.

Time: 7158.71

And they proposed "That people are motivated

Time: 7160.64

to enter relationships..."

Time: 7161.72

I'm reading here.

Time: 7162.553

"In order to enhance the self and increase self-efficacy."

Time: 7165.83

In other words, that one of the reasons why many people

Time: 7168.78

enter relationships is that it makes us feel good

Time: 7171.1

about ourselves and more capable.

Time: 7172.46

And I would see that as a healthy interdependence,

Time: 7174.9

not necessarily codependence.

Time: 7178.38

This is especially strong at the beginning

Time: 7179.961

of a relationship, it turns out,

Time: 7181.59

when people are forming pair bonds,

Time: 7183.69

and it's the case that pleasure, arousal and excitement,

Time: 7188.34

again, all hallmark features

Time: 7190.69

of autonomic nervous system function,

Time: 7192.49

pleasure, arousal and excitement,

Time: 7195.42

give rise to self-expansion, meaning to self-efficacy.

Time: 7199.14

So what this self-expansion model is really about is

Time: 7202.97

how great other people that we are close to and romantically

Time: 7206.15

attached to can potentially make us feel,

Time: 7209.47

in terms of what they say, in terms of what they do,

Time: 7212.73

in terms of the way in which we believe they feel about us.

Time: 7218.17

So it doesn't necessarily have to involve explicit

Time: 7220.88

statements of them telling us how great we are,

Time: 7224.07

or them doing great gestures for us,

Time: 7227.1

but how we actually feel they feel about us.

Time: 7230.28

Turns out to be a very strong parameter in terms

Time: 7232.78

of how we feel about ourselves and the relationship overall.

Time: 7236.25

Now, some of you out there are probably thinking,

Time: 7237.707

"Oh yeah, isn't there this thing

Time: 7238.86

of the love languages?" Right?

Time: 7240.85

I don't have any neuroscience to support, I think,

Time: 7242.88

the love languages, I'm not super familiar with this,

Time: 7244.777

and I didn't list it out, but that some people are...

Time: 7249.32

Their autonomic nervous system, if you will,

Time: 7252.09

tends to be very responsive to gifts, or to quality time,

Time: 7256.47

or to physical touch, or acts of kindness.

Time: 7258.23

I think I've got a few of these right.

Time: 7259.31

I probably have a few wrong.

Time: 7260.28

Anyway, they're easy to find online.

Time: 7261.67

And people do tend to have a bias toward two or three

Time: 7265.42

of these things that are especially

Time: 7266.76

meaningful for them.

Time: 7267.88

And when I hear meaningful, I hear they tend to push

Time: 7270.38

the autonomic nervous system and neurochemical systems

Time: 7272.97

of the brain and body in a direction that makes us feel good

Time: 7275.54

as opposed to lousy or neutral.

Time: 7278.06

In any event, this study looked at whether or not

Time: 7282.3

people have high levels of self-expansion

Time: 7286.41

through the actions or statements

Time: 7289.06

of their significant other, and how that influences

Time: 7292.275

their perception of people outside the relationship.

Time: 7296.25

Meaning how attractive they perceive people

Time: 7298.98

outside the relationship to be,

Time: 7300.87

turns out to be strongly influenced

Time: 7303.28

by, A, whether or not their self-expansion is very strongly

Time: 7310.27

driven by the other person that they are involved with,

Time: 7313.164

that they're in the romantic relationship with,

Time: 7315.47

and whether or not that's being expressed to them.

Time: 7317.6

So here's how the study went.

Time: 7319.391

First of all, they rated or categorized individuals

Time: 7323.91

on the basis of the self-expansion metric.

Time: 7326.18

Some people have more of a potential to experience

Time: 7329.98

self-expansion through others, right?

Time: 7331.83

Some of us feel great about ourselves and we're topped off,

Time: 7335.07

the others don't feel so great about themselves,

Time: 7337.88

but they can feel much better in response to praise,

Time: 7340.38

in particular praise or self-expansion type behaviors

Time: 7343.84

or statements from people that we really care about.

Time: 7345.86

And still other people are a mixture of the two.

Time: 7348.24

They moderate levels of both.

Time: 7350.37

So they rated them on this scale.

Time: 7352.01

And then they had people

Time: 7353.44

experience self-expansion narratives.

Time: 7357.25

They heard their significant others say really terrific

Time: 7359.81

things about them and about the relationship, in particular,

Time: 7362.87

that the relationship that they have was exciting,

Time: 7366.06

novel and challenging.

Time: 7367.37

So that was one form of self-expansion.

Time: 7369.08

And they went into some detail as to why that was the case

Time: 7371.6

in their particular relationship.

Time: 7372.977

Or they heard a narrative from their significant other

Time: 7377.91

about strong feelings of love between the two

Time: 7382.21

that had been experienced previously in the relationship.

Time: 7384.64

So in the one case, it directed more towards them.

Time: 7387.67

And in the other case,

Time: 7388.503

it's more about the relationship itself.

Time: 7390.58

And then they did brain imaging of one person

Time: 7394.47

in the relationship while that person assessed

Time: 7397.13

the attractiveness of people outside the relationship.

Time: 7401.15

And what they found was that people who were primed

Time: 7403.9

for this self-expansion had lower activation of brain areas

Time: 7409.47

associated with assessing others' attractiveness

Time: 7412.59

than did the people who experienced a lot of self-expansion.

