The Science of Love, Desire and Attachment
- Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,
where we discuss science and science-based tools
for everyday life.
[upbeat music]
I'm Andrew Huberman,
and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology
at Stanford School of Medicine.
Today, we are going to talk about the psychology
and the biology of desire, love and attachment.
Today happens to be Valentine's day, 2022.
However, the themes we are going to discuss
pertain to desire, love and attachment on any given day.
And indeed the mechanisms we are going to discuss
almost certainly were at play thousands of years ago,
hundreds of years ago, and no doubt will still be at play
in our minds, and in our bodies, and in our psychologies
for the decades, centuries and thousands of years to come.
Indeed today, I want to focus on core mechanisms
that lead individuals to seek out other individuals
with whom to mate with,
with whom to have children with or not,
with whom to enter short
or long-term relationships with,
and for perhaps to end those relationships
or to seek relationships on the side, so-called infidelity.
I'm certainly not going to encourage or discourage
any of these behaviors.
I'm simply going to cover the peer-reviewed scientific data
on all these aspects of desire, love, and attachment.
I'm going to discuss how our childhood attachment styles,
as they're called, influence our adult attachment styles.
Yes, you heard that right.
How we attached or did not attach to primary caregivers
in our childhood has much to do with how we attach
or fail to attach to romantic partners as adults,
because the same neural circuits,
the neurons and their connections in the brain and body
that underlie attachment between infant and caregiver,
between toddler and parent or other caregiver,
and during adolescence and in our teenage years
are repurposed for adult romantic attachments.
I know that might be a little eerie to think about,
but indeed that is true.
Now the fortunate thing is that regardless of our childhood
attachment styles and experiences, the neural circuits
for desire, love and attachment are quite plastic.
They are amenable to change in response to both
what we think and what we feel as well as what we do.
However, all three aspects that we're discussing today,
desire, love and attachment
are also strongly biologically driven.
We're going to talk about biological mechanisms
such as hormones,
biological mechanisms such as neurochemicals,
things like dopamine, oxytocin and serotonin,
and neural circuits, brain areas, and indeed areas
of the body that interact with the brain that control
whether or not we desire somebody or not,
whether or not we lose or increase our desire
for somebody over time, whether or not we fall in love,
what love means, and whether or not the relationships
we form continue to include the elements
of desire and love over time or not.
In order to illustrate just how powerfully our biology
can shape our perception of the attractiveness
of other people, I want to share with you the results
of a couple of studies.
Both studies explore
how people rate other people's attractiveness.
And in both studies, the major variable is that women
are at different stages of their menstrual cycle.
Now in the first study, men are rating the attractiveness
of women according to the smell of those women.
Now they're not smelling them directly.
They're smelling clothing that women wore for a couple
of days at different phases of their menstrual cycle.
And what they find is that men will rate the odors of women
as most attractive if those women wore
those shirts, that clothing,
in the pre-ovulatory phase of their cycle, okay?
So this is not to say that men do not find women attractive
at other stages of their cycle.
It is to say that men find women's odors
particularly attractive, if those odors were worn by women
that are in the pre-ovulatory phase
of their menstrual cycle, okay?
Now, there was also a study that was done where women
at different stages of their menstrual cycle
are rating the odors of men.
And a similar but mirror symmetric result was found
such that women who are in the pre-ovulatory phase
of their menstrual cycle will rate men's odors
as more attractive than at other stages of their cycle.
So the simple way to put this is that there seems
to be something special about the pre-ovulatory phase
of a woman's menstrual cycle that makes men rate them
as more attractive during that time and women rate men
as more attractive during that particular time as well.
So this is a bidirectional effect.
The way that the second study was done,
where women are rating men was not just to smell the odors
of those men on t-shirts, they did that,
but they correlated that with whether or not
the shirts were worn by men
that were particularly physically symmetrical.
They actually had these men divided into groups.
It was more of a continuum rather,
rated according to body symmetry and face symmetry.
And women preferred more symmetrical men
when they were doing the preference test
during the pre-ovulatory phase of their cycle.
So again, the point is that that pre-ovulatory
phase of the cycle seems to create
a bidirectional mutual attractiveness.
Now, also extremely interesting is that this effect
does really seem to have something to do with ovulation.
Because in both studies,
they had women that were taking oral contraception or not.
And what they found was if a woman is taking
oral contraception, it prevented that peak and perceived
attractiveness by the men, meaning men no longer perceived
a woman to be more attractive
at a particular phase of their cycle.
And also women taking oral contraception no longer preferred
the odors of more symmetrical men
during the pre-ovulatory phase of their cycle.
Now, I want to make sure that it's especially clear
that it is not the case that oral contraception reduced
the perception of a woman as attractive,
that did not happen in these studies.
It reduced the further increase in a male's perception
of her as attractive.
And if women took oral contraception,
it prevented them from preferring more symmetrical men
based on the odors of those men.
Now I realize there are a lot of variables here.
We've got odors, we've got symmetry,
we've got menstrual cycle, pre-ovulatory, non pre-ovulatory.
And we have whether or not people
are taking contraception or not.
But the basic finding is that depending on where women are
in their menstrual cycle, influences both men's perception
of them as attractive and their perception of men
as attractive and oral contraception eliminates that effect.
So I share with you those data to illustrate that
we often think that somebody is attractive or not based on,
I don't know, how they look,
their skin, their hair, et cetera,
but it also illustrates that their odor is a powerful cue
for some people more than others.
Some of us tend to be more factory driven than others.
Although if you watched the "Huberman Lab Podcast" episode
that I did with Professor David Buss
from the University of Texas, Austin,
who's a luminary in the field of evolutionary psychology
and has studied mate choice
and mate selection bias over decades,
he's really one of the founders of that field,
he emphasized findings that odor for many people
is a maker or a deal breaker.
Meaning, there are some people that even if somebody has
all the characteristics that they're looking for in terms
of kindness, and attractiveness, and values,
and other features that would and should be
of very high priority in selecting a mate,
that if someone does not like the way that person smells,
their innate body odor independent of colognes,
and perfumes, and soaps, et cetera,
that that's often a complete and total deal breaker.
I'm sure there are some of you that can relate to that.
And there's some of you perhaps for which that is
not the case, and you can't even imagine that being
such a powerful variable.
And yet the data suggested indeed it is a powerful variable
for many people out there.
Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast
is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.
It is however, part of my desire and effort
to bring zero cost to consumer information about science
and science-related tools to the general public.
In keeping with that theme,
I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.
Our first sponsor is Thesis.
Thesis makes custom nootropics.
Now nootropics is not a word that I'm usually a fan of,
because nootropic means smart drug.
And there are a lot of different aspects
to being smart or to intelligence.
There's the ability to focus.
There's the ability to task switch.
There's the ability to be creative.
And each of those different forms of intelligence
or expressions of intelligence involves
different brain circuits, different neurochemicals,
and indeed different hormones in the body.
So I think it's rather naive to think that
there could be one smart drug or nootropic.
Thesis understands this.
And so they've developed custom nootropics that are tailored
to the specific goals that you might have
in terms of cognitive work, or physical exercise,
or skill learning of any kind.
And they tailor them to the individual.
They only use the highest quality ingredients,
many of which I've talked about here on the podcast.
Things like alpha GPC, which I personally use,
things like phosphatidylserine,
which I also use on occasion.
However, they've taken different combinations
of those ingredients and put them into different
formulations designed for particular goals or endpoints.
For instance, I have Thesis nootropics
that are for motivation, others that are for clarity,
for mental clarity, others that I take pre-workout,
others that I take post-workout, and so on and so forth.
Now, in addition to that Thesis understands that
not every ingredient is terrific for everybody.
In fact, one particular substance that I've talked about
on the podcast, Ginkgo biloba is very useful
for a lot of people.
However, I don't tolerate it well.
I get vicious headaches from Ginkgo biloba.
So none of the formulations that they've made
for me include Ginkgo biloba.
So again, these are custom nootropics that are tailored
for your particular goals,
and where the ingredients really match
your particular needs,
and don't include things that aren't going to work for you,
or that create things like headaches.
So the personalization and the targeted effects
of the nootropics is really where or the power comes from.
I've been using Thesis nootropics
for close to six months now.
And I can confidently say that their nootropics
have been a total game changer for me.
My go-to formula is the motivation formula.
If I'm going to be training, or working out,
or I need extra energy,
and I use the clarity formula nowadays
for any kind of writing or creative work.
Those are the two that I've mainly been taking lately.
If you want to try your own personalized
nootropic starter kit, you can go online
to takethesis.com/huberman.
You'll take a three minute quiz and Thesis will send you
four different formulas to try in your first month.
You'll have the ability to try those different blends
across that month, discover which nootropics
work best for you, your unique brain chemistry in genetics.
You can have a consult with them,
and then they will give you the best nootropics for you.
Again, that's takethesis.com/huberman
and use the code Huberman at checkout
to gain 10% off your first box.
Today's episode is also brought to us by Athletic Greens.
Now called AG1.
I've been taking AG1 since 2012.
So I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast.
The reason I started taking AG1
and the reason I still take AG1 once or twice a day
is that it covers all of my vitamin mineral probiotic needs.
Probiotics are essential because they support what's called
a healthy gut microbiome.
The gut microbiome is vital for things like
metabolism, hormone function,
and also we now know our brain function,
things like focus and memory and our general immune system.
With AG1, I get the probiotics I need,
I get the vitamins and minerals that I need
to cover any nutritional gaps, if I'm not eating optimally.
And even if I am eating optimally, AG1 can further support
metabolism, hormone function, et cetera.
In fact, whenever people ask me,
what's the one supplement that I should take
if I can only take one supplement?
I always say AG1.
I take mine early in the day.
I mix it with water and some lemon or lime juice.
I love the way it tastes.
And I'll take it again later in the day,
typically in the late afternoon.
If you'd like to try Athletic Greens,
you can go to athleticgreens.com/huberman
and claim a special offer.
They're giving you five free travel packs
that make it really easy to mix up AG1
while you're on the road, in the car,
on the plane, et cetera, and a year supply of vitamin D3 K2.
There's a ton of data now supporting the fact that
vitamin D3 is critical and that most of us don't get enough
vitamin D3 even if we're getting ample sunlight.
Vitamin D three is important from metabolism,
hormone function, brain function,
and many other aspects of our biology.
So again, if you go to athleticgreens.com/huberman,
you can get a special offer of the Athletic Greens,
five free travel packs and the year supply of vitamin D3 K2.
Today's episode is also brought to us by Inside Tracker.
Inside Tracker is a personalized nutrition platform
that analyzes data from your blood and DNA
to help you better understand your body
and help you reach your health goals.
I've long been a believer
in getting regular blood work done.
For the simple reason that many of the factors
that influence your immediate and long-term health
can only be discovered from a quality blood test.
And nowadays with the advent of modern DNA tests,
you can also get information for instance about
how your biological age compares to your chronological age,
which of course is a vital measurement.
Now, one of the major issues with blood tests and DNA tests
out there is that people get the information back
that lipid marker of one type, it might be high or low,
or that a hormone of another type might be high or low,
but they don't give you any for information
about what to do with that information.
Inside Tracker makes that all very easy to navigate.
Once you get your results back,
you can click on any of those results.
And Inside Tracker will immediately show you things
that you can do, for instance, with your nutrition,
or supplementation, or lifestyle factors
to help you bring those numbers into the ranges
that are appropriate for you.
So it's immensely powerful, not just in terms
of the measurements, but also it provides some directives
that can help bring those measurements into the ranges
that are best for your immediate and long-term health.
If you'd like to try Inside Tracker,
you can visit insidetracker.com/huberman
to get 20% off any of Inside Tracker's plans.
Just use the code Huberman at checkout.
Let's talk about desire, love, and attachment.
And of course, these are topics
that grab tremendous interest.
So it's worth us defining our terms a little bit
before going any further.
Of course, we can have many different kinds of loves.
There's romantic love, there's love of family,
so-called familial love, there's love of pets.
We can even love objects,
where we can feel as if we love objects.
We can love certain activities.
We can have friends that we love and so on and so forth.
The word love is used to encompass
a lot of different types of relationships.
Today, we are mainly going to be focused on romantic love
and the neural mechanisms of romantic love.
I want to acknowledge here at the outset
that most of the studies of romantic love
have focused on monogamous heterosexual love.
And also when we talk about studies focused on desire,
and attractiveness, and attachment, that's also the case.
And that simply reflects the general bias of the literature
over the last 50 to 100 years.
It does, of course, not rule out that similar
or different mechanisms could be at play
in non-monogamous relationships,
in homosexual relationships,
or in relationships of any kind or variation.
It's also worth us defining our terms around desire.
It can mean lust, it can mean the desire
for long-term partnership.
So we need to define our terms and throughout I will do
my best to very carefully define what I mean by desire,
what I mean by love, and what I mean by attachment.
The formal study of love, and desire, and attachment
goes back to the early 1900s.
One of the classic studies on this is entitled
"Love and Desire."
It was published in 1912 and really focused
on two opposing themes within romance.
One is love which in that paper was really meant to include
attachment and dependence or interdependence
between individuals, right?
And the other end of the spectrum being desire,
or the sexual desire for another.
And romance was meant to encapsulate both those things,
love and desire.
And for much of the 1900s, it was thought that love
and desire were on opposing ends or in a push pull.
And it was the dynamic push and pull between love and desire
that one could define romance.
And that actually led to much of what's out there
in the psychological literature.
Today, we are going to explore some neurobiological studies,
some studies of the endocrine system,
meaning the hormone system
that actually support that general model.
And I'll point you toward what I think is a very useful book
in thinking about how relationships can both form and last
over long periods of time and how those relationships
can include both desire and interdependence.
I'll also talk about some studies that have really focused
on why relationships succeed and why they fail,
and how that relates to whether or not there is sufficient
amounts of attachment and desire.
So today we're going to talk about the science
and indeed you'll also get some tools.
Those tools should be useful to you, whether or not
you happen to be in a relationship or not,
whether or not you're seeking a relationship or not.
I'd like to begin with an anecdote.
And this is not an anecdote
about my relationship history,
it's an anecdote about my scientific history.
When I started graduate school,
the chairman of the department I was in at the time
said to me, "Most PhDs last longer than most marriages."
And indeed he was right.
And also most marriages in this country end in divorce.
