Science of Social Bonding in Family, Friendship & Romantic Love

Time: 0.36

- Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,

Time: 2.29

where we discuss science and science-based tools

Time: 4.92

for everyday life.

Time: 6.307

[upbeat music]

Time: 9.19

I'm Andrew Huberman,

Time: 10.35

and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology

Time: 13

at Stanford School of Medicine.

Time: 14.94

Today's episode is about the biology,

Time: 17.28

psychology and practices of social bonding.

Time: 20.76

From the day we are born until the day we die,

Time: 23.74

the quality of our social bonds

Time: 25.44

dictates much of our quality of life.

Time: 28.28

It should therefore be no surprise that our brain,

Time: 31.49

and indeed much of our entire nervous system

Time: 34.17

is wired for social bonds.

Time: 36.82

Now social bonds occur between infant and parent,

Time: 40.4

there even particular wiring diagrams within the brain

Time: 43.558

and spinal cord and body

Time: 45.81

that are oriented towards the specific bonds

Time: 47.666

that occur between infant and mother,

Time: 50.4

as well as infant and father.

Time: 52.9

And we have specific brain circuitries for friendship,

Time: 56.22

specific brain circuitries that are activated

Time: 58.83

in romantic relationships.

Time: 60.61

And as it goes, specific brain circuitries

Time: 63.285

that are activated when we break up with a romantic partner

Time: 66.661

or when they break up with us,

Time: 68.43

or when somebody passes away, moves away,

Time: 71.24

or otherwise leaves our lives in one form or another.

Time: 75.58

Today, we are going to talk about those brain

Time: 77.77

and nervous system circuitries,

Time: 79.47

we're also going to talk about the neurochemicals

Time: 81.45

and hormones that underlie their function.

Time: 84.3

And we are going to touch on a number of important

Time: 86.93

and actionable tools that you can apply in everyday life.

Time: 90.61

And because we are headed into the holiday,

Time: 93.15

the New Year and Christmas holiday,

Time: 95.88

that you can deploy in your various interactions

Time: 98.35

with family members and friends.

Time: 100.07

And should you not be spending time

Time: 102.07

with family members and friends,

Time: 103.16

today, we are also going to talk about

Time: 104.94

how to achieve social bonds

Time: 106.68

out of the context of family

Time: 108.91

and romantic partnership and friendship.

Time: 111.34

So today's episode is going to include a lot of science,

Time: 114.15

a lot of actionable tools,

Time: 115.85

and I'm confident that you will come away

Time: 117.79

from today's episode

Time: 119.14

with tremendous knowledge about how you function.

Time: 122.64

For instance, if you're an introvert or an extrovert,

Time: 125.03

why is that?

Time: 125.863

Turns out there may be a neurochemical basis for that.

Time: 128.28

Maybe you're somebody that really enjoys social media,

Time: 130.36

maybe you're somebody that doesn't.

Time: 131.71

Today I'm going to talk about a gene or a set of genes

Time: 134.74

that predicts whether or not you will follow more people

Time: 137.132

or seek out more online, social interactions or fewer.

Time: 141.67

Believe it or not, there's biology around that now,

Time: 143.76

and it's excellent peer reviewed work.

Time: 145.77

We will also talk about how bonds are broken,

Time: 149

and why breakups can be so painful,

Time: 150.797

not just romantic breakups,

Time: 152.54

but breakups with friendships and coworkers,

Time: 155.22

and how to move through those more seamlessly.

Time: 158.1

So regardless of your age,

Time: 159.66

and regardless of whether or not

Time: 160.87

you are in a romantic partnership

Time: 162.53

of one form or another, or not,

Time: 165.26

I do believe this episode will be useful to you

Time: 167.33

as you explore the social bonds

Time: 169.42

that already exist in your life,

Time: 170.86

and as you seek out new and changing social bonds.

Time: 175.19

Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize

Time: 176.76

that this podcast is separate

Time: 178.18

from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.

Time: 180.16

It is however, part of my desire and effort

Time: 182.07

to bring zero cost to consumer information about science

Time: 184.65

and science-related tools to the general public.

Time: 187.21

In keeping with that theme,

Time: 188.16

I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.

Time: 190.71

Our first sponsor is Roka.

Time: 192.43

Roka makes eyeglasses and sunglasses

Time: 194.39

that are of the absolute highest quality.

Time: 196.91

I've spent a lifetime working on the biology

Time: 198.39

of the visual system,

Time: 199.317

and the visual system has many important features

Time: 201.7

that allow us, for instance,

Time: 202.78

to see things with crystal clarity,

Time: 204.62

even if it's brightly lit or dimly lit,

Time: 206.84

or there are changing brightness in our environment.

Time: 210.12

Roka eyeglasses and sunglasses

Time: 212.32

are designed with the biology of the visual system in mind.

Time: 215.49

So when you wear them,

Time: 216.53

you don't have to take them off and put them back on

Time: 218.55

like you would with other types

Time: 219.61

of sunglasses and eyeglasses,

Time: 221.02

and you're always going to see things with crystal clarity.

Time: 223.67

In addition, they're very lightweight,

Time: 225.33

so usually forget that you're wearing them.

Time: 226.72

I certainly forget when I'm wearing mine.

Time: 228.13

I wear readers at night and when I drive,

Time: 229.96

and I wear sunglasses often throughout the day.

Time: 232.52

The other thing that's really great

Time: 233.5

about Roka eyeglasses and sunglasses

Time: 235.43

is that you can wear them doing anything.

Time: 238.09

You can wear them while running, while cycling,

Time: 240.68

if you get sweaty, they won't fall off your face.

Time: 243.12

But unlike a lot of other performance eyeglasses out there,

Time: 246.7

they have a terrific aesthetic

Time: 247.84

that you could also wear to work, to school, out to dinner,

Time: 251.16

they basically can be worn anywhere, anytime.

Time: 253.63

If you'd like to try Roka, eyeglasses and sunglasses,

Time: 255.97

you can go to Roka that's R-O-K-A.com

Time: 258.65

and enter the code Huberman

Time: 260.01

to save 20% off your first order.

Time: 262.06

Again, that's Roka, R-O-K-A.com

Time: 264.14

and enter the code Huberman at checkout to get 20% off.

Time: 267.61

Today's episode is also brought to us by Athletic Greens.

Time: 270.32

Athletic greens is an all-in-one

Time: 271.81

vitamin mineral probiotic drink.

Time: 274.08

I started taking Athletic Greens way back in 2012,

Time: 277.089

and I've taken it ever since.

Time: 278.55

So I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast.

Time: 280.95

The reason I started taking Athletic Greens

Time: 282.417

and the reason that I still take Athletic Greens

Time: 284.38

once or twice a day,

Time: 285.61

is because it covers all of my vitamin mineral

Time: 288.46

probiotic foundational needs.

Time: 290.71

There's now a wealth of evidence pointing to the fact

Time: 293.03

that vitamins minerals and probiotics

Time: 294.8

support healthy gut microbiome, healthy immune system,

Time: 297.93

healthy nervous system,

Time: 299.1

healthy endocrine system, that's the hormone system.

Time: 301.44

And with Athletic Greens,

Time: 302.63

it really covers all those bases.

Time: 304.23

In fact, when people ask me

Time: 305.63

what one supplement they should take,

Time: 307.4

I recommend Athletic Greens.

Time: 308.95

If you like to try Athletic Greens,

Time: 310.59

you can go to athleticgreens.com/huberman.

Time: 313.19

And if you do that, you can claim a special offer.

Time: 315.88

They're giving five free travel packs,

Time: 317.57

which make it very easy to mix up the Athletic Greens

Time: 319.58

while on the road, in the car, on the plane, et cetera,

Time: 321.99

plus a year supply of vitamin D3/K2.

Time: 325.34

There is now a lot of evidence

Time: 327.26

supporting that Vitamin D3

Time: 329.14

is great for our metabolism endocrine system,

Time: 332.07

mood sleep, a huge number of biological functions.