Time: 7416.65

Now the takeaway from that, at least the way I read

Time: 7420.01

the study is if you're with somebody who really benefits

Time: 7424.09

from or experiences a lot of self-expansion,

Time: 7428.72

unless you really want them to pay attention

Time: 7430.54

to the attractiveness of other people,

Time: 7432.71

it stands to reason that they would benefit

Time: 7436.38

from more self-expansion type gestures or statements, okay?

Time: 7441.22

Not so much centered on the relationship.

Time: 7442.724

We have such a great relationship.

Time: 7444.61

There's so much love, it's so great, that too,

Time: 7447.07

but in the context of this study and these findings,

Time: 7449.7

that the person is really terrific, that the relationship

Time: 7453

that they've created together is really exciting,

Time: 7454.97

novel, and challenging, that there's a narrative around

Time: 7457.97

the relationship that really has a lot to do

Time: 7459.709

with the dynamics between the individuals, in particular,

Time: 7463.02

that the person who really likes self-expansion

Time: 7465.4

is vital to that dynamic, okay?

Time: 7467.21

So it's not looking down at the relationship

Time: 7468.89

as a set of equals.

Time: 7470.14

There is this bias written into this of that

Time: 7472.55

this person is really essential for the relationship.

Time: 7475.03

I'm not saying this is something that anyone has to do.

Time: 7476.79

I'm not not saying this is right or wrong.

Time: 7478.09

This is just what the data say.

Time: 7480.16

But what's remarkable is that in the absence

Time: 7483.14

of those statements, people who have,

Time: 7487.14

or that rate high on this scale of self-expansion

Time: 7490.72

rate attractive alternative partners as more attractive.

Time: 7494.5

Now, that's interesting to me, because it may means that

Time: 7498.07

their actual perception of others is changing.

Time: 7500.89

It's not that their opportunity

Time: 7502.252

to see others is changing, right?

Time: 7504.94

This is not a matter of them somehow getting access

Time: 7508.44

or no access to attractive alternative partners.

Time: 7511.18

Again, attractive alternative partners,

Time: 7512.56

literally the language in the title of this paper,

Time: 7514.98

they're still seeing all these attractive people.

Time: 7517.18

It's just that if they're feeling filled up, in air quotes,

Time: 7521.49

psychologically filled up, emotionally filled up,

Time: 7524.647

autonomically filled, enhanced in the language

Time: 7528.46

that we're using today by the self-expansion narrative,

Time: 7531.52

well then the same set of attractive faces appear

Time: 7535.32

less attractive to a given individual.

Time: 7538.51

Now, whether or not this predicts cheating or loyalty,

Time: 7540.944

[laughs] I certainly can't say.

Time: 7543.02

That would be very hard to assess in neuroimaging.

Time: 7545.8

And there, of course, people rarely if ever

Time: 7550.82

report accurately their cheating behavior.

Time: 7553.24

There's some studies in which confidentiality is assured

Time: 7556.24

to the point where people seem to be more trusting

Time: 7559.2

and willing to reveal cheating behavior.

Time: 7561.83

But if you look at the statistics on cheating behavior,

Time: 7563.62

it's very hard to track because people lie all the time

Time: 7567.13

about their cheating in and outside of the context

Time: 7569.25

of psychological and neuroimaging studies.

Time: 7572.6

But I find this study, again, the title,

Time: 7575.267

"Manipulation of Self-expansion Alters Responses

Time: 7577.68

to Attractive Alternative Partners"

Time: 7579.18

to be absolutely fascinating, because again, it points

Time: 7582.54

to the fact that the interactions with our significant

Time: 7586.83

others shapes our autonomic arousal, shapes our perception

Time: 7590.48

of self and thereby shapes our perception of other potential

Time: 7595.25

partners in the outside world,

Time: 7596.78

or shuts us down to the potential of other people

Time: 7600.14

in the outside world.

Time: 7601.5

So when I hear statements such as,

Time: 7603.04

it's important that you love yourself

Time: 7604.66

in order to really fall in love with somebody else,

Time: 7607.22

or it is when one is not looking for a relationship

Time: 7611.33

that they're most likely to fall in love

Time: 7612.71

and form a stable relationship,

Time: 7616.29

I can filter that through these findings to say that

Time: 7619.97

it's really the person who needs a lot of self-expansion,

Time: 7625.23

stimulating statements or actions coming from other people

Time: 7629.14

that is most prone to seeing other potential partners

Time: 7633.79

out in the world as attractive.

Time: 7635.75

And in this sense,

Time: 7637.61

we can return to the autonomic nervous system

Time: 7639.69

as a glass that it can be filled up

Time: 7643.43

through various contexts.

Time: 7644.52

It can be filled up through our own ability to regulate it.

Time: 7647.28

It can be filled up through other people's ability

Time: 7650.64

to enhance our sense of wellbeing.

Time: 7652.84

And in some sense, this points to an idea where it is true

Time: 7657.09

that the better that we can feel about ourselves

Time: 7659.07

in the absence of any self-expansion type input

Time: 7663.56

from somebody else really does place us

Time: 7665.67

on more stable grounds, such that when we do receive

Time: 7668.17

that praise, or we do receive those acts of kindness,

Time: 7670.53

or service, or physical touch or whatever they are,

Time: 7673.28

that we are able to further enhance the way that we feel,

Time: 7678.75

but that we don't necessarily tether all of our feelings

Time: 7682.17

of self-worth or self-expansion to that one individual.