I think it's about 50% with a slight skew toward more ending
in divorce than persist until death do them part.
But nonetheless, it's about half,
and most marriages end before the eight-year period is up.
Most PhDs take anywhere from four to nine years.
So there was a bit of a smearing of averages there,
but the point he was trying to make
really landed home for me.
It did not scare me out of relationships
nor did it scare me out of a PhD, obviously.
What it did illustrate was that there's something
about our attachment machinery
that can be very, very compelling,
such that people take on tremendous levels of commitment.
I have to imagine that most people enter marriages
assuming that they're going to stay in those marriages.
I don't think most people enter marriages thinking
they're going to get divorced.
But that if 50% of those commitments end in divorce,
there must also be mechanisms
by which our attachments can break.
And today we're going to talk about both the forming
of attachments and the breaking of attachments,
what can prevent those breaks in attachments,
and indeed what can lead to re-attachments.
There are biological mechanisms to desire, love
and attachment, that's abundantly clear.
Now there's a robust and very large literature
in animal models.
What I mean by that are field studies and laboratory studies
in primates of different kinds,
such as macaque monkeys or bonobos.
People have looked at these sorts of things,
believe it or not, in ducks, in laboratory mice,
in different types of birds, et cetera.
And if you look at that literature, you can essentially find
biological examples in the animal kingdom for just about
any behavior that you can easily map to human behavior.
So for instance, there's a species of animal called
the prairie vole.
In one portion of the United States,
this prairie vole species is monogamous.
They only mate with one other prairie vole,
only raise the young with one other prairie vole
for their entire life.
And in another region of the United States,
the same species of animal, prairie vole,
will mate with many individuals, they're non-monogamous.
And the major difference, at least as far as we know
between the prairie voles in one location
and another location is the levels of a molecule
called vasopressin in the brain and body.
Vasopressin is present in humans.
It has numerous biological roles.
It's responsible, for instance, for controlling
the amount of urine that you excrete,
the amount of water that you retain,
and for sexual desire, as well as mate seeking.
Levels of vasopressin in prairie voles
are strongly determinant of whether or not a prairie vole
is going to be a monogamous or non-monogamous.
Why do I raise this?
Well, I raise this because the literature on prairie voles
is quite beautiful and has been discussed quite a lot
in the popular press.
You can look it up with an easily, just Web Edge search.
You'll find lots of information about this,
lots of news articles about this,
and lots of interpretations as to how vasopressin might
be involved in similar or different mechanisms in humans.
Now, I don't have a problem
with mapping animal studies to humans.
I think there's certainly a place for that.
But if we just lean back and look at the giant mass
of studies in animals and their mating behavior,
and their mate selection behavior,
you can essentially find examples of anything.
You can find examples of polygamy,
you can find examples of cheating, of infidelity,
you can find examples of all sorts of different behaviors
that in your own mind, you can map to human behavior.
But it's really hard to make the leap from animal models
to humans in any kind of direct way.
And so thankfully there's been tremendous work done
in the last mainly 20 years or so,
looking at human mate selection,
human desire, human love and human attachment.
So we're mainly going to focus on those studies today,
and where appropriate, we will map those findings back
to the findings in animals to see if there's some universal
truths or some universal principles
about how the neural circuits
and biological mechanisms work.
But by and large, we're going to focus on human studies today.
So unless I say otherwise, the data that I'm referring
to today are entirely from human beings.
So let's talk about attachment and attachment styles.
And this will offer you the opportunity
to answer some important questions for yourself,
such as what is my, meaning,
your attachment style in relationship?
One of the most robust findings in the field of psychology
is this notion of attachment styles.
And this was something that was discovered
through a beautiful set of studies that were done
by Mary Ainsworth in the 1980s,
in which she developed a laboratory condition
called the strange situation task.
Now, the strange situation task has been studied
over and over again in different cultures,
in different locations throughout the world.
And in preparing for this episode,
I actually spoke to three different psychologists.
I spoke to a psychoanalyst,
I spoke to a cognitive behavioral psychologist,
and I actually spoke to a psychiatrist, excuse me,
not a psychologist, but a psychiatrist with a medical degree
and asked, is the strange situation task
and the various attachment styles that emerge
from that task, are those still considered valid?
And indeed all three of them said if ever there was
a literature in psychology that is absolutely tamped down
and has a firm basis in both data and real world principles
and real world examples,
it's this notion of attachment styles.
So what is the strange situation task?
The strange situation task involves a parent,
typically a mother in the studies that were done,
but a parent or other caregiver bringing their child,
their actual child into a laboratory.
And there's a room with a stranger and the mother enters
the room with the child and there are some toys in the room.
And typically the mother and the stranger will talk.
Obviously the stranger is part of the experiment,
is not just some random person off the street.
And the child is allowed to move about the room.
They can observe the mother interacting
with the other person or not,
they can play with toys or not,
but then at some point the mother leaves.
And then at some point later designated by the experimenter,
the mother comes back.
And what is measured in these studies is both how the child,
the toddler reacts to the mother leaving,
and how the child reacts to the mother returning
at the end of the experiment.
And oftentimes this will have two or three different phases
where the mother will bring the child in then leave,
then come back in and leave.
There are also studies in which the behavior of the child
with the stranger is also examined.
So there are a lot of variations of this,
but the basic findings are that toddlers, children
fall into four different categories of attachment style.
And that these attachment styles can predict many features
of adolescent, teen, young adult, and even adult
attachment styles, not in strange situations
of the sort that I just ascribed,
but in romantic attachments.
I should mention also that attachment style is plastic,
meaning it can change across the lifespan.
So as I describe the results, I describe the different
attachment styles that are out there,
and if any of those resonate with you or bring to mind
certain people in your life, please do not assume that
those attachment styles are rigid and fixed
for the entire lifespan.
There are also terrific data that indicate that
through specific processes, both psychological
and some biological adjustments
that people can change their attachment style,
and that indeed people who have different attachment styles
can change the attachment styles of others.
But just to make very clear what the results
of the study were, I want to review what the four different
attachment styles are, and typically people fall
into one group or another but not several.
So the four patterns of attachment that were revealed
by these studies, again, were revealed by examining
the behavior of the child in response to the mother leaving
and the mother returning, and the child's response
to the stranger that is in the room with them.
The first style is the so-called secure attachment style.
In the nomenclature of this kind of study,
these are the so-called B babies,
as in the letter B, bulldog, B.
Not for bulldogs, but just to designate this category.
The secure attachment style is one in which the child
will engage with the stranger, with the experimenter
while the parent is present in the room.
But that when the parent, typically it's a mother,
but when the parent or other caregiver leaves,
the child does get visibly upset.
They might whine, they might cry,
they might even tantrum a bit.
However, when the caregiver, meaning the mother, or father,
or other caregiver returns, the child visibly expresses
happiness that the caregiver has returned, okay?
So that's the hallmark of the secure attachment style.
And again, this is all pre-verbal.
This is happening long before the child can express
how they feel with words.
And the interpretation of this is that the secure child
feels confident that the caregiver is available
and will be responsive to their needs
and their communications.
So that when the child whines and, or is distressed,
the parent doesn't come right back into the room.
But at some point they do and they seem to have a sense
of trust that if the parent or caregiver leaves
that the parent will come back,
and that they're happy that they do.
These children are also very good at exploring
novel environments after the parent is gone,
and while the parent is there.
And almost always, when the parent is there,
they will explore more broadly,
literally in space, they'll venture out further
than they will when the parent is gone.
They also tend to engage with the caregiver in a way
that's not immediately and completely trusting,
but that over time seems to evolve from one
in which they're kind of suspicious of this person
to one in which they're at least somewhat trusting, okay?
So these were the general contours
of the secure attachment style.
And fortunately, nowadays there are physiological studies
measuring things like heart rate and breathing
and other measures that correlate with the subjective
assessment of what these children are feeling.
Okay, so first category is secure attached.
The second category is a so-called anxious-avoidant
or insecurely attached, which are the category A babies.
The children with anxious-avoidant insecure
attachment patterns generally tend to avoid
or ignore the caregiver, all right?
Meaning the parent, and show very little emotion
when the parent leaves or returns.
So this is the reason they call them avoidant
or anxious-avoidant and kind of insecure.
There isn't this happiness or joy that the parent is back.
They don't seem to express that.
They do not exhibit distress on separation.
And they generally tend to have some tendency to approach
the caregiver when they return,
but there doesn't seem to be a general expression of joy.
And again, physiological measures support that as well.
Things like changes in heart rate tend to be less dramatic
in the anxious-avoidant insecure attachment style
than in the secure attachment style.
Okay, so that's the second one.
The third category is the so-called
anxious-ambivalent/resistant-insecure category.
Okay, I didn't name these categories.
So you have to blame others in this one instance.
For everything else blame me, but in this instance,
you have to blame the psychologists
that named this category.
The anxious-ambivalent/resistant-insecure category
also called the C babies,
for the letter C just as a categorization.
The anxious-ambivalent, resistant-insecure toddlers, really,
show distress even before separation from their mother
or other caregiver.
And they tend to be very clingy and difficult to comfort
when the caregiver returns, okay?
So they're distressed even before the mother leaves the room
and they tend to be very clingy and really hard to calm down
when the mother returns.
They tend to show either what seems to be resentment
in response to the parent's absence.
We don't really know what they're feeling.
Or some helpless passivity.
And there's actually subcategorizations
that the psychologists have come up with, C one subtypes
and C two subtypes.
We don't have to get bogged down in that.
But just know that there isn't one absolute measure
that says, oh, well, this person
is anxious-ambivalent, resistant-insecure.
They could be somewhat passive, or they could be,
seem somewhat angry at the caregiver.
But the basic idea is that before and after the separation,
they are clingy and difficult to comfort.
They just can't seem to calm themselves down,
and physiological measures of heart rate and hormone
measurements such as cortisol also support that statement.
And the third category of attachment style is the so-called
disorganized or disoriented or D for the letter D babies.
This is a categorization that was added later
to this strange situation task.
That is a real hallmark of developmental psychology studies.
It was developed by Mary Ainsworth, graduate student,
Mary Maine, who I actually had the great fortune of taking
a course from and learning from when I was a graduate
student at Berkeley many years ago.
And this fourth categorization was controversial
for a while, but now is generally accepted.
The key feature of the disorganized, disoriented category
is that the toddlers tend to be tense
and they tend to encompass a lot of odd physical postures.
They tend to hunch their shoulders.
They'll put their hands behind their neck.
They'll cock their head to the side.
For those of you listening,
I'm doing this on the video version.
It's not where you don't have to go see that.
But for those of you that are watching this on video,
they tend to constrain their body size a bit
and going to odd postures that they normally wouldn't
do anywhere else.
So this is why it's called the disorganized
or disoriented category.
It seems like these children just don't really know
how to react to a separation.
And they just start to manifest behaviors and emotional
tones that aren't observed in other situations.
Okay, so we've got our four categories.
I'll try and use the shortest possible names
for each category.
We've got category one, which is securely attached.
We've got category two, which is insecurely attached,
also sometimes called anxious-avoidant.
Then we've got category three,
which is the resistant-insecure category,
which is anxious-ambivalent.
And then there's this fourth category,
the disorganized, disoriented category
where they're so-called D babies.
Now, what's interesting about this from the perspective
of desire, love and attachment is that the categorizations
of children into one of these four different categories
as toddlers is strongly predictive of their attachment style
in romantic partnerships later in life.
Which is to me both amazing and surprising
and not surprising all at the same time.
Amazing because it means that, first of all,
we are relatively hardwired for attachment.
I think that that's incredible and beautiful,
that we have designated neurons, nerve cells
and hormonal systems that are there to ensure
that we have some response to a caregiver being there,
or not being there, or returning, or leaving,
but also that the same neural circuitries, the same hormonal
responses are at least in some way repurposed
for entirely different types of attachments later in life.
So when we hear the psychologist talk about how we formed
a template early in life based on experiences
that were even pre-verbal before we had language,
and those templates are superimposed on our relationships,
or we should say, our later relationships are superimposed
on those templates, there really is a basis for that.
We now have neuroimaging studies to support, for instance,
the work of Allan Schore from UCLA, showing that
when a mother and child interact, either through very
soothing interactions, like bottle feeding,
or breastfeeding, or singing to one's baby,
or putting them to sleep, that the brain of the child
and the brain of the mother
are entering a coordinated state of relaxation.
And it's not one direction, mother to child,
the child is also calming the mother.
Typically these studies were done with mothers again,
sometimes with fathers, but typically with mothers.
And in addition to that, when the mother or other caregiver
acts very excited and raises their voice,
or puts a lilt in their voice, or widens their eyes,
that the child will do the same.
And again, there's a bidirectional interaction
in that case of excitement.
And there's the release of neurochemicals like dopamine
into the bloodstream, whereas in the relaxation scenario
and the soothing scenario, there's we know the release
of things like serotonin and oxytocin.
So the neural systems for attachment and the neural systems
for what we call autonomic arousal for being alert and calm
don't act in a vacuum.
They are tethered to other people in our environment.
And of course we know this, right?
We sometimes hear the statement,
no one can make you feel anything.
I've always had a little bit of a problem
with that statement.
I don't think I'm contradicting anyone in particular,
but you hear that a lot, no one can make you feel anything.
Indeed they can, right?
A physical injury can make you feel something.
If somebody says something that you very much like,
it can make you feel something.
And if somebody says something that you very much dislike,
it will make you feel something.
So the idea that no one can make us feel anything
isn't actually true.
Our nervous system is tethered
to the nervous systems of others.
And that is true from the very earliest stages of our lives.
And in this case, we're talking about how our templates
for attachment in romantic relationships, how we find them,
how we maintain them, and indeed how we break them
and reform them is based on a template that was established
through an entirely different set of priorities,
which was how we feel safe and secure in novel environments,
depending on whether or not our primary caregiver
is there or not.
Neuroimaging supports that.
When I say neuroimaging, I mean, brain scan support that,
measures of hormones in the body and brain support that,
measures of neurochemicals support that.
There's simply no way around this truth,
that we have a set of roadmaps in our mind that are reused
for entirely different purposes later in life.
That is vitally important to understand.
Because if one is successful in forming
romantic attachments, maintaining them, et cetera, or not,
does in fact reflect the earlier templates
that you've created.
But as I've mentioned before, the good news is
that these templates can shift over time.