Time: 334.75

And K2 has also been shown to be important

Time: 337.1

for cardiovascular health

Time: 338.45

and various aspects of blood lipids.

Time: 340.26

So again, if you go to athleticgreens.com/huberman,

Time: 342.96

you can claim a special offer,

Time: 344.35

you'll get your Athletic Greens,

Time: 345.39

five free travel packs of Athletic Greens,

Time: 347.32

plus a year supply of vitamin D3/K2.

Time: 350.53

Today's episode is also brought to us by Headspace.

Time: 353.22

Headspace is a meditation app

Time: 354.58

supported by 25 published studies

Time: 356.54

and has over 600,000 five-star reviews.

Time: 360.08

I've been meditating for a very long time,

Time: 362.08

more than three decades,

Time: 363.8

but I confess I haven't been a consistent meditator

Time: 366.62

until recently.

Time: 367.95

I think many people will have that experience,

Time: 369.9

we've all heard about the terrific science

Time: 372.18

and benefits of a regular meditation practice.

Time: 374.4

Things like reduced anxiety, improved sleep,

Time: 377.05

sharper cognition, and focus.

Time: 379.28

However, sticking to a meditation practice can be tricky,

Time: 382.53

and many people start and then they stop.

Time: 384.4

Or at least that's what happened to me

Time: 385.51

until a few years ago,

Time: 386.43

when I started using the Headspace app to do my meditation.

Time: 390.19

Headspace has short meditations

Time: 392.05

of just a couple minutes long,

Time: 393.03

longer meditations.

Time: 394.7

Ideally for me, I like to do a 20-minute meditation

Time: 397.329

at least three times a week,

Time: 398.9

but sometimes because of my schedule

Time: 400.61

and other things going on,

Time: 401.73

I only sneak in a five-minute meditation

Time: 403.61

once or twice a week.

Time: 405.35

With Headspace, you have a huge number

Time: 407.42

of meditations to select from.

Time: 409.36

So if you want to try Headspace,

Time: 410.7

you can go to headspace.com/specialoffer.

Time: 413.51

And if you do that,

Time: 414.37

you'll get a completely free one-month trial

Time: 416.81

with all of Headspace's meditation.

Time: 419.02

That's the full library of meditations

Time: 420.66

that Headspace has to offer.

Time: 421.88

That's the best deal offer by Headspace right now.

Time: 423.609

So again, if you're interested,

Time: 425.133

go to headspace.com/special offer today.

Time: 428.56

Let's talk about the biology of social bonding.

Time: 430.9

And I want to point out

Time: 431.83

that I used the word bonding intentionally.

Time: 434.88

It's a verb.

Time: 436.27

And in biology, we want to think about verbs

Time: 438.652

because everything in biology is a process,

Time: 442.32

it's not an event.

Time: 444.05

And when we think about things in biology as a process,

Time: 446.53

that means it's going to have multiple steps.

Time: 448.4

And today we are going to explore the steps,

Time: 450.92

start to finish of social bonding.

Time: 453.43

Meaning how social bonds are established,

Time: 455.74

how they are maintained,

Time: 457.02

how they are broken and how they are reestablished.

Time: 460.41

Now an important feature of biology generally,

Time: 462.9

but in particular, as it relates to social bonding,

Time: 465.81

is that the neural circuits meaning the brain areas

Time: 469.58

and neurons and the hormones, things like oxytocin,

Time: 472.493

which we'll talk about today,

Time: 474.39

and the other chemicals in the brain and body

Time: 477.03

that are responsible for the process we call social bonding

Time: 480.28

are not unique to particular social bonds,

Time: 484.07

they are generic.

Time: 485.41

What I mean by that is that the same brain circuits

Time: 488.04

that are responsible for establishing a bond

Time: 490.301

between parent and child

Time: 492.87

are actually repurposed in romantic relationships.

Time: 495.977

And this might not come as a surprise to many of you,

Time: 498.67

many of you are probably familiar with this idea

Time: 500.65

of securely attached people versus anxious attached people

Time: 503.92

versus avoidant attached people.

Time: 505.69

We're going to touch on that a little bit,

Time: 507.44

but all of that has roots in whether or not children

Time: 510.51

and parents formed healthy social bonds,

Time: 514.15

or whether or not they had challenged social bonds.

Time: 517.4

Now it's clear from the scientific

Time: 520.23

and psychological literature

Time: 522.56

that just because you might have had

Time: 524.87

a not so great or even terrible social bond with a parent

Time: 529.14

or with some other caretaker or loved one as a child,

Time: 532.28

that doesn't fate you to have poor social bonds as an adult,

Time: 535.22

there's a lot of plasticity in the system,

Time: 537.58

meaning it can change,

Time: 538.99

it can rewire in response to experience.

Time: 541.52

And as we will soon discover,

Time: 543.565

there specific components

Time: 545.317

within the neural circuits of your brain

Time: 548.33

that are responsible for social bonding

Time: 550.36

that allow you to place subjective labels

Time: 553.55

on why you are doing certain things

Time: 555.36

and to rewire the neural circuits for social bonding.

Time: 558.05

So we're going to touch on all of that today.

Time: 559.96

But the important feature really to point out

Time: 562.169

is that we don't have 12 different circuits

Time: 564.768

in the brain and body for different types of social bonds,

Time: 567.86

we have one,

Time: 569.09

and there's some universal features

Time: 570.87

that underlie all forms of social bonds.

Time: 573.7

So we're going to start by exploring

Time: 575.32

what those neural circuits are,

Time: 577.06

and then we're going to see how they plug

Time: 579.15

into different types of social bonds.

Time: 581.32

And then we're going to explore

Time: 582.67

things like introversion, extroversion,

Time: 586.28

where we're going to touch on a little bit

Time: 587.59

about things like trauma bonds, healthy bonds,

Time: 590.41

and various other aspects

Time: 592.28

of how humans can bond to one another.

Time: 594.63

And as you'll soon discover,

Time: 596.32

there is a unique chemical signature

Time: 598.8

of all bonding of all kinds.

Time: 601.25

And you're going to learn how to modulate

Time: 602.96

that chemical signature.

Time: 604.49

Before we talk about social bonding,

Time: 606.14

I want to talk about it's mirror image,

Time: 608.22

which is lack of social bonding or social isolation.

Time: 611.71

Now for better, for worse,

Time: 613.89

there is a tremendous literature on the biology

Time: 617.7

of social isolation,

Time: 619.37

and all of the terrible things that happen

Time: 621.16

when animals or humans are socially isolated

Time: 624.2

at particular phases of life.

Time: 626.78

Now, for those of you that are introverts,

Time: 629.1

you are not necessarily damaging yourself

Time: 631.12

by deciding to spend less time with other people.

Time: 633.12

Many people like time alone,

Time: 634.23

I personally am an introvert,

Time: 636.12

I get a thrill out of spending time

Time: 638.33

with one or two close friends,

Time: 640.4

but I enjoy a lot of time by myself.

Time: 642.59

I like to socialize,

Time: 643.64

so I wouldn't call myself an extreme introvert,

Time: 645.68

but I know there's some extreme introverts out there.

Time: 647.91

But when we talk about social isolation,

Time: 649.73

what we're referring to is when animals

Time: 652.908

or humans are restricted

Time: 654.644

from having the social contacts

Time: 657.01

that they would prefer to have.

Time: 659.21

And to just briefly touch on the major takeaways

Time: 662.015

from this literature,

Time: 663.234

which spans back a hundred years or more,

Time: 667.71

being socially isolated is stressful.

Time: 670.5

And one of the hallmark features of social isolation

Time: 673.51

is chronically elevated stress hormones,

Time: 676.1

like adrenaline also called epinephrin,

Time: 678.08

like cortisol, a stress hormone that at healthy levels

Time: 681.71

is good for combating inflammation,

Time: 684.06

helps us have energy early in the day,

Time: 686.01

focused throughout the day.