Time: 7686.68

So you might think that if person A can only receive

Time: 7690.25

the self-expansion from the statements, from the action

Time: 7694.51

of the they're involved with, person B,

Time: 7696.75

that that will form a very stable bond,

Time: 7699.64

but what this study points to is the fact that

Time: 7701.4

that's a very unstable bond.

Time: 7703.52

That person A is actually very susceptible

Time: 7706.19

to the attractiveness of others,

Time: 7707.48

because they're so desperately attached to this notion

Time: 7710.34

of self-expansion, even if they don't realize it.

Time: 7712.58

And so this really does point to the idea that,

Time: 7715.32

well, it is important to link our autonomic nervous systems

Time: 7718.92

to establish desire, love, and attachment

Time: 7722.37

that we want to have a stable internal representation

Time: 7725.91

of ourselves, a stable autonomic nervous system

Time: 7729.16

to some degree in other so that we can be in stable

Time: 7733.33

romantic partnership with another individual,

Time: 7735.72

if that what we're really trying to do.

Time: 7737.41

So until now, I've been weaving together studies

Time: 7738.822

from the field of experimental psychology

Time: 7741.99

and the fields of neuroscience in particular neuroimaging.

Time: 7746.17

But if you recall back to the very beginning of the episode,

Time: 7750.5

when I was discussing how odors, and how hormones,

Time: 7755.43

and how even birth control can shape people's ratings

Time: 7758.5

of attractiveness of others,

Time: 7761.79

you'll realize that there's a deeper layer to all this,

Time: 7764.71

which is that our biology that resides below our conscious

Time: 7770.31

awareness, things like our hormones,

Time: 7773.22

things like pheromones even are shaping the way

Time: 7778.07

that we choose, interpret and act with other potential

Time: 7784.23

romantic partners or the romantic partners

Time: 7786.44

that we already have.

Time: 7788.4

Now, this cannot be over-emphasized, right?

Time: 7792.68

No matter how much we would like to create

Time: 7796.06

a top-down description, meaning from the cortex

Time: 7799.91

and our understanding of things

Time: 7801.86

onto what we find attractive, who we find attractive,

Time: 7804.68

what we enjoy, what we don't enjoy in the pursuit

Time: 7807.63

in romantic interactions with others,

Time: 7810.75

there always seems to be, and indeed, there always is

Time: 7814.17

a deeper layer in which our subconscious processing

Time: 7818.12

drives us to find a particular person

Time: 7820.85

to be particularly attractive,

Time: 7822.57

or in which we have chemistry with somebody,

Time: 7826.04

or in which we lack chemistry with somebody.

Time: 7829.22

And I would say that one of the more exciting, fascinating,

Time: 7833.81

and indeed mysterious aspects of desire, love and attachment

Time: 7839.38

are those subconscious processes,

Time: 7841.39

those things that we call chemistry, right?

Time: 7844.08

I mean, people will report, for instance, that somebody's

Time: 7847.23

smell is just absolutely positively intoxicating for them,

Time: 7851.48

or that somebody's smell is absolutely repulsive to them

Time: 7853.89

and they don't know why.

Time: 7855.77

That the taste of someone's breath, and I don't mean that

Time: 7859.16

in any kind of poetic sense, I literally mean the taste

Time: 7862.51

of somebody's breath in some cases

Time: 7865

can be very exciting to somebody.

Time: 7867.34

And believe it or not, we can taste each other's breath.

Time: 7869.88

I talked about this in the chemical sensing episode

Time: 7873.46

some months back, but we actually have receptors for taste

Time: 7877.65

and smell that engage in coordinated action,

Time: 7880.35

such that we can't really separate taste and smell

Time: 7883.32

at some level.

Time: 7884.153

And this is especially true when it comes to the formation

Time: 7887.12

of romantic relationships and what we call chemistry.

Time: 7890.07

Now is chemistry absolutely required

Time: 7892.83

for forming stable attachments for love and for desire?

Time: 7896.93

No, of course, they're not.

Time: 7898.373

But in general, these are primitive mechanisms

Time: 7902.86

that exist in all animals,

Time: 7904.46

they exist in special forms in humans,

Time: 7907.38

but that they drive us toward behaviors that will,

Time: 7912.17

as the theory goes, lead to love and attachment.

Time: 7915.61

Not always, as Dr. Fisher pointed out that sex and sex drive

Time: 7920.34

is one way to explore potential love relationships,

Time: 7923.24

and to explore potential attachments,

Time: 7925.84

which, of course, are major investments

Time: 7928.48

that extend well beyond one night, or a week, or a vacation,

Time: 7932.06

or even a year.

Time: 7933.46

When we talk about stable attachments,

Time: 7935.37

in general, that means long-term attachments in humans.

Time: 7938.89

Now, there is a biology to all of that chemistry stuff.

Time: 7944.38

And the studies of oral contraception and men finding women

Time: 7948.85

more attractive at certain phases of their menstrual cycle,

Time: 7952.52

and women finding men more attractive at certain phases

Time: 7955.46

of the women's menstrual cycle point to the incredible power

Time: 7959.62

of those deeper biological mechanisms.

Time: 7963.09

In the "Huberman Lab Podcast,"

Time: 7964.67

I discuss both science and science-based tools.