And one of the more powerful ways to shift those templates
over time is purely by the knowledge that they exist,
and the understanding that those templates are malleable.
They can change through the process of neuroplasticity.
Again, neuroplasticity is just a rewiring
of nerve connection.
That is very much present in childhood,
but also very much present in adulthood.
So if you're somebody who you think falls into category
one, two, three, or four, or you know somebody,
or involved with somebody who falls into category
one, two, three, and four,
the mere knowledge of that can be very useful.
But you might ask, well, what do I do with that knowledge?
Well, fortunately, both psychologists and biologists
have started to leverage that knowledge toward establishing
better, more secure bonds in adult romantic relationships.
And there's a book that has really tapped into this.
I think it's the first book that has really addressed
this head on.
And that book comes from two Columbia professors.
And title of the book is "Attached: The New Science of Adult
Attachment and How It Can Help You Find and Keep Love."
The authors of this book are Amir Levine and Rachel Heller.
Again, both of them are skilled academics and researchers
who have really taken the literature that I described
on the strange situation task
and mapped it to adult attachment styles.
And also they've mapped out ways that they've observed
in their clinical practice.
And that is laboratory supported for, for instance,
people that have an anxious-ambivalent
or what we were calling insecure attachment style,
or for people that fall
into the disorganized or disoriented attachment style,
how they can modify that attachment style in or out
of relationships in order to establish what I think
everybody wants, which is secure attachment.
Why does everybody want that?
Well, secure attachment allows people to be both
in relationship, or if they choose to be on their own,
or to be in relationship but physically separated
from somebody else, or even emotionally separated
from somebody else and maintain
what we call a stable autonomic equilibrium,
the ability to remain calm, clearheaded.
You might not like what's happening,
but you're able to navigate that with some sense of clarity
and not excessive discomfort.
So, is there a goal in all of this stuff
about love, desire, and attachment?
Indeed, there is.
The secure attachment style is the one that leads
to the most stable and predictable long-term relationships.
Put differently, babies, toddlers, adolescents, teens,
and young adults that have a secure attachment style
are more likely to find and form long-term relationships
than are people in the other categories.
But people in other categories can learn and eventually
migrate into the secure attachment style.
And I think that book "Attached..."
I have no affiliation to the authors or the book itself,
I should just mention that.
"Attached: The New Science of Adult Attachment
and How It Can Help You Find and Keep Love,"
really it sounds very pop psychology, yes,
but it is really grounded
in the research psychology literature.
And there's also some interesting biology there.
Another point to make about attachment styles
is that it is possible.
And some of you may be familiar with circumstances
whereby people who are securely attached,
either because they grew up in an environment
where secure attachment was cultivated,
or because they developed that on their own,
can also migrate out of the securely attached category
into insecurely attached, or into avoidant types
of attachment styles as teens, or as young adults,
or as adults at any age or any stage of life,
by virtue of being with somebody who has a different,
perhaps less adaptive attachment style, right?
What this means is that if you have or you develop
a secure attachment style, it's vitally important
to protect that attachment style because it is possible
to become anxiously attached,
even if you grew up in a stable attachment framework.
And again, this can happen at any stage.
So if you're interested in attachment styles
and how they influence adult romantic attachments,
and certainly if you are a parent, I would encourage you
to check out the book, "Attached."
Again, it's quite good and I think that it offers a number
of actionable tools to both form
and hold onto secure attachment styles.
So I mentioned that the neural circuits for child-parent
or child-caregiver attachment are repurposed
for romantic attachment later in life.
But what are these neural circuits?
What do they do?
I mean, it's so attractive, if you will,
to think about a brain area that controls love,
or a brain area that controls desire,
or a brain area that controls attachment,
but it simply doesn't work that way.
As I've talked about before on this podcast
and I will say again and again because it will persist
to be true long after I'm gone,
is that no one brain area can give rise to anything
as complex as desire, or love, or attachment.
Instead, there are multiple brain areas that
through their coordinated action create
a song that we call desire, or a song that we call love,
or a song that we call attachment.
Not a literal song, although there are songs
about desire, love, and attachment, of course, many songs,
some good, some not so good.
But rather different brain areas being active in different
sequences and with different intensities can make us feel
as if we are in the mode that we call desire,
or in the mode of love, or in the mode of attachment.
But beneath all of that is this element
of autonomic arousal.
And I want to focus on autonomic arousal
just for a bit longer,
because it really is one of the three core elements
by which we form and maintain loving attachments,
and by which we break loving attachments.
The autonomic nervous system
as the name suggests is automatic.
In fact, that's what autonomic means.
Now it's actually the case that we can control
our autonomic nervous system to some degree or another.
But the autonomic nervous system controls things like
digestion, our breathing, whether or not we're conscious
of that breathing or not.
It controls things like how alert we are or sleepy we are.
And the autonomic nervous system,
as I just briefly described earlier,
is really something that we come into the world with.
It's hardwired, all the elements are there.
But through interactions with our parent,
either soothing interactions, or fun, playful interactions,
or dare I say, scary interactions,
our autonomic nervous system gets tuned up.
Meaning, we each develop a tendency to either be more alert
and anxious, or more calm, or a balance of alert and calm.
Now, of course, this changes across each day
and depending how tired we are late in the day.
If we've been awake for a while,
we tend to get sleepy early in the day.
If we're very rested,
we tend to wake up and feel very alert.
So the way to think about the autonomic nervous system
is it's kind of a seesaw.
We go back and forth between being very alert,
we can be alert and calm, or we can be very, very alert,
we can be in a state of panic,
we can be fast asleep so we can be extremely calm,
or we can just be sleepy, semi calm but still also alert.
So think about it like a seesaw.
And that seesaw has a hinge,
and that hinge defines how tight or loose that seesaw is,
how readily it can tilt back and forth.
Our autonomic tone is how tight that hinge is.
And there are biological mechanisms to explain this,
but here I just want to stay with the analogy
of the seesaw for now.
The interactions between child and caregiver early in life
take the child and the caregiver
from one end of the seesaw to the other.
From being very alert in a state of play, for instance,
to being nursed and being very soothed until we go to sleep.
And of course, we each have a seesaw,
the parent and the child has a seesaw,
and they're interacting.
What do I mean by that?
Well, there are beautiful studies, and beautiful
not in the sense that they focused on a pleasant topic,
but beautiful because they were done so beautifully well,
that looked at, for instance, the response of mothers
and their physiologies and the response of children
and their physiologies during the bombing of cities
during World War II.
So an unpleasant situation.
But what was revealed during the course of these studies
was that if the mothers were very stressed
during an onslaught of bombing of the city,
the children's physiologies tended to be stressed also,
and persisted in being stressed long after that stressful
episode was done.
They actually followed that these children
well out from many decades afterwards.
Conversely, if the parent, and in this case, again,
it was mothers that were explored in these studies,
had turned this whole business of going
into the bomb shelters into somewhat of a game, right?
Taking it seriously, but essentially telling the children,
"Okay, it's time to go,"
but not expressing much stress or distress,
the children also didn't develop much stress,
or distress, or trauma.
Now there were exceptions to this, of course,
but in general, that was the rule
that the autonomic nervous systems of children tend to mimic
the autonomic nervous systems of the primary caregiver.
And the mechanisms by which this occurs has been explored.
And again I just refer to the beautiful work of Allan Schore
at University of California, Los Angeles.
And again, his name is Schore, spelled S-C-H-O-R-E.
I'm looking down briefly at the floor here
because I'll just reach for the book.
He has a wonderful book called "Right Brain Psychotherapy."
It's a little bit technical,
but if you're interested in some of the studies,
this book "Right Brain Psychotherapy" details how everything
from nursing of children to playtime behavior,
to strange situation type task behavior that we talked
about before which of course occurs when children
get dropped off at daycare, or nursery school,
or with babysitters, et cetera.
And indeed all types of caregiver-child interactions
tune up that autonomic nervous system,
so that the child ends up with an autonomic nervous system
that either tends to lean more towards alert and anxious,
or can be very alert but calm,
or can be very calm and hard to budge.
Again, it's the tightness of that hinge
that really underlies these attachment styles
that we were talking about earlier,
and not on this episode of the "Huberman Lab Podcast,"
but on many other previous previous episodes,
such as the master stress episode,
or some of the optimized health episodes.
You can find these if you want at hubermanlab.com.
A lot of the tools and techniques that are recommended there
have to do with readjusting the autonomic nervous system
in deliberate ways as an adult.
Again, I won't go into the specific tools,
but for instance, the physiological sigh.
This tool that I've talked about extensively of two inhales
through the nose, as deeply as you can on the first one,
sneaking in a little bit more air on the second one,
and then a long exhale through the mouth
is a way of adjusting that autonomic seesaw.
It tends to make us more calm.
It activates what we call the parasympathetic arm
of the autonomic nervous system, which is just fancy nerd
speak for it's a quick way to calm yourself down, right?
Things like ice baths, or cold showers, or cold immersion,
or deliberate hyperventilation by contrast,
or ways in which we can deliberately increase the level
of our so-called sympathetic arm of our autonomic nervous
system to make ourselves more alert.
Why would you want to do that?
Well, you can do that to be more alert,
to be more awake, if you like,
or as a form of self-induced stress inoculation
to be able to tolerate higher levels of adrenaline
by making it a practice that you self-direct.
The reason those tools are out there is because many of us,
for whatever reason, we don't have to blame anyone,
but because of our childhood templates,
because of things that happened and didn't happen
in terms of our interactions with caregivers,
have autonomic nervous systems that are tilted to one side
or the other more than we would like.
Or in which the hinge that I'm talking about in this analogy
is too loose or that is too tight.
And we're stuck in a mode of anxiousness,
or stuck in a mode of lack of energy.
That's what those tools are really about.
But at a deeper level, the autonomic nervous system
is really the system that governs how we will react
in response to a romantic partner being present or leaving.
And I don't necessarily mean leaving the relationship
entirely, although it could mean that, right?
We know people, I'm sure you know people that upon the end
of a relationship that they wanted very much,
are absolutely crushed.
And actually in researching this episode there,
I discovered there's an extensive literature finding
that the feelings that one has after a breakup are very much
like a clinical depression in many cases.
But there are individuals that can look at a breakup
as a transient event that they don't interpret
as going to mean so much for all aspects of their life
or reshaping their view of themselves, why?
Well, we have different levels of autonomic function.
And depending on where our seesaw is, if you will,
some of us become extremely distraught and can't recalibrate
ourselves, can't adjust ourselves down from stress to calm,
or can't take ourselves from exhausted to more alert
if we need to do that on our own.
And so that's why tools for doing that exist.
But attachment itself is about
where our autonomic nervous system resides.
So if I were to offer a set of tools around these topics
of desire, love and attachment, I would say, first of all,
you might want to think about whether or not you fall
into the secure, insecure, or other attachment styles.
Second, I think it is vitally important for all of us,
but certainly for people that are in relationships
or seeking relationships to be able to at least have some
recognition of where our autonomic nervous system tends
to reside both in terms of when we are with somebody
and when they leave.
When we are apart for long periods of time,
can we calm ourselves? Can we self-soothe?
Or are we very much dependent on the presence of another
in order to feel soothed?
Now, I absolutely want to emphasize
that there is nothing wrong.
In fact, there's everything right with feeling great
in the presence of somebody else.
That is actually a hallmark
of strong and quality attachments.
These days, we hear the term codependent a lot.
This was a, I believe, the term was first coined
by Pia Mellody.
And it actually does occupy an important role in the world
of trauma, trauma healing, so-called trauma bonding,
topics of another episode.
Actually, did an episode on fear and trauma.
And we will do one all about trauma bonding
with an expert at some point in the future.
But codependence and codependency, the term can sometimes
be misinterpreted as any dependence on another isn't good.
Interdependence, a healthy interdependence,
of course is good.
It is the hallmark of healthy child-parent relations,
sibling relations and romantic relations.
But a key element of healthy interdependence is that,
yes, our autonomic nervous system is adjusted
by the presence of another, but that also that we can adjust
our own autonomic nervous system,
even in the absence of that person.
That if the person goes away temporarily or permanently,
that we can still regulate our own autonomic nervous system,
both from states of stress to states of calm,
both from states of exhaustion to states of more alertness.
And of course, we all need sleep
to go from exhaustion to alertness.
But what I'm referring to here is the ability to regulate
when distraught, or regulate when fatigued,
or feeling depressed.
And that is and is all about the autonomic nervous system.
So as we talk about attachment styles,
and we talk about infant and toddler
and adult attachment styles,
what we are really talking about is a complex set
of neural circuitries.
And one of those neural circuitries, which is absolutely
crucial is this autonomic nervous system.
So if the autonomic nervous system is one key component
of desire, love and attachment, what are the other two?
And what I'm going to tell you next is largely the pioneering
work of Helen Fisher, who is really an anthropologist,
who's become a bit of a neuroscientist and has collaborated
with neuroscientists to establish brain areas
and neural circuits that are associated
with different aspects of attachment, love and desire.
I think the first really high quality study
of neural circuits associated with these themes
was a paper published in 2005 in a very fine anatomical
journal, perhaps the best neural anatomical journal,
which is the Journal of Comparative Neurology.
The Journal of Comparative Neurology has been around
for more than 100 years and is considered the archival
location for placing really high quality anatomy.
They have tremendously high standards.
And the study that I'm referring to is entitled,
"Romantic Love: An fMRI,"
meaning functional magnetic resonance imaging,
"Study of a Neural Mechanism for Mate Choice."
And Dr. Fisher is a author on this paper
as is Arthur Aron, and Lucy Brown.
So all very fine researchers.
And this study as well as several other studies
using magnetic resonance imaging,
things like EEG, neuro anatomical tracing, et cetera,
have identified a large number of brain areas
that are associated with different aspects
of desire, love, and attachment.
And I'll just throw out a few names of those brain areas
and what they control.
And then I'll tell you how those anchor
to the other two categories of neural circuits
essential for desire, love, and attachment.
So not surprisingly the dopamine system in the brain
is associated with desire, love, and attachment,
and mainly with desire, although to some extent, love.
Dopamine is a neurochemical sometimes
associated with reward.
But as some of you have heard me say before,
it is mainly a molecule of motivation, craving and pursuit.
And that motivation, craving and pursuit that relates
to dopamine is not unique to attachment, or love, or sex,
or mating, et cetera.