Time: 687.35

But if cortisol is elevated for too long,

Time: 690.26

which is the consequence of social isolation,

Time: 693.17

the immune system suffers

Time: 694.9

and other chemicals start to be released

Time: 697.04

in the brain and body

Time: 698.38

that are designed to motivate the organism,

Time: 700.81

animal or human to seek out social bonds.

Time: 704.83

An example of one such chemical

Time: 706.6

is a peptide called tachykinin.

Time: 708.9

Tachykinin is present in flies, in mice and in humans,

Time: 713.73

and under conditions of social isolation,

Time: 716.5

it's levels go up,

Time: 717.91

and because of the brain areas

Time: 719.8

that contain receptors for tachykinin,

Time: 722.44

people start feeling very aggressive and irritable

Time: 726.16

after social isolation.

Time: 727.95

Now that should be a little bit counterintuitive to you,

Time: 730.08

you would think, oh, you know, if you isolate

Time: 731.85

an animal or a human,

Time: 733.2

and then you give them the opportunity

Time: 734.86

for social interaction, they should behave very well,

Time: 737.31

they should be thrilled,

Time: 738.143

they're finally getting the nourishment,

Time: 740.01

the social nourishment

Time: 741.03

that they've been lacking for so long.

Time: 742.83

It turns out that's not the case.

Time: 744.24

Chronic social isolation

Time: 746.07

changes the nature of the brain and body

Time: 748.67

such that it makes social connection harder,

Time: 751.79

and it makes the person who's been isolated, irritable,

Time: 755.03

even aggressive with other people.

Time: 757.6

Now, I don't want to go too deeply

Time: 758.94

into the biology of social isolation,

Time: 761.62

because it doesn't actually afford us that much insight

Time: 763.97

into what healthy social bonding looks like.

Time: 766.55

So today we're going to focus more on the functional biology,

Time: 770.44

a dual meaning of the word functional,

Time: 772.27

as opposed to the pathology of social isolation.

Time: 775.96

However, I do want to point out

Time: 778.1

that social isolation starts to deteriorate

Time: 781.76

certain aspects of brain and body pretty quickly,

Time: 785.46

but how quickly depends again

Time: 787.63

on how introverted or extroverted somebody is.

Time: 790.01

So if you're somebody who is socially isolated

Time: 792.04

for the holidays,

Time: 792.873

or has been socially isolated for a period of time,

Time: 794.83

and is craving social contact, that is a healthy craving.

Time: 798.156

And as we'll learn next,

Time: 800.39

the healthy craving for social contact

Time: 802.41

has a very specific brain circuit,

Time: 804.16

has a very specific neurochemical signature

Time: 806.16

associated with it,

Time: 807.3

and has some remarkable features that you can leverage

Time: 810.79

in social contacts of all kinds.

Time: 813

I think some of the more important and exciting work

Time: 815.81

on social bonding comes from the laboratory of Kay Tye.

Time: 820.08

Kay is a professor at the Salk Institute

Time: 821.92

for Biological Studies,

Time: 823.26

she's an investigator

Time: 824.28

with the Howard Hughes Medical Institute.

Time: 826.27

And in recent years,

Time: 827.74

I would say in about the last five or six years,

Time: 829.86

her laboratory has made a fundamental discovery

Time: 833.29

as to why we seek out and put so much effort

Time: 836.75

into social bonds.

Time: 838.47

And the key discovery that she made

Time: 841.03

is that much like hunger, much like temperature,

Time: 844.64

much like thirst, we have brain circuits

Time: 848.01

that are devoted to what's called a social homeostasis.

Time: 852.91

Many of you have probably heard about homeostasis before,

Time: 855.36

homeostasis is the characteristic

Time: 857.971

of various biological circuits

Time: 859.491

and even individual cells

Time: 861.3

to try and maintain a certain level.

Time: 863.51

It's most easily thought of in the context of hunger,

Time: 865.9

if you don't eat for a while,

Time: 866.85

your drive to pursue food and think about food

Time: 869.54

and make food and spend money on food,

Time: 873.22

and indeed to enjoy food goes up.

Time: 876.2

Whereas when you're well fed,

Time: 877.82

you don't tend to seek out food

Time: 879.77

with as much vigor or as much intensity,

Time: 882.19

you wouldn't invest as much time, effort, money, et cetera.

Time: 884.93

So homeostasis is the aspect of cells, tissues,

Time: 888.31

and organisms to seek some sort of balance

Time: 891.02

to regulate themselves.

Time: 893.28

In a crude way, you can think about the thermostat

Time: 895.3

on your home as a homeostatic circuit.

Time: 897.71

When the temperature goes up a little bit,

Time: 899.25

it cools things down to maintain a certain temperature.

Time: 901.69

When the room gets cold,

Time: 903.54

it hits a certain level and a sensor detects that,

Time: 906.786

it clicks on and then the heat goes on

Time: 908.87

to maintain a certain set temperature.

Time: 910.92

So that's a simple way of thinking about homeostasis.

Time: 913.93

Every homeostatic circuit has three components,

Time: 917.12

or at least three,

Time: 918.28

one is a detector, meaning the organism

Time: 921.97

or the thermostat on your wall

Time: 923.39

has to have some way of detecting

Time: 925.15

what's going on in the environment,

Time: 927.54

all right, in the context of social bonding,

Time: 930.16

whether or not you are interacting with others

Time: 931.837

and whether or not those interactions are going well.

Time: 933.9

So that has to be detected, that's the first thing.

Time: 936.23

Then there has to be a control center,

Time: 937.77

that's the second thing.

Time: 939.32

And the control center is the one that makes the adjustments

Time: 942.96

to, in the case of social bonding,

Time: 944.97

to your behavior and to your psychology.

Time: 947.37

So you'll soon learn that there are ways in which

Time: 950.24

the more time that you spend alone,

Time: 952.2

the more motivated you are to seek out

Time: 954.97

the pictures of faces, the interactions with actual people,

Time: 959.15

physical contact, and so forth.

Time: 961.37

Now that might seem obvious to you,

Time: 963.02

but thanks to the work of Kay Tye and others,

Time: 967.02

it's remarkable to learn

Time: 968.12

that there are specific brain centers

Time: 969.83

that are adjusting our psychology and biology

Time: 971.93

so that we seek out bonds more aggressively,

Time: 974.6

or maybe we don't because we are perfectly sated

Time: 978.34

or satiated with respect to how much contact

Time: 981.043

we've had with other people.

Time: 983.63

Now, the third component of this homeostatic circuit

Time: 986.86

is the effector.

Time: 988.193

The effector is actually what drives

Time: 990.32

the behavioral response,

Time: 992.27

it's what leads you to pick up your social media

Time: 994.311

and start scrolling,

Time: 995.48

it's what leads you to text a friend,

Time: 997.06

it's what leads you to call a friend or make plans

Time: 1000.08

and what leads you to follow through on those plans.

Time: 1002.68

So again, those three components are a detector,

Time: 1005.58

a control center, and an effector.

Time: 1008.55

And as you'll soon learn,

Time: 1010.04

the neural circuit that controls the social homeostasis

Time: 1013.63

actually has a fourth component,

Time: 1015.27

and that fourth component is one

Time: 1017.31

that places subjective understanding

Time: 1019.55

as to why you are doing what you are doing,

Time: 1021.88

and establishes your place in a hierarchy.

Time: 1026.92

Now, I know the word hierarchy can be a little bit

Time: 1029.31

of a barbed wire one because people immediately

Time: 1031.56

start thinking about boss and subordinate,

Time: 1034.98

or in couples, a leader and a follower.

Time: 1037.65

But when we talk about social hierarchies

Time: 1039.99

in the context of human interactions,

Time: 1041.81

social hierarchies are very plastic,

Time: 1044.95

meaning in one setting, one person can be the leader,

Time: 1048.08

in another setting, the other person can be the leader.