Time: 7967.63

And so I'd be remiss if I didn't actually cover

Time: 7970.77

some of the tools that relate

Time: 7972.34

to those deeper biological mechanisms.

Time: 7975.16

Now, the hormones, testosterone and estrogen

Time: 7979.14

are almost always the first biological chemicals

Time: 7982.68

and hormones that are mentioned, and described, and explored

Time: 7986.61

when thinking about desire, and love and attachment, too,

Time: 7991.72

for that matter since love and attachment stem from desire.

Time: 7996.81

I did an entire episode about the biology of testosterone

Time: 8000.38

and estrogen and ways to optimize testosterone and estrogen.

Time: 8005.18

You can easily find that episode at hubermanlab.com.

Time: 8008.53

It's timestamped, there you can find all sorts

Time: 8010.62

of information about how certain behaviors or absence

Time: 8014.27

of behaviors drive up or down testosterone and estrogen.

Time: 8017.71

I also dispel some myths about sexual behavior

Time: 8021.16

and things like masturbation

Time: 8022.6

and how they relate to testosterone and estrogen,

Time: 8025.72

as well as some myths about how those hormones change

Time: 8028.52

across the lifespan.

Time: 8029.61

I also talk about the role of exercise,

Time: 8032.61

I talk about supplementation, and I also talk a little bit

Time: 8035.61

about hormone replacement therapy,

Time: 8037.13

although that will be the topic for a future episode.

Time: 8039.71

So if you're interested in the biology of testosterone

Time: 8041.97

and estrogen, two hormones that absolutely influence

Time: 8045.22

things like libido and desire, please check out that episode

Time: 8048.97

as well as what I'm going to talk about in just a moment here.

Time: 8052.26

The simple stereotyped version of the hormones,

Time: 8054.7

testosterone and estrogen are that testosterone drives

Time: 8058.76

libido or increases it aka sex drive,

Time: 8061.99

and that estrogen somehow blunts it or is not involved

Time: 8065.63

in libido and sex drive.

Time: 8066.86

And that is simply not the case.

Time: 8068.672

As I describe in that testosterone and estrogen

Time: 8071.85

optimization episode and as I'll tell you now,

Time: 8075.06

yes, testosterone and some of its other forms

Time: 8078.85

like dihydrotestosterone are strongly related to libido

Time: 8083.65

and sex drive and the pursuit and ability to mate.

Time: 8086.61

However, the hormone estrogen is also strongly associated

Time: 8091.49

with libido and mating behavior.

Time: 8093.67

So much so that for people that either chemically

Time: 8097.81

or for some other reason have very low estrogen,

Time: 8100.98

libido can severely suffer.

Time: 8102.97

So it's a coordinated dance of estrogen and testosterone

Time: 8106.29

in both males and females that leads to libido or sex drive.

Time: 8110.42

So I absolutely want to make clear that it's not a simple

Time: 8116.06

relationship between testosterone and sex drive,

Time: 8118.56

or estrogen and sex drive.

Time: 8119.61

Both are required at appropriate ratios.

Time: 8123.29

Now, with that said, there are things that can shift libido

Time: 8130.44

in both men and women in the direction of more desire

Time: 8135.39

or more desire to mate, either to seek mates

Time: 8138.63

or to mate with existing partners.

Time: 8141.75

And there's a quite solid literature

Time: 8144.35

around a few of those substances.

Time: 8146.75

Now, a common misconception is that because dopamine

Time: 8149.6

is involved in motivation and drive that simply increasing

Time: 8153.64

dopamine through any number of different mechanisms or tools

Time: 8157.47

will increase libido and sex drive.

Time: 8159.92

And that's simply not the case either.

Time: 8164.08

It is true that some level of dopamine or increase

Time: 8169.29

in dopamine is required for increases in libido.

Time: 8174.24

However, because of dopamine's relationship

Time: 8176.67

to the autonomic nervous system,

Time: 8178.43

and because the autonomic nervous system

Time: 8181.25

is so intimately involved, no pun intended,

Time: 8184.45

in sexual activity in seeking an actual mating behavior,

Time: 8188.81

as I described earlier, it's actually the case that

Time: 8191.78

if people drive their dopamine system too high,

Time: 8195.68

they will be in states of arousal that are high enough

Time: 8200.67

such that they seek and want sexual activity,

Time: 8204.43

but they can't actually engage the parasympathetic arm

Time: 8207.47

of the autonomic nervous system sufficient

Time: 8208.94

to become physically aroused.

Time: 8211.37

There's a whole description of this that awaits us

Time: 8214.56

in a future episode.

Time: 8215.58

But I'll summarize now by saying for people

Time: 8218.74

that are taking substances just simply to increase dopamine

Time: 8224.76

in order to increase libido,

Time: 8226.152

that can be a potentially hazardous route to follow.

Time: 8229.82

Because depending on whether or not that dopamine level

Time: 8233.89

is high enough that it puts them into a mode of seeking

Time: 8238.72

mates or mating, but they can't adjust their autonomic

Time: 8243.11

nervous system during actual mating behavior,

Time: 8245.49

what essentially is I'm saying is it can place people

Time: 8246.889

into a chronic pursuit but in inability to perform sexually.

Time: 8251.01

And this is true for men and women.

Time: 8253.28

Okay, so I would just caution people against just thinking,

Time: 8257.157

"Oh, a lack of libido is simply a lack of dopamine."