It is a universal generic currency in the brain
for pursuing something.
Food when you're hungry, a mate when you want one,
to mate when you want to, warmth when you're cold,
et cetera, et cetera, okay?
So it's not for one specific purpose.
But the brain areas associated with dopamine involve,
for instance, the ventral tegmental area,
the substantia nigra, areas of that sort, the basal ganglia.
You don't need to know these names,
just understand that these are networks of neurons
that tend to have put the person, you into a state
of forward action and pursuit and craving and motivation.
They are not about being quiescent, relaxed, et cetera.
The neural circuits for quiescence and relaxation
are most associated with love and attachment,
not surprisingly, and they're the neurochemical
serotonin, and to some extent, oxytocin
are the predominant neurochemicals involved.
And those are released from brain areas,
such as the raphe nucleus in the back of the brain.
You may have heard that the majority of serotonin
in your body is made in your gut, and indeed, that's true,
but I hate to break it to you, the serotonin in your gut
is not responsible for your feelings of love and attachment,
at least not to a high degree.
That's mainly going to be the reflection of neurons
in your brain that make serotonin.
And there are other areas of the brain that make serotonin
as well and oxytocin as well,
but they tend to be associated with the warmth and calm,
and the soothing that we feel in the presence of another.
And again, these are not strictly divided circuits.
We can have dopamine and serotonin present in our brain
and body at the same time to equal or different degrees.
And we return in a little bit to what happens when levels
of dopamine are very high and levels of serotonin are low
and vice versa and so on,
including in states of neurochemically modified states,
as it were in, when we talk about things like MDMA,
so-called ecstasy.
But in the meantime, I want to just discuss
the two neural circuits that use dopamine,
that use serotonin and oxytocin,
and that collaborate with the autonomic nervous system
to drive what we call desire, love and attachment.
And the three circuits are autonomic nervous system,
we talked about that one.
Then there's the nervous system components
or the neural circuits for empathy, for being able to see
and respond to indeed match the emotional tone
or the autonomic tone of another.
And then there's the third category,
and this might surprise some of you,
it certainly surprised me,
but the data point to the fact that the third neural circuit
that's very important for establishing bonds
is one associated with positive delusions.
So given that the neural circuits for empathy
are absolutely crucial for falling in love
and maintaining stable attachments,
I'd like to talk about those neural circuits
and what they are.
Now, often when we hear empathy, we think,
oh, empathy is really about listening to and really
understanding what somebody else is feeling.
Maybe even feeling what they're feeling.
And indeed that's the case, but what do we mean by that?
Right? What is it to feel what another feels?
Well, what it means is that their seesaw
is driving your seesaw,
or your seesaw is somehow driving their seesaw.
That there's a match in terms of the tilt of those seesaws.
Now it doesn't have to be an exact match, right?
If someone that you really he care about
is very, very stressed, you could also become very stressed.
That's a form of empathic matching,
and there are indeed neural circuits for that.
I'll describe what those neural circuits are in a moment.
But sometimes the best role for us to take is actually one
in which we are calm when the person that we care about
or that we are romantically involved
with is very, very anxious.
And in a few minutes, I'll talk about how matching
of emotional tone can be good or bad
for the stability of a relationship,
and complementarity of autonomic matching
can be good or bad.
In other words, sometimes it's beneficial for a relationship
to go into the same state as the other.
And sometimes it's more beneficial for us to not go
into the same state as the other.
But the important feature here is that when we talk about
emotional matching or empathy or going into the same state
or not going into the same state, what we are really talking
about is whether or not the autonomic seesaw
of one individual is driving the autonomic seesaw
of the other individual.
And this is a vital principle for how we fall in love
and form attachments.
And it's actually part of the desire
and mating process itself.
I would go so far as to say that one of the prerequisites
to the propagation and expansion of our species
is this notion of autonomic regulation.
And to some extent, matching of autonomic nervous systems.
Let me explain what I mean.
Last I checked, the only way that new humans can be created
is by way of sperm meeting egg, either in body or in dish,
but sperm meets egg and then typically nine months later,
we have a human baby.
The process of bringing sperm to egg, right?
Mating behavior, sex behavior in humans
is one of autonomic regulation.
And what I mean by that is the process of finding a mate,
and in this case, I mean, actually someone to mate with,
typically, well, scenarios vary, typically,
is one of elevated autonomic arousal,
meaning increased activation
of the so-called sympathetic nervous system.
This is related to dopamine release
and it's related to epinephrine release.
There has to be a pursuit or at least there has to be
a mobilization to arrive in the same location
whereby one can mate, right?
That almost always is the case.
However, the sexual arousal itself
in both males and females
is actually driven primarily by the parasympathetic arm
of the autonomic nervous system.
So while pursuit is one of alertness and sympathetic drive,
as we say, again, sympathy is not really
what's at play here,
the word simple means together,
and the activation of the autonomic nervous system
toward more alert state
is because of a sympathetic nervous system,
meaning the co-activation together of many neurons
in the brain and spinal cord.
But then the actual physiological arousal state
that we call sexual arousal is predominantly
parasympathetically driven, okay?
To be quite direct about this,
if the sympathetic nervous system activation is too high,
the sexual arousal response cannot happen in either males
or in females, it's inhibited.
However, the orgasm and ejaculation response,
which if you think about it is required for sperm
to meet egg is sympathetic driven.
And then after orgasm and ejaculation,
the parasympathetic nervousness system kicks back in,
and there's a calming and relaxation.
So the arc of mating involves sympathetic arousal, okay?
Not sympathy, but alertness and arousal for pursuit,
then a tilt of the seesaw at least to some degree
for arousal of the sort that we typically hear
of sexual arousal.
Then at the point of orgasm and ejaculation is back
to a sympathetic response.
And how can I say that? How do I know that?
The sympathetic nervous system meaning neurons
within the sympathetic arm of the autonomic nervous system
are what drive ejaculation and orgasm.
And then afterward there's a return
to increased parasympathetic activation.
And we don't know for sure why that happens,
but it's thought that in species that pair bond,
humans generally pair bond, not always,
there the return to more parasympathetic activation
after orgasm and ejaculation is thought to increase
the exchange of pheromonal orders, odors, excuse me,
and to increase pillow talk and pair bonding
of different kinds, okay?
So that's the seesaw going back and forth is actually built
into the process by which our entire species propagates.
So in some ways, every human is required to navigate
that process if they want their offspring to persist.
And of course, nowadays there are technologies like
in vitro fertilization, intrauterine insemination,
there are a variety of ways that technology has allowed
people to circumvent the actual physical mating process
in the way that I described.
But by and large, that's the way it it's done.
And certainly that's the way it was done historically
for if not tens of thousands
or hundreds of thousands of years.
That process is also what happens
in all mammalian species that mate.
Okay, so I'm overlooking an entire literature
of animal studies that classic studies of this were done
by two individuals, I'll just briefly mention them
in case you want to look at the literature.
There's a guy at the Rockefeller University named
Donald Pfaff, P-F-A-F-F, who has done beautiful studies
identifying the neural circuitry,
what's called the lordosis response.
Unlike in humans, the mating behavior of animals
is rather stereotyped in terms of the positions
that they occupy.
And the lordosis response is a U-shaping
or a bending up of the hindquarters of typically of rodents,
but of other animals as well.
The the male mounting is almost always from behind
except in some species of primates.
And that lordosis response is only going to occur
during particular phases of the estrous cycle.
The estrous cycle is the analog to the menstrual cycle,
but it's not 28 days, it's four days or some other duration
in other animals, depending on the animal.
The lordosis response is strongly regulated by odors,
by contact and is estrogen and testosterone controlled.
And then the male portion of the mating sequence in animals,
the mounting and thrusting and ejaculation as they're called
or mounting, thrusting, intromission and ejaculation.
Those are the four scientific categories
that have been described,
that's presence in rodents and also in dogs
where it was primarily studied by Frank Beach,
who was at University of California, Berkeley
for a long time.
And the entire literature around the neural circuitry
for sexual mating behavior in animals largely stemmed
from the work of Donald Pfaff and Frank Beach
and their scientific offspring, not their actual offspring.
You can look at that literature, if you like.
There have been human neuroimaging studies of the process
that I described a few minutes ago, believe it or not,
of people in brain scanners, not necessarily mating
with other people, but going through that arc of arousal,
sympathetic activation during orgasm or ejaculation
and then the post-ejaculatory or orgasmic phase
in both men and women.
And the brain areas associated with those
have all been mapped out now.
The spinal cord areas that control things like erection,
vaginal lubrication, ejaculation and orgasm,
those have also been mapped out.
And this has all been explored from the perspective
of both basic science, just to get an understanding
of how our species has sexual interactions and reproduces,
but also from the perspective of, for instance,
trying to repair sexual function after spinal cord injury,
which is a prominent concern for a lot of people,
depending on where they have their injury,
but in the number of people that have spinal cord injuries.
So this is both vital biological and clinical data.
The neural circuits for everything that I just described
reside in the autonomic nervous system and are coordinated
with the neural circuits that are associated with empathy.
The neural circuits for empathy, again, there are many,
but mainly two structures that you should know about,
the prefrontal cortex,
which is how we perceive things outside of us
and make decisions on the basis of those perceptions,
how we organize those decisions,
and an area of the brain called the insula, I-N-S-U-L-A.
The insula is a really interesting brain area that allows us
to interocept, to pay attention to what's going on inside
our body and to split some of our attention, to exterocept.
And the mating dance, whether or not it's the dinner
and date portion of the mating dance,
or the actual physical dance part of the main dance,
or actual mating and sexual behavior, kissing or otherwise,
that is a coordinated activity of two bodies,
typically it's two.
I realize sometimes it's more, sometimes it's only one,
but typically it's two bodies at least in the framework
we're using here.
That coordinated dance is one
in which the autonomic nervous system of one individual
in general is coordinating with the autonomic nervous system
of the other individual.
And the insula is essentially splitting one's attention
between how we feel ourselves, how our body feels,
what we're thinking with the thinking
and the bodily sensations of the other.
And that can be communicated, obviously, through words,
it can be communicated through sounds,
it can be communicated through touch,
and it can be communicated through a number
of more subtle cues like pupil size or whether or not,
certainly in cases where we recognize the person
and we know their responses, their autonomic responses
under different conditions.
We can assess, is the person comfortable?
Are they uncomfortable?
Are they more focused on me or on themselves?
This is the coordinated silent dance that
if we look out in neurobiological terms,
we can really see is all about the autonomic nervous system,
whether or not it's time to tip the seesaw to one side
or the other, depending on whether or not the other person's
seesaw is tilted higher or lower than the other.
Okay, so we have the autonomic nervous system,
and then we have this thing that we're calling empathy,
which is really about autonomic matching.
And again, the insula and the prefrontal cortex
are neural circuits that are crucial for autonomic matching,
because they allow us to say, what's out there,
and do I want to match to it or not? Okay?
And then the third category of neural circuit
that Helen Fisher and others have found to be important
for desire, love and attachment is the neural circuit
associated with self-delusion.
What do we mean by that?
Well, first of all, self-delusion implies a kind of cynicism
about love and attachment.
And I think it was George Bernard Shaw that said,
"Love is really about overestimating
the differences between individuals."
Actually, when I hear that, and as I say it,
I really don't like that quote.
I have no bone to pick with George Bernard Shaw,
but what it suggests and I think what he meant was that,
in love and attachment, we tend to put so much value
in the other that we forget that many of the processes
that are going on in our brain and body
actually could be evoked by many other people too.
But I think it somewhat overlooks the enormous power
of attachment and the ways in which somebody's smell,
somebody's voice, somebody's everything,
or somebody's particular thing or things can really
become so vital for our autonomic nervous system
to feel soothed and to feel elated, et cetera.
So I think that while the quote is accurate,
in the one sense, I think it does overlook
the neural circuits for attachment
and just how deeply wired those can become for us.
So I will balance that quote with an enormous number
of other quotes that I won't mention, but that you can find
out there that really point to how incredible the person is
that one tends to be attached to that there's really
only one or several people that could ever exist
that could evoke those feelings from us.
And of course you can read your Neruda poetry,
and you can find these things all over the place,
in music, and poetry, and writing.
So for every cynical quote about these neural circuits
being generic and could be activated by anybody,
I think you'll find an ample number of opposing quotes
that these neural circuits can only be activated
by that special someone, or that particular person,
or maybe in just a small set of those people.
So what of delusion?
Well, the work of Helen Fisher and others has really pointed
to the fact that desire, love and attachment
are three separate phases
of what we call romantic relationships.
That typically, not always, but typically desire tends
to come first or falls in the early phase.
That the process of romantic/sexual interactions,
it doesn't necessarily have to be sex itself,
but certainly something that involves intimacy
of some kind, right?
And generally touch of some kind,
eventually transitions into what we call love,
which eventually transitions into what we call attachment.
And I should just mention touch because touch
is a fundamental aspect of this whole process.
There's an article, a research article, I should say,
the title of it is,
"Relationship-specific Encoding of Social Touch
in Somatosensory and Insular Cortices."
Cortices being cortex.
Cortex's plural, cortices.
And again, there's our friend the insula.
So this is a study that explored what brain areas
and what body areas are activated by specific forms
of attachment and social touch.
And what they found not surprisingly is that the areas
of the brain that are associated with touch,
the somatosensory areas, but more interestingly,
the insular cortex are strongly activated by touch.
So touch in the amount of touching, and proximity,
and skin contact, not surprisingly activates brain areas
associated with somatosensory, touch,
but also the insular cortex, which again,
is this brain area that links the internal
or feelings about what's going on inside us
and at the surface of our skin with events external.
And they found activation of a number of brain areas,
the amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex, and so on and so on.
That's not as essential as just understanding
that the insular is the place in which we start to take
our experience of our internal landscape,
attach that to our perceptions of the external landscape,
and then assign that a value or assign that
some sort of interpretation.
And positive delusion is predictive of long-term attachment.
What do we mean by positive delusion?
Positive delusion is the contradiction
of that George Bernard Shaw quote.
It's the belief that only this person
can make me feel this way.
This other person holds the capacity to make me feel
this way physically or emotionally or both.