Time: 1050.46

You probably have groups of friends or family members

Time: 1052.75

where you're constantly passing the baton

Time: 1054.97

as to who's going to drive, who's going to navigate,

Time: 1057.84

who's going to pick the restaurant,

Time: 1060.45

who's going to clear the dishes

Time: 1062.2

and who's going to do certain activities and not others.

Time: 1064.68

So hierarchies are very dynamic.

Time: 1066.92

And as a consequence, social has to be very plastic

Time: 1070.682

and very fluid so that you move

Time: 1072.76

from one environment to the next,

Time: 1074.49

even with the same people,

Time: 1075.92

you have to be able to make those adjustments.

Time: 1078.17

And in the case of the social homeostasis circuit,

Time: 1081.06

those adjustments are made by a particular brain structure,

Time: 1084.06

I've talked about on this podcast before,

Time: 1085.57

it's called the prefrontal cortex,

Time: 1087.18

it is the seat of our higher consciousness, if you will.

Time: 1090.02

It's what allows us to play subjective labels on things

Time: 1093.08

so we are not strictly input output, we're not robotic.

Time: 1096.85

Meaning if you go to dinner with a friend

Time: 1099.45

and they are exceptional at choosing restaurants,

Time: 1102.06

well, in the context of the social homeostasis circuit,

Time: 1104.485

your prefrontal cortex would allow them

Time: 1106.159

to pick the restaurant

Time: 1107.44

because basically they are dominant over you

Time: 1110.05

in their capacity to pick good restaurants,

Time: 1112.69

at least in this example.

Time: 1114.06

Whereas as you leave that restaurant

Time: 1116.25

and perhaps you are navigating

Time: 1117.41

to a where to get a drink after dinner,

Time: 1119.87

or where to walk through the city,

Time: 1121.6

perhaps you have the better sense of direction.

Time: 1123.55

And so then the social bonding has to be maintained

Time: 1127.1

as you switch the hierarchy, okay?

Time: 1129.42

So that's the role of that fourth element,

Time: 1131.25

the prefrontal cortex.

Time: 1132.63

Now I just briefly want to touch on some of the brain areas

Time: 1136.25

that thanks to the work of Kay Tye and others,

Time: 1138.81

we now know underlie the detection, control and response.

Time: 1143.67

Okay, I call them the detector, control center and effector

Time: 1146.65

because inside of that description,

Time: 1148.43

isn't just a bunch of names of neural structures,

Time: 1151.2

there are also hints as to what the underlying

Time: 1153.58

neurochemicals are,

Time: 1154.94

and by understanding what the neurochemicals are,

Time: 1157.48

you can start to think about tools that you can use

Time: 1159.77

to form social bonds and maintain social bonds

Time: 1162.547

in better, healthier ways.

Time: 1164.67

So let's talk about the detector first.

Time: 1166.81

Now, keep in mind that you have your senses,

Time: 1168.79

you have your vision, you have your hearing, you have touch,

Time: 1171.32

you have smell, you have taste.

Time: 1173.4

Sensation as I've talked about many times

Time: 1175.82

before in the podcast, but I'll just remind you,

Time: 1177.89

sensation is the conversion

Time: 1180.28

of physical stimuli in the environment

Time: 1182.66

into electrical and chemical signals

Time: 1184.51

in your nervous system.

Time: 1185.62

The language of the nervous system

Time: 1186.75

is electrical and chemical signals.

Time: 1188.55

So photons of light are converted to electrical

Time: 1190.424

and chemical signals,

Time: 1191.54

pressure on the skin or light touch on the skin

Time: 1194.1

is converted into electrical and chemical signals

Time: 1196.16

and so on and so forth.

Time: 1197.49

So all of that of course is flowing into the nervous system,

Time: 1200.54

but the detector that underlies social homeostasis

Time: 1204.68

involves mainly two structures,

Time: 1206.97

one is called the ACC, the anterior cingulate cortex,

Time: 1209.41

and the other is the BLA, basal lateral amygdala.

Time: 1212.18

And when you hear the word amygdala,

Time: 1213.29

you're probably thinking fear.

Time: 1214.94

But today, as you'll see,

Time: 1216.46

the amygdala actually has many different

Time: 1218.11

sub compartments and components.

Time: 1219.97

And there's a reason why the basal lateral amygdala,

Time: 1222.65

which is associated with certain aspects

Time: 1224.621

of aversive behaviors,

Time: 1226.253

meaning moving away from certain types

Time: 1228.044

of things or interactions,

Time: 1229.9

there's a reason why the BLA is such an integral part

Time: 1232.8

of the detector system,

Time: 1234.68

and that's because just as it's important

Time: 1236.73

to form healthy social bonds,

Time: 1238.2

it's vitally important to try and avoid

Time: 1240.129

unhealthy social bonds.

Time: 1242.37

And so the basolateral amygdala is mainly associated

Time: 1245.23

with these aversive type responses

Time: 1246.844

of just moving away from certain things.

Time: 1251.43

The control center in the social homeostasis circuit

Time: 1254.26

involves a brain area called the lateral hypothalamus

Time: 1256.92

and the periventricular hypothalamus.

Time: 1260.17

The lateral hypothalamus

Time: 1261.5

and the periventricular hypothalamus

Time: 1263.62

contain neurons that are able to access the hormone system

Time: 1268.61

in order to influence the release of things like oxytocin,

Time: 1271.59

which is a hormone neuropeptide,

Time: 1273.7

it's got a part hormone, part neurotransmitter,

Time: 1276.03

it's kind of a hybrid,

Time: 1276.89

we're going to talk a lot about oxytocin today.

Time: 1279.43

So we've got the ACC and the BLA,

Time: 1281.64

these are their areas are mainly involved

Time: 1283.12

in moving away from things, although also toward them,

Time: 1285.87

that's the detector.

Time: 1286.703

Then we got the control center,

Time: 1288.22

which is in the hypothalamus,

Time: 1290.08

and then there's a very special and important area

Time: 1294.29

associated with social bonding

Time: 1295.78

that I want everyone to learn,

Time: 1297.45

which is the dorsal raphe nucleus or DRN,

Time: 1300.87

dorsal raphe nucleus.

Time: 1302.04

The dorsal raphe nucleus is a small collection

Time: 1304.35

of neurons in the midbrain, so it's deep in the brain.

Time: 1307.87

And most of the time when you hear about raphe,

Time: 1310.93

R-A-P-H-E, by the way, raphe nucleus,

Time: 1314.75

you're talking about serotonin.

Time: 1316.4

Serotonin is a neuromodulator that is often associated

Time: 1319.31

with feelings of satiety after eating, warmth,

Time: 1323.13

basically satisfaction with things that you already have.

Time: 1327.78

However, within this dorsal raphe nucleus,

Time: 1330.78

there is a small subset of neurons that release dopamine.

Time: 1334.73

Dopamine is a neuromodulator

Time: 1336.15

most often associated with movement,

Time: 1338.24

craving, motivation and desire.

Time: 1341.02

And the neural circuits that are rich with dopamine

Time: 1344.38

are things like the substantia nigra,

Time: 1346.08

the mesolimbic dopamine system, the VTA,

Time: 1348.9

the nucleus accumbens, et cetera.

Time: 1350.16

Those names don't have to mean anything to you.

Time: 1352.08

However, this unique population of dopamine neurons

Time: 1355.84

in the raphe is truly unique

Time: 1357.95

in that it's responsible for mediating

Time: 1360.62

what I've been calling social homeostasis.

Time: 1364.36

It is the effector or the response

Time: 1366.83

that mediate social homeostasis.

Time: 1368.87

Now I haven't told you exactly what social homeostasis is.

Time: 1371.95

Social homeostasis, just like hunger,

Time: 1374.83

is the process by which when you lack social interaction,

Time: 1379.46

you start to crave it.