Time: 8260.81

That is not the case.

Time: 8261.96

It could be from lower levels of dopamine,

Time: 8264.85

but it could also be for other reasons.

Time: 8267.73

And so these systems, these signaling systems

Time: 8269.877

and these neurochemicals are very intricate.

Time: 8273.15

And just simply ramping up dopamine has actually been found,

Time: 8276.85

for instance, in amphetamine and cocaine users,

Time: 8280.1

there's a phenomenon in which they become hyper aroused,

Time: 8282.54

but can't perform sexually.

Time: 8283.76

This is also true for people who take elevated levels

Time: 8286.75

of other recreational drugs or who take antidepressants

Time: 8291.08

that increase the dopamine system too much, right?

Time: 8294.25

Dosage has to be worked out with your physician,

Time: 8297.06

with your psychiatrist, such that mood is enhanced

Time: 8301.05

and the various aspects of a healthy wellbeing,

Time: 8304.77

mind and body are enhanced, but not so much so that

Time: 8307.19

that what we call the arousal arc is locked with the seesaw

Time: 8311.45

in the sympathetic drive position

Time: 8314.56

such that sexual arousal can't occur, okay?

Time: 8317.61

So this is an important point to make,

Time: 8319.93

because I think that a lot of people

Time: 8321.87

are under the impression that

Time: 8323.242

if they just drive up testosterone, increase dopamine,

Time: 8327.13

and generally get themselves into high states

Time: 8329.28

of autonomic arousal that that's going to increase the libido.

Time: 8331.92

But that's simply not the way the system works.

Time: 8334.67

It's that seesaw and that seesawing back and forth

Time: 8337.61

that is the arc of arousal that we talked about earlier.

Time: 8341.32

Now, there are substances, legal over-the-counter substances

Time: 8346.51

that fall under the categorization of supplements

Time: 8349.29

that do indeed increase libido and arousal.

Time: 8353.17

And so I'm going to talk about some of those in the context

Time: 8355.04

of peer-reviewed literature now.

Time: 8357.66

I want to be clear, however,

Time: 8359

that these are by no means required.

Time: 8361.63

Many people have healthy libidos or have libidos

Time: 8364.33

that are healthy for their life and what they need and want.

Time: 8369.41

And as always in any discussion about supplementation,

Time: 8372.55

you absolutely have to check with your physician.

Time: 8374.39

I don't just say that to protect us.

Time: 8375.67

I say that to protect you.

Time: 8377.16

Your health and wellbeing is dependent on you doing certain

Time: 8379.47

things and not doing others, and everybody is different.

Time: 8382.12

Nonetheless, there are studies that point

Time: 8384.13

to specific substances that are sold over the counter

Time: 8387.47

that at least in the United States are legal

Time: 8389.38

and that have been shown to be statistically significant

Time: 8393.58

in increasing measures of libido.

Time: 8396.21

There are many such substances, but three that in particular

Time: 8400.97

have good peer-reviewed research to support them

Time: 8404.3

are Maca, M-A-C-A, which is actually a root.

Time: 8410.505

Tongkat Ali, also sometimes called longjack,

Time: 8413.21

I didn't name them, forgive me, and Tribulus, or Tribulus,

Time: 8417.65

it's sometimes called.

Time: 8418.85

I'm going to talk about each of these in sequence.

Time: 8421.47

But on the whole, the studies on Maca are quite convincing

Time: 8427.97

that consumption of two to three grams per day of Maca,

Time: 8433.44

which generally is sold as a powder or a capsule,

Time: 8438.27

typically consumed early in the day,

Time: 8440.05

because it can be somewhat of a stimulant,

Time: 8442.05

meaning it can increase alertness and you wouldn't want it

Time: 8444.93

to interfere with sleep by taking it too late in the day.

Time: 8448.69

But in studies that include both men and women of durations

Time: 8452.62

anywhere from eight to 12 weeks

Time: 8455.35

of athletes and non-athletes,

Time: 8457.53

and different variations of Maca,

Time: 8460.59

turns out there's black Maca, red Maca, yellow Maca.

Time: 8463.64

There are a bunch of different forms of Maca.

Time: 8466.11

But that they can increase subjective reports

Time: 8470.08

of sexual desire, independent of hormone systems.

Time: 8475.21

Meaning, it does not seem at least based on the existing

Time: 8478.37

literature that Maca increases testosterone

Time: 8481.03

or changes estrogen, at least not on the time scales

Time: 8483.41

that these studies were done,

Time: 8484.659

or with the measures that were performed in these studies.

Time: 8487.81

But that Maca, again, consumed in doses of anywhere

Time: 8491.91

from two to three grams per day,

Time: 8494.27

has been shown to significantly increase libido.

Time: 8497.55

And in fact, those dosages of Maca have been shown to offset

Time: 8503.66

so-called SSRI induced sexual dysfunction.

Time: 8507.33

So there are various routes to sexual dysfunction.

Time: 8511.1

The SSRIs are selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors.

Time: 8515.2

They go by name brands like Prozac and Zoloft,

Time: 8517.31

and there are many others now on generic forms and so forth.

Time: 8520.7

Those don't always, I should point out,

Time: 8523.66

lead to sexual dysfunction.

Time: 8524.95

There's a dose dependence.

Time: 8526.91

Some people do quite well on SSRIs and don't have any issues

Time: 8530.95

with sexual function.