And so, as we move from desire to love to attachment,
our brain circuitry is literally getting tuned up such that
that individual that we happen to be attached to,
again, here thinking about monogamous relationships,
but I guess for non-monogamous relationship individuals
is and are the way in which our autonomic nervous system
can be regulated.
They actually get access to our control panel.
So it's our autonomic nervous system empathy
and this positive delusion.
Now positive delusion is critical.
If you look at the stability of relationships over time,
something that's been extensively studied
mainly by psychologists, but now also by neurobiologists,
what you find is that there are some key features
of interactions between individuals that predict
that a relationship will last.
And those are many,
but mainly fall under this category of positive delusions.
I'll return to those in what those exactly look like.
But there are also just a handful of things that predict
that a relationship will fail over time.
This is largely the work of the Gottman's,
is actually a husband and wife team
up at the University of Washington in Seattle.
The Gottman's have, run a laboratory
in the department of psychology for a long time.
They've also done a lot of public facing
work around relationships.
And they've talked about the various aspects
of relationships and interactions between people
that predict either staying together or breaking up.
So much so that they've established a method
by which they can look at video interactions between couples
and with very high degree of certainty predict
whether or not those couples will stay together
or break up over time.
And they've identified what they call
the four horsemen of relationships.
These are things that essentially,
almost always predict that a couple will break up.
And I think the current number on this is that Gottman
can predict divorce with 94% accuracy,
which if you think about is pretty remarkable.
So even though these are purely psychological studies,
I'm not aware of any analysis of underlying physiology,
there are some things that they can observe between couples
that can lead them to predict whether or not a couple
will stay together or break up with 94% accuracy.
So what are those things?
Those four behaviors, what they call the Four Horsemen
of the Apocalypse [laughs] for relationships
are one, criticism, two, defensiveness, three, stonewalling,
and four, contempt, with contempt being the most powerful
predictor of breaking up.
Criticism, of course, does not mean that there's no place
for criticism in stable relationships.
Of course, there is.
Has to do with how frequent and how intensely
that criticism is voiced.
Defensiveness, of course, is defensiveness.
We know as the lack of ability to hear another
or to adopt their stance.
So lack of empathy,
I think is one way to interpret defensiveness.
Stonewalling, which is actually another form
of lack of empathy.
It's a turning off of this neural circuit
that's so critical for desire, love and attachment.
The stonewalling essentially means the emotional response
or the request of another is completely cut off.
So it's, I don't think there's been brain imaging of this,
but I think, we can reasonably imagine that it involves
untethering your insular response from the other
and what they're dealing with,
and focusing your insular response,
[giggles] no pun intended, on your own internal state
or perhaps the state of someone else entirely.
I'll talk about infidelity in a moment.
And then contempt.
And contempt has actually been referred
to as the sulfuric acid of relationship.
I didn't say that, but Gottman and colleagues have.
That it is such a powerful predictor
of divorce and breakups in the future.
And contempt, of course, by definition is the feeling
that a person or thing is beneath consideration,
worthlessness or deserving scorn.
And apparently they can identify this in the videos
of couples having discussions and interacting
by very elaborate eye rolls, by expressions of anger
in one individual when their partner is actually expressing
enthusiasm or excitement about something.
It's the, oh yeah, you would say that,
or deep seated resentment toward the other,
so much so that it's apparent that one actively dislikes
the other partner.
So contempt, disregard for something that should be taken
into account is the other way to think about it.
That runs counter to all of the neural circuits,
all three of the neural circuits
that we talked about before.
It certainly, it is the antithesis of empathy.
It is anything but a positive delusion.
It's really looking at the other individual,
either accurately or inaccurately
as somebody that you despise.
And then it is an absolute inversion of the autonomic
seesaw matching that I was talking about before.
It's a dissociating of your seesaw from their seesaw.
They're very excited about something,
you're unexcited by it.
In fact, it's an inversion of their seesaw
where they're excited, you're down.
They're down, you are up, okay?
So it's basically an inversion of all of the neural circuits
that Helen Fisher and others have identified as critical
for desire, love, and attachment.
And therefore it's not surprising that it is so strongly
predictive of breakups,
and in the case of married couples, of divorce.
For those of you that are interested in the work
of the Gottman's and similar work,
they've written several popular books.
They're fairly easy to find.
We can link to one of those in the caption.
But they've also developed
some quite interesting online resources
in their so-called Love Lab.
I guess it's fortunate that they didn't call it the Hate Lab
or the Breakup Lab,
'cause they focused a lot on what predicts breakups.
But they've also written extensively and researched
extensively in peer-reviewed studies
what makes people find appropriate partners for them,
and to maintain those partnerships over time.
So you can go, you can search for Love Lab,
University of Washington, Gottman,
or any number of their various books.
I think you'll find some useful resources there.
I want to shift back to the work of Helen Fisher.
She's made some very interesting statements
and some very interesting observations
that at least to my mind map very well
onto the knowledge of neural circuitry both in humans
and in non-human primates and in other species.
I realize that she's the only name in the game.
But she's made some observations that I think are very,
as we say, parsimonious, meaning they allow us to organize
a lot of this stuff into some distinct frameworks.
Now, she's also done some really beautiful studies
that involve data from millions or even tens of millions
of individuals on dating sites.
So I'm going to share those with you in a moment.
But before I do that, I just want to paraphrase Dr. Fisher,
who said that sex drive or desire that the pursuit
of someone to mate with, meaning to mate, the verb,
not necessarily to find a mate, may be,
she didn't say definitively, but may be a way to forage
for potential love partners that the arc of this
whole business is really the order that we're describing it,
that it's desire, then love, and then attachment.
And that oftentimes people can get confused.
You may know some of these people,
you may be one of these individuals who might confuse desire
for attachment or might confuse love for attachment,
but that there's a sequence of recruitment
of these neural circuits that's established first
from the pursuit of someone to mate with.
And she's placed us in the context of more modern dating
themes where depending on culture, people might explore
several, maybe many, many individuals before,
quote, unquote, settling down with somebody,
at least for some period of time.
I think that's an interesting framework
because it circumvents a lot of the, frankly, unanswerable
questions about whether or not humans were meant
to be monogamous or whether or not they weren't.
Those are conversations that hold cultural context,
that hold all sorts of context,
that really can't be addressed in a laboratory setting.
But this idea that sex drive is a way to forage
for potential love partners and that love is a litmus test
for whether or not longer term or deeper attachments
can and will form, is one that at least makes sense to me.
Later in the episode, we'll talk about this notion
of sex drive and desire.
I'll actually talk about some tools that have very strong
data really to support them in terms of things that people
can do or take to increase libido, both men and women,
because there's actually quite good data on that now.
But in the meantime, I want to talk about some of the work
that Dr. Fisher has done in terms
of categorizing people into...
Again, we have four groups.
These are distinct from the A, B, C, and D
attachment styles described earlier.
Although as I describe them,
you might be able to map them somewhat onto those.
And these four groups are groups that were defined
through her studies of people that were, or are,
I don't know if they were or if they are still on match.com,
but a very extensive dataset.
So again, millions if not tens of millions of individuals.
The number I heard her quote,
and forgive me if this is not accurate,
is that in upwards of 40 million individuals,
in terms of whether or not their neurochemical
and hormone systems are tuned
toward particular types of behaviors.
What do I mean by that?
Well, both men and women, males and females
have both testosterone and estrogen.
Typically, again, these are averages, but typically
men have more testosterone than they do estrogen,
and they have more testosterone than do women,
and less estrogen then do women.
Typically women have more estrogen
than they do testosterone, again, averages.
And they have less testosterone than men,
more estrogen than men, and so on and so forth.
But both hormone systems are active
in both sets of individuals.
And of course, all humans, as far as we know,
manufacture both dopamine and serotonin.
Dopamine, as I mentioned earlier, has a number of effects
in the brain and body.
But one of the primary effects is that it places us
into states of motivation and pursuit for various things.
There is somewhat close relationship between the dopamine
system and the testosterone system in the hypothalamus,
this brain area above the roof of your mouth.
And the pituitary gland, which is responsible
for making hormones that make the ovaries and or testes,
secrete testosterone or estrogen.
So there's a lot of signaling that occurs such that dopamine
and testosterone tend to operate as close cousins
in a system of pursuit.
And conversely, the serotonin system tends to on average
collaborate with the estrogen system to impart certain
physiological functions and behaviors.
So these aren't hard and fast, or I guess better stated,
these aren't strict black and white categorizations,
but I think those general themes hold
when you look at the animal and human data.
So Dr. Fisher has taken some liberties, but I think they are
what I would call logically, and scientifically,
and neurobiologically grounded liberties
in classifying individuals who are on these dating sites
according to the types of things they report
about themselves and the type of people they tend
to match with on these dating sites,
and created these four categories.
The four categories are, she calls one,
the dopamine category.
So these are people who would have high dopamine.
And again, that's just a name.
It doesn't mean they have low anything else,
but they are high on the dopamine scale.
People who rate high on the dopamine scale tend to be
what the scientists and psychologists call
high sensation seeking, novel seeking.
They like new things, they like spontaneity,
they tend to be very adventurous.
And I think that's largely true.
If you look at the conditions
where dopamine is super physiological,
it's elevated beyond abnormal levels.
Things like mania or when people take certain drugs
of abuse, like cocaine or amphetamine
that really raise dopamine levels up very, very high.
For some period of time, they do tend to increase
energy motivation and novelty seeking.
And of course, drugs like amphetamine and cocaine
have all sorts of deleterious effects
that I don't need to go into here,
but it's worth pointing out.
But they don't tend to make people calm and relaxed
and seek soothing interactions.
Conversely, the group that Dr. Fisher calls
the serotonin group tend to be more grounded
in soothing activities, quiescent type activities.
They actually tend to be, on average,
they tend to like rules and follow rules,
they tend to be homebodies, this sort of thing.
They're really, you can imagine them this stable types,
but they really like stability.
They're not really into spontaneity as much,
again, averages.
And then she created two other categories,
the testosterone category of high testosterone.
This again, could be males or females.
And then the estrogen category,
again, could be males or females.
And she gave these different names
that I won't go into here.
You can look up her work online,
but she names like the director, and the follower,
and things like that.
But I don't really want to use those as much as I want to stick
to the biological terms.
So we have dopamine, serotonin, testosterone, and estrogen.
Now that might seem like an unfair overgeneralization,
but what's interesting is not necessarily the name
or the neurochemical system, right?
Those could have just been called category one, two, three
and four for all that matters here.
What is interesting is seeing how those different groups
of individuals that she absolutely can categorize
based on their self-reported preferences
about behaviors and certain kinds of interactions.
How those groups tend to pair up with people
in the same or opposite categories.
So what her studies reveal is that people that fall
into the high sensation seeking, novelty seeking,
spontaneity category, the one that she calls
the high dopamine category tend to pair up with, at least
in the short term, tend to pair up with people
who are also in that dopaminergic category.
So these would be people that would spontaneously
take a trip or explore something new or new restaurant.
They tend to be creative and explorative types.
So that group on average tends to date and mate,
and potentially form long-term
relationships within category.
Again, averages.
Individuals that she placed into the serotonin group
or what she hypothesized would be a high serotonin group,
again, they didn't measure serotonin,
but people that tend to place value on stability, on rules,
on certain forms of traditional organization
at home and in relationships,
those people also tended to pair up with select date,
we presume mate with and form stable relationships
with people in the same category.
Now, individuals in the other two categories,
the high testosterone group,
and again, testosterone wasn't measured,
but she called it the high testosterone group,
but these are people that tend to be very directive,
they tend to know what they want and are comfortable telling
other people what they want.
And from them, these are individuals that in her studies
and in other studies tend to be a little bit challenging,
meaning they not necessarily challenging to be around,
but they tend to challenge other people,
push them in order to expand their boundaries,
either for sake of the relationship or just in general.
And the people they tend to push are the people that
they pair up with, which are the people
in the estrogen category, what she called high estrogen.
Again, they didn't measure estrogen,
but the people in the estrogen category were the ones
that described themselves and their choices in life
and their preferences as being nurturing.
They actually seem to like it when someone else
is making the major decisions, not every decision.
They certainly like to be heard, of course,
in terms of their preferences.
But that those two types, what she called the testosterone
and the estrogen type tend to pair up.
So why are these categorizations
and these averages interesting to me,
at least interesting enough to convey to you?
The reason they're interesting to me is,
again, not because of their names,
these molecules were not measured in these individuals,
but that they once again bring us to the themes
that we addressed before,
which are the autonomic nervous system and whether or not
it tends to be shifted more towards alertness in action
or more towards a stable equilibrium or more towards calm,
and whether or not individuals are selecting for people
who have autonomic nervous systems that are more or less
like theirs before they even meet, right?
So again, going back to this seesaw analogy,
it's almost like people who have the flat seesaw,
alert but calm, but not extremely alert,
not extremely overly calm in situations but in the middle,
seem to be seeking out people that are also
at that autonomic equilibrium.
People in the, what she called the dopamine category,
which are really can just be described
as high sensation seeking, novelty seeking,
they seem to want to pair with one another.
So there's a selection for similar,
in two of the groups, autonomic tone.
I find that very interesting because in that decision
or that preference for similar autonomic tone,
it essentially eliminates a lot of the requirement
for figuring out how to match
one's autonomic nervous system to another.
They simply find someone with a similar tendency, okay?
Whereas in the other two groups that she called
testosterone and estrogen, the director type
and the nurturing somewhat follower type,
there's an establishment of balance,
but not in between two who individuals as a match,
but rather on the whole in the relationship.
One person is driving the novelty seeking
in the course of decisions and actions,
and the other person is essentially agreeing to those.
Now, assuming that those decisions are good for both people.
And I emphasize good for both people,
because one of the themes that Dr. Fisher underscores,
and I'd like to underscore here as well,
is that it need not be the case that people pair up
exactly according to these categorizations
that I've described, dopamine with dopamine,
serotonin with serotonin, testosterone with estrogen,
and so on.
What is important is that there be a recognition
and a respect for the other types,
or a recognition and a respect for the fact that
both are of the same type.
You could actually imagine, for instance, that two people
of this high sensation seeking, novelty seeking
could have a terrifically exciting relationship,
but that it actually might be a relationship
in which the financial stability isn't quite there,
or in which the basic stability isn't there.