Time: 1381.58

What's very interesting about the fact

Time: 1383.47

that there are dopamine neurons in this raphe structure

Time: 1386.73

that is the effector for social homeostasis,

Time: 1391.8

what this means is that when you are not interacting

Time: 1395.58

with people at a frequency or intensity,

Time: 1398.7

that is right for you,

Time: 1400.66

dopamine is released into the brain.

Time: 1404.06

In most popular conversations about dopamine,

Time: 1406.33

and even in scientific circles,

Time: 1408.06

when you hear dopamine release,

Time: 1409.44

you think about reward or feeling good,

Time: 1411.66

because indeed many behaviors

Time: 1413.25

and drugs of abuse increase dopamine.

Time: 1416.22

That's one of the reasons they have

Time: 1417.48

so much addictive potential.

Time: 1419.27

However, dopamine is not associated with feeling good,

Time: 1422.45

it is actually the neurochemical

Time: 1424.58

that's responsible for movement

Time: 1426.86

toward things that feel good.

Time: 1429

So to zoom out and conceptualize what we have here,

Time: 1431.89

we have a brain area that is a detector

Time: 1434.46

that either will move us toward or away

Time: 1436.38

from certain types of experiences or sensations.

Time: 1440.88

We have a control center that is going to release

Time: 1443.52

certain hormones and neuropeptides into our brain and blood,

Time: 1446.75

depending on the sorts of interactions

Time: 1448.44

that we happen to be having.

Time: 1450.24

And we have this response system,

Time: 1452.53

which is the dorsal raphe nucleus

Time: 1454.5

that contains dopamine neurons.

Time: 1455.97

And when we are not interacting with people

Time: 1459.25

at the frequency or intensity that we crave,

Time: 1462.12

dopamine is released,

Time: 1463.55

and that dopamine causes us to seek out

Time: 1466.53

social interactions of particular kinds.

Time: 1468.89

So let's talk about what social homeostasis is

Time: 1471.52

and how it plays out.

Time: 1472.78

And again, let's use hunger as an example.

Time: 1475.41

So let's say you're a person who eats

Time: 1477.89

every three or four hours regularly.

Time: 1480.18

So on Monday, Tuesday, Wednesday, Thursday,

Time: 1481.95

you're just accustomed to eating every three or four hours.

Time: 1484.91

If just suddenly I steal your meal

Time: 1488.68

out of the fridge at work, something I would not do,

Time: 1490.82

but just for sake of mental experimentation,

Time: 1494.8

that would probably cause you to go

Time: 1496.37

and seek out food through some other route.

Time: 1498.77

You might buy food,

Time: 1499.71

you probably be upset first,

Time: 1500.78

but then you go buy food or replace the food

Time: 1502.51

that you were going to eat,

Time: 1503.343

you'd be hungry for that food.

Time: 1504.36

And indeed there are hormonal type mechanisms

Time: 1508.32

and other mechanisms that when we eat regularly

Time: 1511.35

and we predict that food is coming in,

Time: 1513.08

we actually start secreting insulin,

Time: 1514.57

which is for mobilizing blood sugar,

Time: 1516.49

there are hormones in the bloodstream

Time: 1518.31

that make us hungry on a regular clock-like schedule,

Time: 1521.52

and you would seek out more food.

Time: 1524.23

Similarly, if you're somebody who is accustomed

Time: 1526.97

to a lot of social interaction,

Time: 1529.89

and suddenly I take away that social interaction,

Time: 1533.56

you would feel kind of let down,

Time: 1535.086

you would crave a replacement social interaction.

Time: 1539.06

You might be upset that you had a lunch date with a friend,

Time: 1541.44

you're used to having lunch with them every Wednesday,

Time: 1543.55

and they cancel and you would crave the interaction.

Time: 1547.09

Okay, this is called a pro-social craving.

Time: 1549.53

And indeed, this is what you see in animals and humans.

Time: 1552.57

If you, what's called acutely isolate them,

Time: 1555.781

which is just a fancy scientific word

Time: 1557.95

of saying deprive them of social interactions

Time: 1559.9

in a short-term basis,

Time: 1561.96

they start engaging in pro-social behaviors,

Time: 1564.3

they start texting other people,

Time: 1565.44

they start seeking out social interactions

Time: 1568.07

of different kinds.

Time: 1568.903

And that makes perfect sense, right?

Time: 1571.51

But thought of from the different side,

Time: 1573.98

you could also imagine

Time: 1575.29

how well, if you're getting a social interaction

Time: 1578.42

with somebody on a daily or weekly basis,

Time: 1579.9

and suddenly you remove that interaction,

Time: 1582.78

well, then people might not care,

Time: 1584.493

they might just think,

Time: 1585.46

well, I'll get the interaction tomorrow

Time: 1586.72

or the next day or the next day,

Time: 1587.66

because they're sated,

Time: 1588.9

much in the same way that the person

Time: 1590.03

who eats very regularly might say,

Time: 1591.497

"Well, I ate four hours ago,

Time: 1592.77

and I'll eat eight hours later, no big deal."

Time: 1594.32

But that's not what happens.

Time: 1595.9

There's a prediction that we are going to have

Time: 1598.44

certain types of interactions,

Time: 1599.92

and when those interactions don't happen,

Time: 1602.49

we replace that lack of interaction with a drive

Time: 1605.148

and a motivation to seek out social interaction.

Time: 1608.66

And that drive and motivation is caused by,

Time: 1612.07

or I should say,

Time: 1612.903

is driven by dopamine release from that dorsal raphe.

Time: 1616.56

And so the takeaway is that when we lack social interaction

Time: 1620.13

that we expect, we become pro-social.

Time: 1623.11

However, if we are chronically socially isolated,

Time: 1627.45

meaning we don't have interactions

Time: 1629.22

with people for a long time,

Time: 1630.91

we become actually more introverted.

Time: 1633.54

This is separate from all of the tachykinin stuff

Time: 1635.93

that I talked about earlier,

Time: 1637.03

or falling into states of chronic stress,

Time: 1639.05

but it's well-established now that in humans and in animals,

Time: 1643.25

if you don't give them enough social interaction,

Time: 1645.91

they actually become antisocial.

Time: 1648.1

And so this is actually a little bit

Time: 1649.78

like what one might see with long-term fasting.

Time: 1652.89

Okay, I gave the example of eating every four hours,

Time: 1654.85

now let's give the parallel example of somebody

Time: 1658.17

who's been fasting perhaps for two or three days,

Time: 1661.19

if they are expecting to eat,

Time: 1662.95

and then the meal doesn't arrive,

Time: 1665.5

they are not necessarily going to immediately

Time: 1668.52

try and seek out food.

Time: 1669.93

And that's a little bit counterintuitive,

Time: 1671.21

you would have thought,

Time: 1672.043

well, they haven't eaten in a very long time,

Time: 1673.21

they're going to be very motivated to seek out food,

Time: 1675.49

but no, they are accustomed to fasting.

Time: 1678

Similarly, the social homeostasis circuit

Time: 1680.98

works in a way such that when we don't have

Time: 1683.49

social interactions for a very long time,

Time: 1685.85

we start to lose our craving for social interactions.

Time: 1689.76

Let's look at the social homeostasis circuit

Time: 1691.76

through the lens of what's commonly

Time: 1693.45

called introversion and extroversion.

Time: 1696.44

Now, typically when we hear about introverts,

Time: 1698.35

we think about the quiet person at the party,

Time: 1700.33

or the person that doesn't want to go out at all.

Time: 1702.17

And we think about an extrovert as somebody

Time: 1703.92

who's really social, the so-called social butterfly,

Time: 1706.58

who enjoys social interactions, is really chatty,

Time: 1709.31

is kind of life of the party type person.

Time: 1711.04

That's the cliche or the kind of pop psychology cliche,

Time: 1713.97

but actually in the psychology literature,

Time: 1715.95

that's not really the way it holds up.

Time: 1718.93

Many people who appear introverted are actually extroverted.

Time: 1722.4

The quiet person at a party could be an extrovert,

Time: 1726.91

except that they just don't talk very much.