Time: 8531.783

Other people suffer quite a lot from sexual dysfunction

Time: 8535.31

while taking SSRIs, highly variable.

Time: 8538.07

You need to work with a physician, a qualified psychiatrist.

Time: 8542.87

But nonetheless, everything I've been saying about Maca

Time: 8546.55

thus far has also been explored in the context of SSRI

Time: 8551.08

induced sexual dysfunction.

Time: 8552.04

The paper that I'm referring to here is

Time: 8555.547

"A Double‐Blind, Randomized, Pilot Dose‐Finding Study

Time: 8557.87

of Maca Root..."

Time: 8559.06

It goes by the name L. meyenii.

Time: 8561.95

These always have fancy names.

Time: 8562.987

And the Latin names in biology are always more complicated,

Time: 8565.82

but it's Maca root.

Time: 8566.977

"for the Management of SSRI‐Induced Sexual Dysfunction."

Time: 8569.92

First author is Dording, D-O-R-D-I-N-G.

Time: 8573.68

This was a study done at Mass General,

Time: 8576.09

which is one of the satellite locations around Harvard Med.

Time: 8580.7

It's associated with Harvard Med,

Time: 8582.78

that found significant improvements in libido

Time: 8586.98

when people were taking a pretty low dose.

Time: 8588.92

It was actually in this case, just 1.5 grams per day,

Time: 8592.84

up to a high dose, three grams per day of Maca.

Time: 8597.15

And they were doing this

Time: 8597.983

in 20 remitted depressed outpatients.

Time: 8600.6

So these are people that had depression,

Time: 8601.843

their depression was successfully treated with SSRIs,

Time: 8605.04

but they were suffering from some of these SSRI related

Time: 8607.86

sexual effects and Maca seemed to offset

Time: 8610.17

some of those effects significantly in this population.

Time: 8613.72

The other studies exploring the lack of effect on serum

Time: 8617.97

testosterone in adult healthy men was a 12-week study,

Time: 8622.77

again, consuming anywhere from 1.5 to three milligrams,

Time: 8626.69

meaning one, excuse me, 1500 milligrams

Time: 8629.13

to 3000 milligrams or placebo.

Time: 8631.27

So again, this is 1.5 up to three grams of Maca or placebo,

Time: 8636.5

and they rated sexual desire, depression, and other measures

Time: 8642.5

such as testosterone in the blood.

Time: 8644.65

Again, no change in testosterone or estrogen

Time: 8648.54

estradiol levels in men treated with Maca

Time: 8651.06

and those treated with placebo.

Time: 8652.58

But nonetheless, there was a significant and positive effect

Time: 8656.6

on libido with this dosage of 1.5

Time: 8659.44

to three grams per day of Maca.

Time: 8660.877

And there are several other studies that also show this.

Time: 8664.95

Again, in people that taking SSRIs and people that are not

Time: 8669.37

taking SSRIs, in chronically over-trained athletes,

Time: 8675.02

this was also found to be the case.

Time: 8676.22

So seems like across the board,

Time: 8678.91

Maca is a fairly useful supplement

Time: 8682.06

for those that are seeking to increase their libido.

Time: 8684.69

And there are fewer studies involving women,

Time: 8687.47

but there are a few such studies that also point to the same

Time: 8691.07

general positive effect on libido in women taking Maca

Time: 8694.68

at equivalent doses to those I just described.

Time: 8697.46

I think it's noteworthy that Maca supplementation

Time: 8700.15

does not seem to adjust testosterone or estrogen levels

Time: 8704.22

to any significant degree, but it does change libido.

Time: 8707.47

I think that points to the fact that there are multiple

Time: 8710.35

systems in the brain and body that influence libido,

Time: 8713.06

not just testosterone and estrogen.

Time: 8715.2

And indeed we know that to be the case.

Time: 8716.5

Things like PEA, which is a substance found in chocolate

Time: 8721.08

and is a substance that some people supplement,

Time: 8723.72

is known, for instance, to increase sexual desire,

Time: 8727.13

but also the perception of sexual experiences

Time: 8730.26

as more stimulating for instance.

Time: 8732.7

So there are a lot of pathways in the brain, in particular,

Time: 8734.97

in the hypothalamus, this ancient area of our brain,

Time: 8738.68

that harbors neurons and hormone secreting cells,

Time: 8742.55

including neurons that can shape our perceptions

Time: 8745.85

of even just our tactile experience

Time: 8749.21

of others and their attractiveness.

Time: 8751.82

And indeed can shift levels of desire, independent

Time: 8755.27

of changing levels of circulating hormones.

Time: 8758.17

Another substance that has been shown to increase libido

Time: 8761.17

across a range of human populations

Time: 8764.12

is so-called Tongkat Ali.

Time: 8766.83

I've talked a little bit about this before

Time: 8769.11

on the "Huberman Lab Podcast" in reference to testosterone.

Time: 8773.42

And I've talked about it extensively as a guest

Time: 8775.38

on other podcasts.

Time: 8776.64

Tongkat Ali goes by a number of different names.

Time: 8780.42

One of them is exceedingly difficult for me to pronounce.

Time: 8782.69

It's Eurycoma longifolia, also called longjack.

Time: 8787.86

But Tongkat Ali is the typical name.

Time: 8790.43

This is an herb, there's a Malaysian version

Time: 8792.8

and an Indonesian version.