You could imagine, for instance, a situation
in which a relationship between two people
of what she called the high serotonin preference
would have a relationship that was actually dull,
in which both of them found themselves bored at some point,
or in which there wasn't enough of the dynamic tension
that sometimes is required in order to keep this cycle
of desire, love, and attachment going.
Something that we will talk about in a moment.
So the point here is not that one should necessarily pair up
according to these arrangements that I described.
The point is that on average, that's what tends to happen.
And that through a recognition that these categorizations
exist similar to the recognition that the type
A, B, C, and D infant and toddler type attachments exist,
that we can gain better self-awareness of who we are
and how we tend to show up in romantic attachments,
and thereby navigate healthier mate seeking,
healthier breakups, if the case dictates it,
and in some cases healthy long-term relationships
by understanding that the other person can either be similar
or complimentary to us.
One is neither better than the other.
It's simply the case that in all romantic attachments,
from the initial inception of the romantic attachment,
desire, love, and attachment,
there is an autonomic coordination.
And of course there's coordination of all sorts
of other things like food, sex, and sleep, and finances,
and where people are going to live, and many other features,
but that at the core of all that is a seeking
of either autonomic likeness or autonomic differences.
And I think that recognition can be extremely valuable
in thinking about tools to enter and maintain relationships.
If one thinks about their autonomic nervous system,
not simply as something that is driven by external people
and events, but that we can actually gain some control over
through techniques of the sort that I talked about earlier
and on previous podcasts,
but also generally, if we are able to adjust
our autonomic nervous system in order to at least appreciate
or get some empathy into what someone else is experiencing,
then we gain actual cognitive empathy.
And this episode isn't about empathy per se,
but the theme keeps coming up again and again.
And I think it's worth mentioning that when you talk
to psychologists, whether or not they're psychoanalyst
or from another source of training,
what you find is that they don't talk about empathy
as a general term, they will talk about emotional empathy,
they'll talk about cognitive empathy.
And what I'm talking about here today is that
yet a third category that is very strongly determinant
of relationship dynamics, and that's autonomic empathy.
I'm a biologist, I'm not a psychologist.
So I love mechanism.
And fortunately there are studies that have been done
recently using modern techniques
to look at neural mechanisms of romantic attachment.
I mentioned earlier some of the brain imaging studies
that have been done on child and mother literally imaging
the activity of neurons in the brain as child is nursing,
or as a mother is soothing baby,
and as you learned earlier, baby is soothing mother as well.
Those are remarkable studies.
You may have seen some of these pictures online.
You can see the silhouette of the infant and mother,
and their brains and even some of the brain activation
patterns, really, really beautiful studies.
Similar studies have been done in romantic couples
with those couples, either touching one another,
touching and kissing, or in clever, I think,
control experiments of the person just touching a pillow,
[laughs] or something, or kissing a pillow
in order to try and create the most reasonable control
for what are actually pretty
complicated interpersonal dynamics
to do in a brain imaging scanner.
But some of the other studies that have been done recently
involved so called EEG.
So these are electrical recordings
that are done noninvasively,
putting a bunch of electrodes on the outside of the scalp.
EEG is useful in that you can do it noninvasively,
you can do it while people are moving and doing things,
kissing, touching, et cetera.
It doesn't allow one to image or to evaluate neural activity
very deeply in the brain.
So you can miss out on a lot of things.
It's like looking at the wave structure on the ocean
without actually looking into the depths of the ocean.
So you can miss certain things,
but if you see things generally you trust they're there,
but you can't see what you don't see.
Nonetheless, there's some studies
that I'll just point you to and that form the segue
for what I'm going to discuss in a moment,
which is a study published in Scientific Reports in 2021,
entitled "Investigating Real Life Emotions
in Romantic Couples: a Mobile EEG Study."
So this is, as the title suggests, having people wear
these EEG caps of electrodes, get engage in very passionate,
emotional kisses, emotional speech toward one another
standing at different distances.
So a lot of cool stuff that you can do that you really
couldn't do in a brain scanner, because in a brain scanner,
people have to be there usually in a bite bar,
they're actually jaw flicked like this.
I've been in one of these things.
There's not a lot of moving around to be heard,
at least not using the current technology.
In any case, what they found was
there is a shift in brain waves, brain states,
things like alpha waves or just a particular frequency
of brain waves in the neocortex, the outer shell
of the brain just beneath the skull.
And in people that are kissing or in people that are engaged
in romantic speech, or I didn't actually hear what they said
to one another, but what the couple seems exciting,
romantic and arousing to them, they see more alpha wave
activity compared to the control conditions.
And there was some what we call lateralization,
where the left hemisphere was more active than the right
and so forth.
And these studies are important because we know that
the autonomic nervous systems of individuals tend
to start to collaborate and actually synchronize
at the level of heartbeats, at the level of breathing
during romantic interactions of different kinds.
But these studies are some of the first
of their kind to start looking
at neural synchronization between individuals.
Now, the simple version of looking at this
and the way I would've thought this would all go was,
okay, two people start kissing, they start talking
about what they find particularly romantic
and arousing for them.
And their brain waves will just match to one another.
And that's really the basis of romantic attachment
and romantic engagement in that sort of thing.
But it turns out that the opposite is true.
So a really nice study published in a really fine journal,
Cerebral Cortex is a journal that I've known
about for many years, they published strong anatomy,
physiology and neuroimaging.
There's a study that was published, first author, Kajimura,
and this paper really points...
Again, this is 2021.
And the title of this paper is,
"Brain Knows Who Is on the Same Wavelength:
Resting-State Connectivity Can Predict Compatibility
of a Female-Male Relationship."
Now what this study did was a little bit different.
They looked at the resting or default mode
activity of the brain.
So rather than evoked activity, as it's called,
where people are kissing or are engaged in some activity,
this was a neuroimaging study, not EEG, but fMRI,
functional magnetic resonance imaging,
which is similar to EEG in principle,
but allows you to look deep into the brain.
And it has a very good resolution in time and space.
So fast events can be monitored and the precise location
of those events can be monitored somewhat better than EEG.
And there are exceptions to this.
So for you EEGers out there.
EEG don't come after me with electrodes.
Just understand that fMRI gives you a fuller picture
of what's going on.
And what Kajimura et al found was that contrary
to what your reflexive prediction might be,
people tend to select people that have resting brain states
that different than theirs,
or sometimes they found that are actually opposite
to their own resting brain state.
And you might say, "Well, that doesn't make any sense.
I thought this is all about autonomic coordination."
But actually if we go back to Helen Fisher's categorizations
of the dopamine types, the sensation seeking types,
that is serotonin, the stable rule following types,
testosterone and estrogen types,
remember that the two categories that she called
testosterone and estrogen type,
the director and the follower, the nurturer,
I guess it would be the more accurate way,
the director and the nurturer,
those tend to pair up across categories,
not within category.
And so I think what's really needed for this field
which to my knowledge hasn't happened yet,
is to really start to map the neuroanatomical
and neurophysiological findings of, in this case,
that resting brain state is in one form in one individual,
and they tend to seek out people whose resting brain state
is different than theirs, not similar.
That needs to be mapped onto the more subjective
psychological categorizations that Helen Fisher
and indeed the Gottman's and others have created,
that sort of the state of the field now.
And I mentioned this not to confuse you,
but to the contrary, to illustrate that it's not just
about finding someone just like you,
and it's not just about finding someone
who's opposite to you.
This is actually the reason that I decided to become
a biologist at some point in my life, which is that
we can find verbal sayings, and stories, and examples
to support just about anything.
This is not a knock on the field of psychology,
as you can probably tell from today's episode.
I have great respect for in reverence
for the field of psychology, especially its collaboration
with neuroscience and vice versa.
But in the popular culture,
we can find examples and sayings that support essentially
anything as it relates to a relationship.
For instance, I've heard and you've probably heard,
absence makes the heart grow fonder.
And indeed I've experienced that and I believe it's true.
But I also have experienced and I believe to be true
that out of sight out of mind also exists
and that there will be a biological mechanism for that.
The point here is that matching of same to same
or same to different can both be effective in creating
the desire, love, attachment process.
It's a matter of, who is looking for same,
and who is looking for different?
And there, I think Dr. Fisher and the work
of these neurophysiologist and brain imagers
really does point in a direction whereby
there is not one form of attachment
that is going to be wholly above [indistinct]
will predict good outcomes.
There is not going to be a case in which opposites attract,
and that's always the best rule to follow.
Sometimes it will, sometimes it won't.
There is also not the case that people tend
to pair up with similar.
Sometimes it will be the case, sometimes it won't.
Now there are certain statistics
that support that statement.
For instance, people, on average,
people pair up with individuals of similar educational
background, income, and attractiveness.
That is true on average, but it's not always the case.
And again, a knowledge of and a respect for the different
categorizations of attachment, the different categorizations
of mate seeking described by Fisher and others,
and the recognition that matching of autonomic nervous
systems but also mismatching of resting state brain networks
are all at play in driving what we are calling
desire, love and attachment.
So in keeping with the exploration of the fact that
there's a saying, or a book, or a song, or an example
of pretty much any relationship dynamic,
I want to now talk about an article that came out a little
over 10 years ago that talked about the universality of love
and the ability to fall in love.
So this would be very much in line
with the George Bernard Shaw quote that I mentioned earlier
that "Love is really overestimating
the differences between individuals."
And again, I should say that is not something
that I personally believe,
although maybe I'm just diluting myself.
I like to think that the people that we fall in love
with are really special for us,
that they could not easily be replaced with anybody else.
That's simply my stance.
I'm not basing that
on any hardcore neurobiological mechanism.
But nonetheless, an article was published
in The New York Times in 2015 that related
to some psychological studies that were done,
as well as some clinical work, as well as some,
what I would call pop psychology or things that fall
outside the domains of academic science.
And the whole basis of this article was
36 questions that lead to love.
And it involved a listing out indeed of 36 questions,
divided into set one, set two, and set three that progressed
from somewhat ordinary questions about life experience
and self-report to more,
let's call them deep questions about people's values
and things that are emotionally close to them.
And I'll just give an example of a few of these.
You can find this easily online by just putting
into your search engine, 36 questions that lead to love.
Some of the questions in set number one, where for instance,
what would constitute a perfect day for you?
For what in your life do you feel most grateful?
Kind of standard questionnaire stuff.
In set two, what is your most treasured memory?
What is your most terrible memory?
So these, as you can tell, are drilling a little bit deeper
into one's personal experience and emotional system.
And then set three, questions 25 through 36 are things,
what is a very embarrassing moment in your life?
When did you last cry in front of another person
and by yourself?
What is something that's too serious to be joked about?
So it's going deeper into one's emotional system.
And even questions like, of all the people in your family,
whose death would you find most disturbing and why?
So pretty, pretty heavy stuff there at the end.
Now, the reason this article got so much traction,
and the reason I'm bringing it up today is that
there was a statement that was made in and around
this article that if two people went on a date
or simply sat down and asked each other these questions,
and each answered these questions and the other was paying
attention carefully and at some level emotionally responding
or not out responding, but certainly paying attention
to the answers of the other person,
that by the end of that exchange, where one person asks
36 questions and the other person answers all 36,
and then the other person asks all 36 and the other person
answers all 36, that they would fall in love, right?
Which seems like a ridiculous thing.
And yet it is the case that people who go
through this exercise report feeling as if they know
the other person quite well and feeling certain levels
of attachment or even love and desire for the other person
that they were, would not have predicted, excuse me,
would not have predicted
had they not gone through that process.
So what's going on in this exchange of questions
and answers of a progressively more emotional
and deep level?
Well, what I predict is going on is that inside
of that exchange, people are creating a delusional story
about the nature of the exchange being a reflection
of some deeper attachment.
And so even though people are just exchanging words,
they're not physically touching, they are not, at least not
at the point where they're running these kinds
of questionnaire studies.
They may touch afterwards for all I know,
and probably did in some cases,
but they're not exchanging life experience
in an immediate way.
They're not actually going off into the world
and doing things together yet.
They are simply exchanging narrative.
But we know based on recent studies and I've covered
this before on this podcast, but I'll mention again,
there was a study published in Cell Reports,
a Cell Press journal, excellent journal
showing that when individuals listen to the same narrative,
their heart rates tend to synchronize
or at least follow a very similar pattern,
even if they're not in the same room
listening to a given narrative.
Whereas in this case, people are facing one another
listening to the narratives of each other.
Certainly they are having autonomic responses.
And it stands to reason that their autonomic nervous systems
are synchronizing much in the same way that
the Cell Report study found that people will synchronize
their autonomic nervous systems to a shared,
heard story from another.
In other words, whether or not we hear a story,
watch a movie, listen to a song,
or exchange our own individual stories,
our autonomic nervous systems have the potential
to map onto one another.
So I'm not all that surprised that people find
that they fall in love, in quotes,
after answering these questions to one another,
because essentially the way these questions
are laid out is they establish a narrative.
They establish a very personal narrative
and the other person is listening very closely.
And we don't have physiological or brain imaging studies
to support what I'm about to say,
but the reasonable interpretation is that
that's causing some autonomic synchronization.
So if you want to try this on a date or even it's actually
been hypothesized that this could be useful
for existing couples, even if they already know the answers
to some of these questions.
And that doesn't surprise me either.
I think the autonomic coordination is present
during mating behavior,
it's present during shared experience of the outside world,
movies, concerts, watching one's children
with somebody else, et cetera.
And it's established by sharing one's own narrative
of their own personal experience.
So I don't want to seem overly reductionist.
I'll never propose that all of our sensation, perception,
action and experience in life boils down to us just being
bags of chemicals and the action of those chemicals,
or any aspect of our nervous system.
And yet in looking across the psychological literature
of development of attachment, in the psychological
literature of adult and romantic attachment,
and what makes and breaks those attachments,
it's very clear to me.
And I think courses through the literature multiple levels,
that autonomic coordination is absolutely key
for the establishment of desire, love and attachment.
In fact, I talked earlier about how our actual conception
is born out of autonomic coordination
of one sort or another.
So again, it doesn't necessarily mean that autonomic nervous
systems always be synchronized in the case of the two
categorizations that Fisher proposed
of the director/testosterone type
and the nurturing follower/estrogen type.
It was actually the coordination but in opposite directions
of individuals that fall into each of those categories
that led to more stable attachments
or the seeking out of those attachments, I should say.