Time: 1729.45

The characteristic of an extrovert

Time: 1731.13

is somebody that gets energy

Time: 1732.93

or feels good from social interactions,

Time: 1735.15

they sort of get a lift,

Time: 1736.42

and we can predict that that lift occurs

Time: 1739.04

because of some release of dopamine

Time: 1741.08

within their brain and body.

Time: 1742.17

And indeed there's evidence for that,

Time: 1743.5

neuroimaging study support that,

Time: 1745.54

other forms of neuro-biological analysis

Time: 1747.312

support that as well.

Time: 1749.15

We can also imagine that the person

Time: 1750.96

who's talking a lot is somebody who's very extroverted,

Time: 1754.55

but oftentimes people who talk a lot for their work

Time: 1757.46

or they're somebody who's very social

Time: 1759.55

when you interact with them,

Time: 1761.12

that person gets back to their car

Time: 1762.64

and is absolutely depleted and exhausted by that interaction

Time: 1765.61

or all sorts of social interactions.

Time: 1768.44

So we really can't predict whether or not somebody

Time: 1771.07

is an introvert or an extrovert

Time: 1772.158

simply based on their behavior,

Time: 1774.08

it's really more of an internal subjective label.

Time: 1777.15

However, if we look at introversion and extroversion

Time: 1779.58

through this lens of the social homeostatic set point,

Time: 1782.92

and we think about dopamine as this molecule

Time: 1785.75

that drives motivation to seek out social interactions,

Time: 1790.44

what we can reasonably assume

Time: 1792.72

is that introverts are people that when they engage

Time: 1796.4

in certain forms of social interaction,

Time: 1798.89

either the amount of dopamine that's released

Time: 1801.97

is greater than it is an extrovert,

Time: 1804.665

that's right, I said greater than it is at an extrovert,

Time: 1807.147

and so they actually feel quite motivated,

Time: 1810.03

but also satisfied by very brief,

Time: 1812.95

or we could say sort of sparse social interactions.

Time: 1817.29

They don't need a lot of social engagement to feel sated.

Time: 1820.55

Again, the parallel example would be hunger.

Time: 1822.79

This would be somebody who doesn't need to eat much

Time: 1825.05

in order to feel satisfied.

Time: 1826.94

Whereas the extrovert,

Time: 1828.99

we can reasonably assume releases less dopamine

Time: 1832.198

in response to an individual social interaction.

Time: 1835.12

And so they need much more social interaction

Time: 1837.33

in order to feel filled up by that interaction.

Time: 1840.46

And indeed this is supported

Time: 1841.61

by the neuro-biological imaging studies.

Time: 1843.85

So rather than thinking about introverts and extroverts

Time: 1846.28

as chatty versus quiet,

Time: 1849.02

it's useful to think about people, maybe yourself,

Time: 1851.76

maybe other people you know,

Time: 1853.87

as how much social interaction they need

Time: 1856.37

in order to bring this social homeostasis into balance.

Time: 1859.7

Now there's the fourth component

Time: 1861.05

of this social homeostasis circuit that I mentioned before,

Time: 1863.46

and that's the prefrontal cortex.

Time: 1865.22

The prefrontal cortex is involved in thinking

Time: 1867.27

and planning and action,

Time: 1868.66

and has extensive connections with areas of the brain

Time: 1871.61

like the hypothalamus,

Time: 1873.3

which is responsible for a lot of motivated drives.

Time: 1876.68

It also has connections with the various

Time: 1878.47

reward centers of the brain,

Time: 1879.78

and it can act as kind of an accelerator,

Time: 1882.53

meaning it can encourage more electrical activity

Time: 1885.56

of other brain centers,

Time: 1887.05

or as a break on those brain centers.

Time: 1889.05

A really good example, it's kind of a trivial one

Time: 1891.93

in the context of today's discussion,

Time: 1893.39

but it's a concrete one so I'll use it,

Time: 1894.96

it would be, I know many people out there use cold showers

Time: 1898.363

as a way to stimulate metabolism

Time: 1899.906

and build up resilience and this sort of thing.

Time: 1902.23

If you get into a very cold shower

Time: 1904.14

and you feel as if you want to get out,

Time: 1906.79

but you force yourself to stay in,

Time: 1909.33

you're forcing yourself to stay in

Time: 1910.59

because your prefrontal cortex

Time: 1911.91

is placing some subjective label on that experience.

Time: 1914.7

Either you're doing it for a certain benefit

Time: 1916.7

or you've got a timer,

Time: 1918.66

and you're using the timer as the regulator

Time: 1920.5

of how long you're going to stay in,

Time: 1921.73

basically your overriding reflexes.

Time: 1923.55

And that's the main function of the prefrontal cortex.

Time: 1926.76

But as I mentioned earlier,

Time: 1927.66

the prefrontal cortex components

Time: 1929.62

that wire into the social homeostasis circuit

Time: 1932.23

are responsible for evaluating

Time: 1934.17

where you are in a given hierarchy.

Time: 1936.43

And that affords you a ton of flexibility

Time: 1938.915

in terms of the types of social interactions

Time: 1941.01

that you can engage in,

Time: 1941.95

and whether or not you're going to spend time

Time: 1944.49

with certain people or not,

Time: 1946.1

whether or not you're going to engage and then disengage.

Time: 1947.724

What do I mean by this?

Time: 1948.93

Well, let's say you're an extroverted person,

Time: 1951.24

you're somebody that likes a lot of social interaction

Time: 1953.51

and you get a lot of dopamine release on whole

Time: 1956.663

from a lot of social interactions.

Time: 1958.49

So maybe one interaction with a teller at the supermarket,

Time: 1961.37

isn't really going to give you much dopamine,

Time: 1963.26

but going to a party will give you more dopamine,

Time: 1965.71

and so you seek out these larger social interactions.

Time: 1969.03

However, you might go to a party

Time: 1970.92

where somebody says something

Time: 1972.14

or you see somebody there that you'd much prefer not to see,

Time: 1974.67

and therefore you decide to leave.

Time: 1977.52

The deciding to leave is regulated

Time: 1979.43

by that prefrontal cortex component.

Time: 1981.6

So it's important to understand

Time: 1983.38

that just because there's a homeostatic circuit

Time: 1985.34

that involves areas like the amygdala

Time: 1987.01

and the hypothalamus and these deep brain regions,

Time: 1989.25

like the dorsal raphe,

Time: 1991.8

as a human being,

Time: 1992.633

you have flexibility over your social interactions

Time: 1994.457

and that flexibility arise from those prefrontal circuits.

Time: 1997.6

So there's a ton of subjective nature to it,

Time: 2000.16

there's a lot of context to it.

Time: 2001.93

So while there are some predictable

Time: 2004.23

elements of these circuits,

Time: 2005.62

they are not simply what we would call, plug and chug,

Time: 2007.944

you have flexibility,

Time: 2009.42

you are able to say, "You know, I love parties,

Time: 2011.034

but I really don't want to go to that party

Time: 2012.99

because so-and-so is there."

Time: 2014.25

Or, "I very much don't like going across town in traffic,

Time: 2018.12

but I'm going to do it today

Time: 2019.33

because a certain collection of people,

Time: 2021.65

or perhaps a certain individual

Time: 2023.86

will be at that particular party."

Time: 2025.42

And so the prefrontal cortex again,

Time: 2027.07

is what allows you that subjective ruling

Time: 2029.64

or ruling over what would otherwise just be reflexes.

Time: 2032.89

So now I'd like to drill a little bit deeper

Time: 2035.1

into this incredible neural structure

Time: 2037.72

that is the dorsal raphe nucleus

Time: 2039.82

and this small collection of neurons,

Time: 2042.18

the dopamine neurons of the dorsal raphe

Time: 2044.02

because while it's a small collection,

Time: 2045.34

they are very powerful.

Time: 2048.4

Loneliness has been defined

Time: 2050.22

by the great psychologist, John Cacioppo,

Time: 2053.22

as the distress that results from discrepancies

Time: 2055.562

between ideal and perceive social relationships.