Time: 8794.92

My understanding is that the Indonesian variety

Time: 8797.53

of Tongkat Ali is the one that is most potent

Time: 8800.01

for its effects on libido.

Time: 8803.91

Previously, I've talked about Tongkat Ali

Time: 8806.32

taken in 400 milligram per day capsules

Time: 8809.75

as a means to increase the amount of free,

Time: 8813.19

meaning unbound testosterone.

Time: 8815.26

So testosterone has a both bound form and an unbound form.

Time: 8818.97

Very briefly, the bound form is bound to albumin

Time: 8822.5

in the blood or to so-called sex hormone binding globulin.

Time: 8826.47

When it's bound, it can't be biologically active

Time: 8830.65

at many cells.

Time: 8831.99

It is important that some of it be bound in order

Time: 8834.22

to get a time release and proper distribution

Time: 8837.3

of testosterone through the body,

Time: 8838.45

but is the unbound free testosterone that can really

Time: 8841.58

have its most potent effects.

Time: 8843.51

And there's some evidence that Tongkat Ali can increase

Time: 8847.38

the amount of unbound so-called free testosterone

Time: 8850.67

by lowering sex hormone binding globulin,

Time: 8853.22

although it is almost certain that it has other routes

Time: 8856.31

of mechanism as well.

Time: 8858.6

Nonetheless, there are some reports of Tongkat Ali

Time: 8862.76

increasing libido.

Time: 8865.11

One particular article, last author, or I should say

Time: 8869.02

last name of first author, excuse me, Ismail, I-S-M-A-I-L.

Time: 8873.52

This was published in Evidence-Based Complementary

Time: 8877.71

and Alternative Medicine, it's from 2012.

Time: 8880.21

Reports a significant increase in libido

Time: 8883.85

and sexual function.

Time: 8885.17

There are other studies, not a lot of them,

Time: 8887.95

not as many robust, controlled, quality peer-reviewed

Time: 8892.54

studies as there are from Maca.

Time: 8894.17

Nonetheless, a number of people, men and women that I know

Time: 8897.55

do take Tongkat Ali and it seems to work well for them.

Time: 8901.22

The question always comes up around

Time: 8902.96

discussion of supplements.

Time: 8903.92

Do you need to cycle these things?

Time: 8905.46

The only way to determine that is really

Time: 8907.23

to do your blood work, monitor liver enzymes,

Time: 8909.96

monitor hormone levels and so forth.

Time: 8912.21

So I simply can't say whether or not you need to,

Time: 8915.16

or you don't need to cycle them.

Time: 8916.355

Typically Tongkat Ali and Maca are not cycled

Time: 8920.97

in any regular kind of way that I'm aware of.

Time: 8923.64

But again, you really need to check with your doctor

Time: 8926.33

if you're going to initiate taking any of these things.

Time: 8929.26

And you certainly should do your best to monitor

Time: 8931.44

your blood work as well as subjective measures in evaluating

Time: 8934.33

whether or not they're working for you,

Time: 8935.46

safe for you and so forth.

Time: 8936.77

The third and final substance/supplement

Time: 8939.32

that I want to touch on as it relates to libido

Time: 8942

is called Tribulus Terrestris.

Time: 8944.77

So that's T-R-I-B-U-L-U-S, terrestris, T-E-R-R-E-S-T-R-I-S.

Time: 8952.41

This is a commonly sold over-the-counter supplement

Time: 8956.39

for increasing testosterone,

Time: 8959.02

for fitness purposes and so on.

Time: 8962.62

Whether or not it actually does that

Time: 8964.69

to a meaningful degree, isn't clear.

Time: 8967.71

But I'm aware of four peer-reviewed studies

Time: 8972.1

that were focused on both males and females

Time: 8976.76

ranging anywhere from 18 years old

Time: 8979.84

all the way up to 65 plus.

Time: 8982.13

They say 65 plus.

Time: 8983.25

I guess it could be 70, it could be 80.

Time: 8985.09

I don't know.

Time: 8985.923

But a fairly broad age range where people took anywhere

Time: 8990.67

from 750 milligrams per day divided into three equal doses.

Time: 8997.43

So 750 total per day divided into three equal doses

Time: 9001.37

of Tribulus or placebo for 120 days.

Time: 9005.44

This particular study was focused on females.

Time: 9008.75

And according to the female sexual function index

Time: 9011.59

questionnaire, no significant difference

Time: 9013.98

between any of the groups, however free and bio-available

Time: 9018.54

testosterone increased in the group taking

Time: 9019.82

Tribulus Terrestris, total testosterone

Time: 9023.27

did not reach statistical significance.

Time: 9025.29

So this is the inverse of what we see with Maca,

Time: 9028.46

where there do seem to be increases in testosterone,

Time: 9031.16

which would predict that there would be increase in libido.

Time: 9034.76

In this case, this was postmenopausal women,

Time: 9037.93

there was no increase in libido.

Time: 9039.24

There was an increase in testosterone.

Time: 9041.15

I mentioned it only because there might be instances

Time: 9043.7

in which people want to increase their testosterone.

Time: 9047.77

It does seem that Tribulus, at least in that population

Time: 9050.25

is capable of doing that.

Time: 9051.86

Now there's a separate study that was done,

Time: 9054.47

a double-blind study lasting anywhere from one to six months

Time: 9058.44

that had a clear and significant increase in libido.