But nonetheless, it's, at least to my mind, very clear
that autonomic coordination is a hallmark feature
of desire, a hallmark feature of what we call love,
and a hallmark feature of what we call attachment.
And that the breaking of attachments were the failures
of desire, the failures of love,
and the failures of attachment over time
in line with the work of Gottman and others.
And in even just simply what's required for mating behavior
is also reflected in the autonomic nervous system.
But in that case, a failure to coordinate
the autonomic nervous systems in some sort of concerted way.
Any discussion about desire, love, and attachment
would be incomplete
if we didn't talk about the dreaded infidelity and cheating.
Much has been made of infidelity and cheating
and whether or not people who are higher on dopamine
and sensation seeking tend to cheat more or less.
Frankly, I don't think there's any solid evidence for that.
I think there are a lot of examples that we can draw
from in our own lives and in the lives of others
that would generally support one or the other model,
but I'm not aware of any decent physiological studies
or psychological studies that really point to that.
For instance, I would never say that the serotonergic
phenotype as described by Fisher is less prone to cheat,
or that the people who have an insecure attachment
are more likely to cheat, et cetera, for instance.
I don't think those correlations have been drawn
in any kind of meaningful way yet.
So I would be cautious about assigning them
without that evidence.
However, there are some interesting studies involving,
again, neuroimaging and some subjective measures in humans,
meaning asking them questions
that they're good ways to tease out lies from truths
in these sorts of studies and whether or not people tend
to find their partner or others more or less attractive
depending on how people feel about themselves.
And I think this is a very interesting aspect
to desire, love, and attachment for the following reason.
You hear a lot out out there that in order
to form a really strong relationship,
you have to have a good relationship with yourself,
or you have to love yourself.
Or you often hear for instance that it's exactly when you're
not looking for a relationship that you're going to find one.
You hear this stuff, right?
But none of that is really grounded in any studies.
Again, that's like out of sight out of mind,
or absence makes the heart grow fonder.
There are many life examples to support those statements.
And there are many life examples
to support statements to the opposite.
There's a particular study that I found.
This was published in Frontiers in Psychology,
but it's a experimental study that involves neuroimaging.
The title of this study is "Manipulation of Self-Expansion
Alters Responses to Attractive Alternative Partners."
And I love the design of this study.
What they did in this study is they took couples
and they evaluated members of that relationship
for what's called self-expansion.
Now self-expansion is a metric that involves
one's perception of self
as seen through the relationship to the other.
And this is something that was developed by,
the authors are Aron and Aron.
So they have the same last name.
So I'm assuming this was either a sibling team
or a somehow related team or a romantic couple team.
A-R-O-N and A-R-O-N, Aron and Aron, in 1986,
proposed this self-expansion model of close relationships.
And they proposed "That people are motivated
to enter relationships..."
I'm reading here.
"In order to enhance the self and increase self-efficacy."
In other words, that one of the reasons why many people
enter relationships is that it makes us feel good
about ourselves and more capable.
And I would see that as a healthy interdependence,
not necessarily codependence.
This is especially strong at the beginning
of a relationship, it turns out,
when people are forming pair bonds,
and it's the case that pleasure, arousal and excitement,
again, all hallmark features
of autonomic nervous system function,
pleasure, arousal and excitement,
give rise to self-expansion, meaning to self-efficacy.
So what this self-expansion model is really about is
how great other people that we are close to and romantically
attached to can potentially make us feel,
in terms of what they say, in terms of what they do,
in terms of the way in which we believe they feel about us.
So it doesn't necessarily have to involve explicit
statements of them telling us how great we are,
or them doing great gestures for us,
but how we actually feel they feel about us.
Turns out to be a very strong parameter in terms
of how we feel about ourselves and the relationship overall.
Now, some of you out there are probably thinking,
"Oh yeah, isn't there this thing
of the love languages?" Right?
I don't have any neuroscience to support, I think,
the love languages, I'm not super familiar with this,
and I didn't list it out, but that some people are...
Their autonomic nervous system, if you will,
tends to be very responsive to gifts, or to quality time,
or to physical touch, or acts of kindness.
I think I've got a few of these right.
I probably have a few wrong.
Anyway, they're easy to find online.
And people do tend to have a bias toward two or three
of these things that are especially
meaningful for them.
And when I hear meaningful, I hear they tend to push
the autonomic nervous system and neurochemical systems
of the brain and body in a direction that makes us feel good
as opposed to lousy or neutral.
In any event, this study looked at whether or not
people have high levels of self-expansion
through the actions or statements
of their significant other, and how that influences
their perception of people outside the relationship.
Meaning how attractive they perceive people
outside the relationship to be,
turns out to be strongly influenced
by, A, whether or not their self-expansion is very strongly
driven by the other person that they are involved with,
that they're in the romantic relationship with,
and whether or not that's being expressed to them.
So here's how the study went.
First of all, they rated or categorized individuals
on the basis of the self-expansion metric.
Some people have more of a potential to experience
self-expansion through others, right?
Some of us feel great about ourselves and we're topped off,
the others don't feel so great about themselves,
but they can feel much better in response to praise,
in particular praise or self-expansion type behaviors
or statements from people that we really care about.
And still other people are a mixture of the two.
They moderate levels of both.
So they rated them on this scale.
And then they had people
experience self-expansion narratives.
They heard their significant others say really terrific
things about them and about the relationship, in particular,
that the relationship that they have was exciting,
novel and challenging.
So that was one form of self-expansion.
And they went into some detail as to why that was the case
in their particular relationship.
Or they heard a narrative from their significant other
about strong feelings of love between the two
that had been experienced previously in the relationship.
So in the one case, it directed more towards them.
And in the other case,
it's more about the relationship itself.
And then they did brain imaging of one person
in the relationship while that person assessed
the attractiveness of people outside the relationship.
And what they found was that people who were primed
for this self-expansion had lower activation of brain areas
associated with assessing others' attractiveness
than did the people who experienced a lot of self-expansion.
Now the takeaway from that, at least the way I read
the study is if you're with somebody who really benefits
from or experiences a lot of self-expansion,
unless you really want them to pay attention
to the attractiveness of other people,
it stands to reason that they would benefit
from more self-expansion type gestures or statements, okay?
Not so much centered on the relationship.
We have such a great relationship.
There's so much love, it's so great, that too,
but in the context of this study and these findings,
that the person is really terrific, that the relationship
that they've created together is really exciting,
novel, and challenging, that there's a narrative around
the relationship that really has a lot to do
with the dynamics between the individuals, in particular,
that the person who really likes self-expansion
is vital to that dynamic, okay?
So it's not looking down at the relationship
as a set of equals.
There is this bias written into this of that
this person is really essential for the relationship.
I'm not saying this is something that anyone has to do.
I'm not not saying this is right or wrong.
This is just what the data say.
But what's remarkable is that in the absence
of those statements, people who have,
or that rate high on this scale of self-expansion
rate attractive alternative partners as more attractive.
Now, that's interesting to me, because it may means that
their actual perception of others is changing.
It's not that their opportunity
to see others is changing, right?
This is not a matter of them somehow getting access
or no access to attractive alternative partners.
Again, attractive alternative partners,
literally the language in the title of this paper,
they're still seeing all these attractive people.
It's just that if they're feeling filled up, in air quotes,
psychologically filled up, emotionally filled up,
autonomically filled, enhanced in the language
that we're using today by the self-expansion narrative,
well then the same set of attractive faces appear
less attractive to a given individual.
Now, whether or not this predicts cheating or loyalty,
[laughs] I certainly can't say.
That would be very hard to assess in neuroimaging.
And there, of course, people rarely if ever
report accurately their cheating behavior.
There's some studies in which confidentiality is assured
to the point where people seem to be more trusting
and willing to reveal cheating behavior.
But if you look at the statistics on cheating behavior,
it's very hard to track because people lie all the time
about their cheating in and outside of the context
of psychological and neuroimaging studies.
But I find this study, again, the title,
"Manipulation of Self-expansion Alters Responses
to Attractive Alternative Partners"
to be absolutely fascinating, because again, it points
to the fact that the interactions with our significant
others shapes our autonomic arousal, shapes our perception
of self and thereby shapes our perception of other potential
partners in the outside world,
or shuts us down to the potential of other people
in the outside world.
So when I hear statements such as,
it's important that you love yourself
in order to really fall in love with somebody else,
or it is when one is not looking for a relationship
that they're most likely to fall in love
and form a stable relationship,
I can filter that through these findings to say that
it's really the person who needs a lot of self-expansion,
stimulating statements or actions coming from other people
that is most prone to seeing other potential partners
out in the world as attractive.
And in this sense,
we can return to the autonomic nervous system
as a glass that it can be filled up
through various contexts.
It can be filled up through our own ability to regulate it.
It can be filled up through other people's ability
to enhance our sense of wellbeing.
And in some sense, this points to an idea where it is true
that the better that we can feel about ourselves
in the absence of any self-expansion type input
from somebody else really does place us
on more stable grounds, such that when we do receive
that praise, or we do receive those acts of kindness,
or service, or physical touch or whatever they are,
that we are able to further enhance the way that we feel,
but that we don't necessarily tether all of our feelings
of self-worth or self-expansion to that one individual.
So you might think that if person A can only receive
the self-expansion from the statements, from the action
of the they're involved with, person B,
that that will form a very stable bond,
but what this study points to is the fact that
that's a very unstable bond.
That person A is actually very susceptible
to the attractiveness of others,
because they're so desperately attached to this notion
of self-expansion, even if they don't realize it.
And so this really does point to the idea that,
well, it is important to link our autonomic nervous systems
to establish desire, love, and attachment
that we want to have a stable internal representation
of ourselves, a stable autonomic nervous system
to some degree in other so that we can be in stable
romantic partnership with another individual,
if that what we're really trying to do.
So until now, I've been weaving together studies
from the field of experimental psychology
and the fields of neuroscience in particular neuroimaging.
But if you recall back to the very beginning of the episode,
when I was discussing how odors, and how hormones,
and how even birth control can shape people's ratings
of attractiveness of others,
you'll realize that there's a deeper layer to all this,
which is that our biology that resides below our conscious
awareness, things like our hormones,
things like pheromones even are shaping the way
that we choose, interpret and act with other potential
romantic partners or the romantic partners
that we already have.
Now, this cannot be over-emphasized, right?
No matter how much we would like to create
a top-down description, meaning from the cortex
and our understanding of things
onto what we find attractive, who we find attractive,
what we enjoy, what we don't enjoy in the pursuit
in romantic interactions with others,
there always seems to be, and indeed, there always is
a deeper layer in which our subconscious processing
drives us to find a particular person
to be particularly attractive,
or in which we have chemistry with somebody,
or in which we lack chemistry with somebody.
And I would say that one of the more exciting, fascinating,
and indeed mysterious aspects of desire, love and attachment
are those subconscious processes,
those things that we call chemistry, right?
I mean, people will report, for instance, that somebody's
smell is just absolutely positively intoxicating for them,
or that somebody's smell is absolutely repulsive to them
and they don't know why.
That the taste of someone's breath, and I don't mean that
in any kind of poetic sense, I literally mean the taste
of somebody's breath in some cases
can be very exciting to somebody.
And believe it or not, we can taste each other's breath.
I talked about this in the chemical sensing episode
some months back, but we actually have receptors for taste
and smell that engage in coordinated action,
such that we can't really separate taste and smell
at some level.
And this is especially true when it comes to the formation
of romantic relationships and what we call chemistry.
Now is chemistry absolutely required
for forming stable attachments for love and for desire?
No, of course, they're not.
But in general, these are primitive mechanisms
that exist in all animals,
they exist in special forms in humans,
but that they drive us toward behaviors that will,
as the theory goes, lead to love and attachment.
Not always, as Dr. Fisher pointed out that sex and sex drive
is one way to explore potential love relationships,
and to explore potential attachments,
which, of course, are major investments
that extend well beyond one night, or a week, or a vacation,
or even a year.
When we talk about stable attachments,
in general, that means long-term attachments in humans.
Now, there is a biology to all of that chemistry stuff.
And the studies of oral contraception and men finding women
more attractive at certain phases of their menstrual cycle,
and women finding men more attractive at certain phases
of the women's menstrual cycle point to the incredible power
of those deeper biological mechanisms.
In the "Huberman Lab Podcast,"
I discuss both science and science-based tools.
And so I'd be remiss if I didn't actually cover
some of the tools that relate
to those deeper biological mechanisms.
Now, the hormones, testosterone and estrogen
are almost always the first biological chemicals
and hormones that are mentioned, and described, and explored
when thinking about desire, and love and attachment, too,
for that matter since love and attachment stem from desire.
I did an entire episode about the biology of testosterone
and estrogen and ways to optimize testosterone and estrogen.
You can easily find that episode at hubermanlab.com.
It's timestamped, there you can find all sorts
of information about how certain behaviors or absence
of behaviors drive up or down testosterone and estrogen.
I also dispel some myths about sexual behavior
and things like masturbation
and how they relate to testosterone and estrogen,
as well as some myths about how those hormones change
across the lifespan.
I also talk about the role of exercise,
I talk about supplementation, and I also talk a little bit
about hormone replacement therapy,
although that will be the topic for a future episode.
So if you're interested in the biology of testosterone
and estrogen, two hormones that absolutely influence
things like libido and desire, please check out that episode
as well as what I'm going to talk about in just a moment here.
The simple stereotyped version of the hormones,
testosterone and estrogen are that testosterone drives
libido or increases it aka sex drive,
and that estrogen somehow blunts it or is not involved
in libido and sex drive.
And that is simply not the case.
As I describe in that testosterone and estrogen
optimization episode and as I'll tell you now,
yes, testosterone and some of its other forms
like dihydrotestosterone are strongly related to libido
and sex drive and the pursuit and ability to mate.
However, the hormone estrogen is also strongly associated
with libido and mating behavior.
So much so that for people that either chemically
or for some other reason have very low estrogen,
libido can severely suffer.
So it's a coordinated dance of estrogen and testosterone
in both males and females that leads to libido or sex drive.
So I absolutely want to make clear that it's not a simple
relationship between testosterone and sex drive,
or estrogen and sex drive.
Both are required at appropriate ratios.
Now, with that said, there are things that can shift libido
in both men and women in the direction of more desire
or more desire to mate, either to seek mates
or to mate with existing partners.
And there's a quite solid literature
around a few of those substances.