Time: 2058.62

Let me repeat that.

Time: 2059.453

Loneliness is not just being isolated,

Time: 2061.91

loneliness, as he defines it,

Time: 2064.76

is the distress that results from discrepancies

Time: 2067.028

between ideal and perceive social relationships.

Time: 2070.07

It's when we expect things to be one way,

Time: 2072.04

and they're actually another way

Time: 2073.6

and which way we expect them to be

Time: 2076.02

and which way they turn out, again, is highly subjective.

Time: 2079.08

What you expect from friendships,

Time: 2080.94

and what other people expect from friendships

Time: 2082.46

could be entirely different,

Time: 2083.83

but the circuit that underlies friendship bonding

Time: 2086.27

is exactly the same.

Time: 2087.69

And it is this dorsal raphe nucleus

Time: 2090.04

and the dopamine neurons in that nucleus

Time: 2091.77

that underlie the bond that is social friendship

Time: 2094.13

and all types of social bonds.

Time: 2096.06

There's a key finding in the literature,

Time: 2098.38

the title of this paper is Dorsal Raphe

Time: 2100.33

Dopamine Neurons Represent the Experience

Time: 2102.4

of Social Isolation.

Time: 2103.473

This is a paper from Kat Tye's lab,

Time: 2105.7

the first author is Matthews,

Time: 2107.26

Gillian Matthews to be specific.

Time: 2109.57

What they did is they were able to selectively activate

Time: 2112.44

the dopamine neurons in the dorsal raphe nucleus.

Time: 2115.57

And when they did that,

Time: 2116.94

they induced a loneliness-like state.

Time: 2119.41

Now, how did they know it was a loneliness-like state?

Time: 2121.88

They knew because it motivated

Time: 2123.54

the seeking out of social connections.

Time: 2126.5

This is the kind of social hunger

Time: 2128.72

that I was referring to before.

Time: 2130.61

Whereas when the dopamine neurons of the dorsal raphe

Time: 2133.57

are inhibited, meaning their activity is quieted

Time: 2136.78

that suppressed a loneliness state.

Time: 2140.89

So that's a little counterintuitive, right?

Time: 2142.97

It's a group of neurons that when activated

Time: 2145.267

makes you feel lonely,

Time: 2147.31

and when this brain area is not activated,

Time: 2151.1

it suppresses loneliness.

Time: 2152.66

But if you think about it,

Time: 2153.493

that's exactly the kind of circuit that you would want

Time: 2156.22

in order to drive social behavior.

Time: 2158.78

When you're feeling lonely,

Time: 2160.69

dopamine is released and it causes you to go out

Time: 2162.78

and seek social interactions.

Time: 2164.66

When this brain area has enough social interactions,

Time: 2168.107

that's sort of a figure of speech,

Time: 2169.83

brain areas don't have enough social interactions,

Time: 2172

but when enough social interactions have happened,

Time: 2174.5

that the neurons in this brain areas

Time: 2175.76

shut down their production of dopamine.

Time: 2177.74

Well, the loneliness state turns off.

Time: 2181.13

So what we think of as loneliness

Time: 2183.15

as this big kind of dark cloud,

Time: 2185.297

or, you know, fog in our psychological landscape,

Time: 2189.27

boils down to a very small set of neurons,

Time: 2191.48

releasing a specific neurochemical for motivation.

Time: 2194.58

And to me, this really changes the way

Time: 2196.4

that we think about loneliness

Time: 2197.85

and that we think about social interactions.

Time: 2200.8

There's so much subjective landscape to loneliness

Time: 2203.424

and to social interactions,

Time: 2205.92

but at the end of the day,

Time: 2206.88

what it really is is that we are all social animals

Time: 2209.68

to some extent or another,

Time: 2211.23

and we all crave social interactions

Time: 2213.132

to some extent or another,

Time: 2214.9

although the extent will vary

Time: 2216.762

depending on where you are

Time: 2218.33

in the introversion extroversion continuum.

Time: 2220.86

And it is indeed a continuum.

Time: 2222.58

Now, the other aspect of the study that was really important

Time: 2225.42

gets back to that issue of hierarchy and social rank.

Time: 2229.06

What they found is that depending

Time: 2231.21

on where you see yourself in the social rank,

Time: 2235.3

the dopamine neurons in the raphe

Time: 2237.28

will lead to one consequence or another,

Time: 2239.587

meaning moving toward social interactions

Time: 2241.834

or moving away from them.

Time: 2244.17

So the whole system is set up

Time: 2246.1

so that you have a ton of flexibility

Time: 2247.563

and control over social interactions.

Time: 2249.89

So just a couple of key points and actionable takeaways

Time: 2252.51

based on the information I've offered up until now,

Time: 2256.31

if you think of yourself as an introvert,

Time: 2259.66

it's very likely that you get a lot of dopamine

Time: 2262.5

from a few or minimal social interactions.

Time: 2266.21

Whereas if you're an extrovert,

Time: 2267.96

contrary to what you might think,

Time: 2270.02

social interactions are not going to flood

Time: 2271.82

your system with dopamine,

Time: 2273.23

they actually are going to lead to less dopamine release

Time: 2277.19

than it would for an introvert,

Time: 2278.82

and therefore you're going to need

Time: 2280.02

a lot more social interactions

Time: 2282.07

in order to feel filled up by those interactions.

Time: 2285

Now I've been drawing a lot of parallels

Time: 2286.53

between this social seeking

Time: 2288.87

or avoiding social isolation and hunger,

Time: 2292.43

but is that really the case?

Time: 2294.24

And could it be that they're actually interactions

Time: 2296.953

between the different drives?

Time: 2298.78

Meaning could social isolation

Time: 2301.53

or the desire to seek out social interactions

Time: 2303.887

actually relate to the hunger system and vice versa?

Time: 2307.3

And indeed the answer is yes.

Time: 2309.61

We don't have 50 different homeostatic systems

Time: 2313.72

and 50 different neurochemicals to underlie our drive,

Time: 2319.01

to eat our drive for romantic interactions,

Time: 2322.92

our drive for friendship interactions,

Time: 2326.12

we have essentially one, maybe two,

Time: 2329.13

and they all funnel into the same dopamine system.

Time: 2332.14

And there's a beautiful paper that illustrates

Time: 2333.929

some of the crossover

Time: 2335.24

between these different homeostatic drives.

Time: 2338.56

The title of the paper is Acute Social Isolation

Time: 2341.14

Evokes Midbrain Craving Responses Similar to Hunger.

Time: 2344.81

This from Rebecca Saxe's lab at MIT,

Time: 2347.5

Massachusetts Institute of technology,

Time: 2350.02

Dr. Kay Tye is also an author on this paper,

Time: 2352.4

the paper was published in Nature Neuroscience.

Time: 2354.01

It's a really terrific paper.

Time: 2356.39

Just to briefly summarize what they did,

Time: 2358.7

they took people that were categorized

Time: 2360.93

as socially connected healthy human adults.

Time: 2363.16

So these are people that are used

Time: 2364.24

to pretty frequent social interactions,

Time: 2367.4

and they socially isolated them for about 10 hours.

Time: 2371.4

And they had no opportunity to access social media,

Time: 2375.07

email, fiction reading even,

Time: 2378.21

and certainly didn't have the opportunity

Time: 2379.78

to interact with people face to face.

Time: 2381.86

So what this did is it increased social craving,

Time: 2384.317

both subjectively the people said

Time: 2386.57

that they were now craving social interactions,

Time: 2388.63

and then they did brain imaging

Time: 2390.06

in response to images of people,

Time: 2392.13

people interacting, food, flowers,

Time: 2395.08

other types of stimuli,

Time: 2396.11

some of the stimuli or these images

Time: 2397.886

that we call them stimuli

Time: 2399.25

but their images really, had a lot of social engagement

Time: 2402.38

going on in them, others did not,

Time: 2403.73

some had a lot of faces showing, others did not.