Time: 9062.35

Now this was taking six grams.

Time: 9065.08

So that's 6,000 milligrams of Tribulus root for 60 days.

Time: 9069.35

And it did seem to increase various aspects

Time: 9071.74

of sexual function.

Time: 9073.95

And there was what appeared to be a substantial 16.3%

Time: 9078.6

increase in testosterone, but in this particular study

Time: 9081.8

because of the variability across individuals

Time: 9084.67

that did not actually arrive at statistical significance.

Time: 9087.62

Now, there were a number of other studies that explored

Time: 9090.738

the role of Tribulus, in particular, in females.

Time: 9094.67

And one of those studies was a study

Time: 9097.66

that was actually quite short, it was two to four weeks.

Time: 9100.71

It involved 67 subjects.

Time: 9103.07

These were subjects that had experienced a loss of libido

Time: 9106.53

and took Tribulus divided into two equal doses,

Time: 9110.07

compared that to placebo.

Time: 9111.64

And they did see a significant improvement in these measures

Time: 9116.13

of sexual desire and function

Time: 9118.07

on this female sexual function index.

Time: 9120.44

So there is some evidence that Tribulus can be effective

Time: 9123.21

in increasing testosterone in certain populations,

Time: 9126.11

in increasing sexual desire and function

Time: 9128.79

in certain populations in particular in females.

Time: 9132.03

I think more studies are certainly needed.

Time: 9133.95

But these three substances/supplements,

Time: 9137.91

Maca, Tongkat Ali, in particular, Indonesian Tongkat Ali,

Time: 9142.6

and Tribulus can indeed create significant

Time: 9146.26

increases in sexual desire.

Time: 9148.48

And in some cases by adjusting the testosterone

Time: 9152.06

and estrogen system, in some cases not by adjusting

Time: 9155.04

the testosterone and estrogen system.

Time: 9156.64

Again, pointing to the complexity of neurochemicals

Time: 9160.69

and features that adjust things like libido aka desire.

Time: 9164.66

So we covered a lot of material today related

Time: 9167.28

to desire, love, and attachment.

Time: 9169.13

And yet I acknowledge that it is not exhaustive

Time: 9172.64

of the vast landscape that is the psychology and biology

Time: 9176.8

of desire, love and attachment.

Time: 9179.14

Nonetheless, I hope that you found the information

Time: 9182.23

interesting and hopefully actionable in some cases

Time: 9185.81

toward the relationships of your past, of present

Time: 9188.91

and potentially for the relationships of your future.

Time: 9193.24

If you're enjoying and or learning from this podcast,

Time: 9195.71

please subscribe to our YouTube channel.

Time: 9197.67

That's a very straightforward, zero cost way to support us.

Time: 9200.71

And it really does help us.

Time: 9202.21

In addition, please subscribe to our podcast

Time: 9204.15

on Apple and Spotify.

Time: 9206.2

And on Apple, you have the opportunity to leave us

Time: 9208.51

up to a five star review.

Time: 9210.48

In addition, please leave us comments, feedback,

Time: 9213.06

and suggestions for future guests

Time: 9215.22

in the comment section on YouTube.

Time: 9217.16

We do read all of those comments.

Time: 9219.25

Please also check out the sponsors mentioned

Time: 9221.06

at the beginning of today's episode.

Time: 9222.85

That is the best way to support the "Huberman Lab Podcast."

Time: 9225.95

In addition, we have a Patreon,

Time: 9227.54

it's patreon.com/andrewhuberman.

Time: 9230.54

And there you can support the podcast

Time: 9232.54

at any level that you like.

Time: 9234.239

Please also follow us on Instagram and Twitter.

Time: 9237.4

I teach neuroscience and neuroscience-related tools

Time: 9239.81

on both Instagram and Twitter.

Time: 9241.31

Some of that material overlaps

Time: 9243.04

with material covered on the podcast.

Time: 9244.96

Some of it is unique material only covered

Time: 9246.97

on Instagram and Twitter.

Time: 9248.51

During today's episode and on many previous episodes

Time: 9250.82

of the "Huberman Lab Podcast," we discussed supplements.

Time: 9254.05

While supplements aren't necessary or appropriate

Time: 9256.27

for everybody, many people derive tremendous benefit

Time: 9259.08

from them, for things like enhancing sleep, enhancing focus,

Time: 9262.76

or as discussed today for enhancing libido and desire.

Time: 9266.64

If you want to see the supplements that I take,

Time: 9268.36

you can go to Thorne, that's thorne.com/u/huberman.

Time: 9274.29

And there you can get 20% off the supplements that I take.

Time: 9278.11

And if you navigate deeper into the Thorne site

Time: 9280.31

through that portal, thorne.com/u/huberman,

Time: 9284.94

you can also get 20% off any of the other supplements

Time: 9287.87

that Thorne makes.

Time: 9288.92

We partnered with Thorne because Thorne has the absolute

Time: 9291.37

highest standards with respect to the quality

Time: 9293.95

of the ingredients in their supplements

Time: 9295.46

and the precision of the amounts of those supplements.

Time: 9298.24

Thank you for joining me for today's discussion

Time: 9300.56

about desire, love, and attachment.

Time: 9303.05

And last but certainly not least,

Time: 9305.27

thank you for your interest in science.

Time: 9306.103

[upbeat music]

Copyright © 2024. All rights reserved.