Now, a common misconception is that because dopamine
is involved in motivation and drive that simply increasing
dopamine through any number of different mechanisms or tools
will increase libido and sex drive.
And that's simply not the case either.
It is true that some level of dopamine or increase
in dopamine is required for increases in libido.
However, because of dopamine's relationship
to the autonomic nervous system,
and because the autonomic nervous system
is so intimately involved, no pun intended,
in sexual activity in seeking an actual mating behavior,
as I described earlier, it's actually the case that
if people drive their dopamine system too high,
they will be in states of arousal that are high enough
such that they seek and want sexual activity,
but they can't actually engage the parasympathetic arm
of the autonomic nervous system sufficient
to become physically aroused.
There's a whole description of this that awaits us
in a future episode.
But I'll summarize now by saying for people
that are taking substances just simply to increase dopamine
in order to increase libido,
that can be a potentially hazardous route to follow.
Because depending on whether or not that dopamine level
is high enough that it puts them into a mode of seeking
mates or mating, but they can't adjust their autonomic
nervous system during actual mating behavior,
what essentially is I'm saying is it can place people
into a chronic pursuit but in inability to perform sexually.
And this is true for men and women.
Okay, so I would just caution people against just thinking,
"Oh, a lack of libido is simply a lack of dopamine."
That is not the case.
It could be from lower levels of dopamine,
but it could also be for other reasons.
And so these systems, these signaling systems
and these neurochemicals are very intricate.
And just simply ramping up dopamine has actually been found,
for instance, in amphetamine and cocaine users,
there's a phenomenon in which they become hyper aroused,
but can't perform sexually.
This is also true for people who take elevated levels
of other recreational drugs or who take antidepressants
that increase the dopamine system too much, right?
Dosage has to be worked out with your physician,
with your psychiatrist, such that mood is enhanced
and the various aspects of a healthy wellbeing,
mind and body are enhanced, but not so much so that
that what we call the arousal arc is locked with the seesaw
in the sympathetic drive position
such that sexual arousal can't occur, okay?
So this is an important point to make,
because I think that a lot of people
are under the impression that
if they just drive up testosterone, increase dopamine,
and generally get themselves into high states
of autonomic arousal that that's going to increase the libido.
But that's simply not the way the system works.
It's that seesaw and that seesawing back and forth
that is the arc of arousal that we talked about earlier.
Now, there are substances, legal over-the-counter substances
that fall under the categorization of supplements
that do indeed increase libido and arousal.
And so I'm going to talk about some of those in the context
of peer-reviewed literature now.
I want to be clear, however,
that these are by no means required.
Many people have healthy libidos or have libidos
that are healthy for their life and what they need and want.
And as always in any discussion about supplementation,
you absolutely have to check with your physician.
I don't just say that to protect us.
I say that to protect you.
Your health and wellbeing is dependent on you doing certain
things and not doing others, and everybody is different.
Nonetheless, there are studies that point
to specific substances that are sold over the counter
that at least in the United States are legal
and that have been shown to be statistically significant
in increasing measures of libido.
There are many such substances, but three that in particular
have good peer-reviewed research to support them
are Maca, M-A-C-A, which is actually a root.
Tongkat Ali, also sometimes called longjack,
I didn't name them, forgive me, and Tribulus, or Tribulus,
it's sometimes called.
I'm going to talk about each of these in sequence.
But on the whole, the studies on Maca are quite convincing
that consumption of two to three grams per day of Maca,
which generally is sold as a powder or a capsule,
typically consumed early in the day,
because it can be somewhat of a stimulant,
meaning it can increase alertness and you wouldn't want it
to interfere with sleep by taking it too late in the day.
But in studies that include both men and women of durations
anywhere from eight to 12 weeks
of athletes and non-athletes,
and different variations of Maca,
turns out there's black Maca, red Maca, yellow Maca.
There are a bunch of different forms of Maca.
But that they can increase subjective reports
of sexual desire, independent of hormone systems.
Meaning, it does not seem at least based on the existing
literature that Maca increases testosterone
or changes estrogen, at least not on the time scales
that these studies were done,
or with the measures that were performed in these studies.
But that Maca, again, consumed in doses of anywhere
from two to three grams per day,
has been shown to significantly increase libido.
And in fact, those dosages of Maca have been shown to offset
so-called SSRI induced sexual dysfunction.
So there are various routes to sexual dysfunction.
The SSRIs are selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors.
They go by name brands like Prozac and Zoloft,
and there are many others now on generic forms and so forth.
Those don't always, I should point out,
lead to sexual dysfunction.
There's a dose dependence.
Some people do quite well on SSRIs and don't have any issues
with sexual function.
Other people suffer quite a lot from sexual dysfunction
while taking SSRIs, highly variable.
You need to work with a physician, a qualified psychiatrist.
But nonetheless, everything I've been saying about Maca
thus far has also been explored in the context of SSRI
induced sexual dysfunction.
The paper that I'm referring to here is
"A Double‐Blind, Randomized, Pilot Dose‐Finding Study
of Maca Root..."
It goes by the name L. meyenii.
These always have fancy names.
And the Latin names in biology are always more complicated,
but it's Maca root.
"for the Management of SSRI‐Induced Sexual Dysfunction."
First author is Dording, D-O-R-D-I-N-G.
This was a study done at Mass General,
which is one of the satellite locations around Harvard Med.
It's associated with Harvard Med,
that found significant improvements in libido
when people were taking a pretty low dose.
It was actually in this case, just 1.5 grams per day,
up to a high dose, three grams per day of Maca.
And they were doing this
in 20 remitted depressed outpatients.
So these are people that had depression,
their depression was successfully treated with SSRIs,
but they were suffering from some of these SSRI related
sexual effects and Maca seemed to offset
some of those effects significantly in this population.
The other studies exploring the lack of effect on serum
testosterone in adult healthy men was a 12-week study,
again, consuming anywhere from 1.5 to three milligrams,
meaning one, excuse me, 1500 milligrams
to 3000 milligrams or placebo.
So again, this is 1.5 up to three grams of Maca or placebo,
and they rated sexual desire, depression, and other measures
such as testosterone in the blood.
Again, no change in testosterone or estrogen
estradiol levels in men treated with Maca
and those treated with placebo.
But nonetheless, there was a significant and positive effect
on libido with this dosage of 1.5
to three grams per day of Maca.
And there are several other studies that also show this.
Again, in people that taking SSRIs and people that are not
taking SSRIs, in chronically over-trained athletes,
this was also found to be the case.
So seems like across the board,
Maca is a fairly useful supplement
for those that are seeking to increase their libido.
And there are fewer studies involving women,
but there are a few such studies that also point to the same
general positive effect on libido in women taking Maca
at equivalent doses to those I just described.
I think it's noteworthy that Maca supplementation
does not seem to adjust testosterone or estrogen levels
to any significant degree, but it does change libido.
I think that points to the fact that there are multiple
systems in the brain and body that influence libido,
not just testosterone and estrogen.
And indeed we know that to be the case.
Things like PEA, which is a substance found in chocolate
and is a substance that some people supplement,
is known, for instance, to increase sexual desire,
but also the perception of sexual experiences
as more stimulating for instance.
So there are a lot of pathways in the brain, in particular,
in the hypothalamus, this ancient area of our brain,
that harbors neurons and hormone secreting cells,
including neurons that can shape our perceptions
of even just our tactile experience
of others and their attractiveness.
And indeed can shift levels of desire, independent
of changing levels of circulating hormones.
Another substance that has been shown to increase libido
across a range of human populations
is so-called Tongkat Ali.
I've talked a little bit about this before
on the "Huberman Lab Podcast" in reference to testosterone.
And I've talked about it extensively as a guest
on other podcasts.
Tongkat Ali goes by a number of different names.
One of them is exceedingly difficult for me to pronounce.
It's Eurycoma longifolia, also called longjack.
But Tongkat Ali is the typical name.
This is an herb, there's a Malaysian version
and an Indonesian version.
My understanding is that the Indonesian variety
of Tongkat Ali is the one that is most potent
for its effects on libido.
Previously, I've talked about Tongkat Ali
taken in 400 milligram per day capsules
as a means to increase the amount of free,
meaning unbound testosterone.
So testosterone has a both bound form and an unbound form.
Very briefly, the bound form is bound to albumin
in the blood or to so-called sex hormone binding globulin.
When it's bound, it can't be biologically active
at many cells.
It is important that some of it be bound in order
to get a time release and proper distribution
of testosterone through the body,
but is the unbound free testosterone that can really
have its most potent effects.
And there's some evidence that Tongkat Ali can increase
the amount of unbound so-called free testosterone
by lowering sex hormone binding globulin,
although it is almost certain that it has other routes
of mechanism as well.
Nonetheless, there are some reports of Tongkat Ali
increasing libido.
One particular article, last author, or I should say
last name of first author, excuse me, Ismail, I-S-M-A-I-L.
This was published in Evidence-Based Complementary
and Alternative Medicine, it's from 2012.
Reports a significant increase in libido
and sexual function.
There are other studies, not a lot of them,
not as many robust, controlled, quality peer-reviewed
studies as there are from Maca.
Nonetheless, a number of people, men and women that I know
do take Tongkat Ali and it seems to work well for them.
The question always comes up around
discussion of supplements.
Do you need to cycle these things?
The only way to determine that is really
to do your blood work, monitor liver enzymes,
monitor hormone levels and so forth.
So I simply can't say whether or not you need to,
or you don't need to cycle them.
Typically Tongkat Ali and Maca are not cycled
in any regular kind of way that I'm aware of.
But again, you really need to check with your doctor
if you're going to initiate taking any of these things.
And you certainly should do your best to monitor
your blood work as well as subjective measures in evaluating
whether or not they're working for you,
safe for you and so forth.
The third and final substance/supplement
that I want to touch on as it relates to libido
is called Tribulus Terrestris.
So that's T-R-I-B-U-L-U-S, terrestris, T-E-R-R-E-S-T-R-I-S.
This is a commonly sold over-the-counter supplement
for increasing testosterone,
for fitness purposes and so on.
Whether or not it actually does that
to a meaningful degree, isn't clear.
But I'm aware of four peer-reviewed studies
that were focused on both males and females
ranging anywhere from 18 years old
all the way up to 65 plus.
They say 65 plus.
I guess it could be 70, it could be 80.
I don't know.
But a fairly broad age range where people took anywhere
from 750 milligrams per day divided into three equal doses.
So 750 total per day divided into three equal doses
of Tribulus or placebo for 120 days.
This particular study was focused on females.
And according to the female sexual function index
questionnaire, no significant difference
between any of the groups, however free and bio-available
testosterone increased in the group taking
Tribulus Terrestris, total testosterone
did not reach statistical significance.
So this is the inverse of what we see with Maca,
where there do seem to be increases in testosterone,
which would predict that there would be increase in libido.
In this case, this was postmenopausal women,
there was no increase in libido.
There was an increase in testosterone.
I mentioned it only because there might be instances
in which people want to increase their testosterone.
It does seem that Tribulus, at least in that population
is capable of doing that.
Now there's a separate study that was done,
a double-blind study lasting anywhere from one to six months
that had a clear and significant increase in libido.
Now this was taking six grams.
So that's 6,000 milligrams of Tribulus root for 60 days.
And it did seem to increase various aspects
of sexual function.
And there was what appeared to be a substantial 16.3%
increase in testosterone, but in this particular study
because of the variability across individuals
that did not actually arrive at statistical significance.
Now, there were a number of other studies that explored
the role of Tribulus, in particular, in females.
And one of those studies was a study
that was actually quite short, it was two to four weeks.
It involved 67 subjects.
These were subjects that had experienced a loss of libido
and took Tribulus divided into two equal doses,
compared that to placebo.
And they did see a significant improvement in these measures
of sexual desire and function
on this female sexual function index.
So there is some evidence that Tribulus can be effective
in increasing testosterone in certain populations,
in increasing sexual desire and function
in certain populations in particular in females.
I think more studies are certainly needed.
But these three substances/supplements,
Maca, Tongkat Ali, in particular, Indonesian Tongkat Ali,
and Tribulus can indeed create significant
increases in sexual desire.
And in some cases by adjusting the testosterone
and estrogen system, in some cases not by adjusting
the testosterone and estrogen system.
Again, pointing to the complexity of neurochemicals
and features that adjust things like libido aka desire.
So we covered a lot of material today related
to desire, love, and attachment.
And yet I acknowledge that it is not exhaustive
of the vast landscape that is the psychology and biology
of desire, love and attachment.
Nonetheless, I hope that you found the information
interesting and hopefully actionable in some cases
toward the relationships of your past, of present
and potentially for the relationships of your future.
If you're enjoying and or learning from this podcast,
please subscribe to our YouTube channel.
That's a very straightforward, zero cost way to support us.
And it really does help us.
In addition, please subscribe to our podcast
on Apple and Spotify.
And on Apple, you have the opportunity to leave us
up to a five star review.
In addition, please leave us comments, feedback,
and suggestions for future guests
in the comment section on YouTube.
We do read all of those comments.
Please also check out the sponsors mentioned
at the beginning of today's episode.
That is the best way to support the "Huberman Lab Podcast."
In addition, we have a Patreon,
it's patreon.com/andrewhuberman.
And there you can support the podcast
at any level that you like.
Please also follow us on Instagram and Twitter.
I teach neuroscience and neuroscience-related tools
on both Instagram and Twitter.
Some of that material overlaps
with material covered on the podcast.
Some of it is unique material only covered
on Instagram and Twitter.
During today's episode and on many previous episodes
of the "Huberman Lab Podcast," we discussed supplements.
While supplements aren't necessary or appropriate
for everybody, many people derive tremendous benefit
from them, for things like enhancing sleep, enhancing focus,
or as discussed today for enhancing libido and desire.
If you want to see the supplements that I take,
you can go to Thorne, that's thorne.com/u/huberman.
And there you can get 20% off the supplements that I take.
And if you navigate deeper into the Thorne site
through that portal, thorne.com/u/huberman,
you can also get 20% off any of the other supplements
that Thorne makes.
We partnered with Thorne because Thorne has the absolute
highest standards with respect to the quality
of the ingredients in their supplements
and the precision of the amounts of those supplements.
Thank you for joining me for today's discussion
about desire, love, and attachment.
And last but certainly not least,
thank you for your interest in science.
[upbeat music]