Time: 2406.47

And as you might suspect,

Time: 2408.18

there was activation of many of the brain areas

Time: 2411.56

that we've talked about earlier,

Time: 2412.75

dorsal raphe nucleus and other brain areas

Time: 2414.72

associated with dopaminergic neurons.

Time: 2417.44

When the socially isolated people viewed social cues,

Time: 2422.74

people interacting, faces,

Time: 2424.31

and so on and less so for things like flowers,

Time: 2427.13

however, they also had increased responses

Time: 2430.578

to images of food, which is interesting

Time: 2434.42

and actually is consistent with the literature

Time: 2436.87

that when people are socially isolated,

Time: 2438.72

they often will start eating more,

Time: 2441.19

or they will change the nature of the foods that they eat.

Time: 2443.95

Now we think of that as comfort foods

Time: 2446.34

or soothing oneself through eating

Time: 2448.94

rather than social interaction as a kind of pathology,

Time: 2451.93

but while it might not be healthy,

Time: 2453.87

depending on the context and the person,

Time: 2456.1

it's really important to understand

Time: 2457.48

that the reason that happens

Time: 2459.68

is because we have a common circuit

Time: 2461.81

and that the system meaning the person

Time: 2463.91

is actually craving dopamine release.

Time: 2465.9

They don't consciously know this,

Time: 2466.743

this is all subconsciously carried out,

Time: 2468.78

but they're craving dopamine release.

Time: 2470.46

And if they can't get it from social interactions,

Time: 2472.51

as they normally would, they'll start seeking it from food.

Time: 2475.54

Now they did an important reverse experiment as well,

Time: 2478.94

where they had subjects go on 10 hours of food fasting.

Time: 2482.67

Now these were not people that were familiar with fasting,

Time: 2485.56

they weren't doing intermittent fasting,

Time: 2488.27

they were eating more typical meal schedules,

Time: 2490.84

and so that created increased hunger, et cetera,

Time: 2493.75

but it also increased their appetite if you will,

Time: 2497.1

for social interactions.

Time: 2498.76

And so the important point here

Time: 2500.38

is that there's a common biology,

Time: 2502.38

there's a common circuitry that underlies homeostatic

Time: 2505.49

craving of things that maintain us as individuals

Time: 2509.08

and as a species.

Time: 2510.1

And it really places social interactions as right up there

Time: 2514.84

in the list of things that we could consider so vital

Time: 2517.53

for our survival and for our health.

Time: 2519.22

Things like food, water, social interactions

Time: 2522.73

really sit within a top tier amongst each other,

Time: 2527

and they use the same common circuitry,

Time: 2529.21

dorsal raphe dopamine neurons,

Time: 2531.15

in addition to other structures,

Time: 2532.43

in order to create this drive,

Time: 2534.84

to seek out certain types of stimuli.

Time: 2537.21

Now, this is a very reductionist view of social bonding,

Time: 2539.97

I realized that,

Time: 2541.4

but it's important to realize

Time: 2543.1

that while we place all the subjective context,

Time: 2545.59

oh, I miss this person,

Time: 2548.389

or I really would like to avoid that person,

Time: 2550.43

at the end of the day,

Time: 2551.263

it really all funnels into a system

Time: 2553.534

whereby a single neurochemical

Time: 2555.71

is either being released and motivating us

Time: 2558.61

to seek out more of a particular type of interaction

Time: 2560.931

or is not released,

Time: 2562.15

and therefore we are perfectly comfortable

Time: 2563.61

staying exactly where we are.

Time: 2565.69

As I say this, some of you are probably thinking,

Time: 2567.847

"Oh, that's probably what happens when you fall in love."

Time: 2570.27

And indeed that's the case.

Time: 2571.72

When people enter romantic relationships,

Time: 2574.33

that to them are very satisfying,

Time: 2576.36

there's this period that, you know,

Time: 2578.34

that the theory is that it lasts anywhere

Time: 2580.36

from six days to six months,

Time: 2582.19

although some people report that this feeling

Time: 2583.89

can last many, many years,

Time: 2585.18

even decades of just feeling completely filled up and sated

Time: 2590.19

by the experience of being with that person.

Time: 2592.78

So much so that cravings for food are reduced,

Time: 2595.93

cravings for sleep are reduced.

Time: 2597.56

Now there's all sorts of activities

Time: 2598.78

and things that go along with new romantic partnerships

Time: 2601.18

that take up time, that might get in the way of things

Time: 2603.73

like sleep or things like food.

Time: 2605.64

But the point is that dopamine is the final common pathway

Time: 2609.86

by which we seek out things

Time: 2611.74

and we end up feeling as if we are satisfied

Time: 2613.886

by certain types of interactions.

Time: 2616.54

Now, similarly, if you've ever been isolated

Time: 2618.66

for a long period of time,

Time: 2620.43

your focus might have shifted to what you're going to eat,

Time: 2623.66

what you're going to cook for dinner

Time: 2624.77

in a much more heightened way,

Time: 2626.55

the importance of those sensory stimuli

Time: 2628.89

and those types of interactions,

Time: 2630.15

and indeed the taste of food itself expands.

Time: 2632.86

So normally when we are in social relationships

Time: 2635.72

that are ones that are familiar to us,

Time: 2638.15

we have a balance of these different drives.

Time: 2640.29

But when one particular drive takes over

Time: 2643.31

and we are very focused on it,

Time: 2645.86

because they all funnel into the same circuitry,

Time: 2648.51

there really isn't the seeking out

Time: 2650.48

of certain types of behaviors

Time: 2651.6

like food seeking when we're newly in love.

Time: 2653.96

Now that doesn't mean that food won't taste good to us

Time: 2656.19

so that we don't seek it.

Time: 2657.023

And indeed, there are experiments that have been done

Time: 2658.79

where if people have just fallen in love,

Time: 2660.32

the taste of a strawberry can just be incredible.

Time: 2663.01

The other effect of dopamine is that it changes

Time: 2665.04

the way that we interpret sensory stimuli,

Time: 2666.87

our detectors actually change

Time: 2669.02

when we are in heightened states

Time: 2670.45

of dopaminergic activity or drive.

Time: 2673.39

Basically what this means is that things seem better

Time: 2675.73

than they would when we have less dopamine in our system.

Time: 2678.93

The point here is that there's a lot of crossover,

Time: 2681.51

there's a lot of meshing together

Time: 2683.5

of different homeostatic drives

Time: 2685.23

that they don't exist in separate channels.

Time: 2687.58

And it's only under conditions

Time: 2689.44

in which one particular homeostatic drive

Time: 2691.99

is kind of being played out to the extreme,

Time: 2693.85

such as the example of falling in love,

Time: 2696.21

that we tend to avoid or sort of overlook

Time: 2699.92

the other homeostatic drives,

Time: 2701.33

and that's because simply we're getting enough dopamine,

Time: 2704.07

we don't need anymore.

Time: 2705.63

Up until now, I've been focused

Time: 2706.86

on the organizational logic of social bonding,

Time: 2709.334

which is really just nerd speak

Time: 2710.9

for how is it that we form bonds, avoid bonds?

Time: 2713.54

Why do people seek out more or fewer bonds

Time: 2715.69

than others, et cetera?

Time: 2717.28

Now I'd like to shift gears a bit,

Time: 2719.64

and focus on what are some things that we can do

Time: 2723.1

to encourage the formation of healthy bonds?

Time: 2727.68

There's a beautiful study that was published

Time: 2729.29

in Cell Reports, Cell Press Journal, excellent journal.

Time: 2732.45

The title of this paper is Conscious Processing

Time: 2734.175

of Narrative Stimuli Synchronizes Heart Rate

Time: 2736.48

Between Individuals.

Time: 2738.07

I mentioned this on a previous podcast,

Time: 2739.77

but I'd like to mention it again

Time: 2741.21

and go into a little bit more depth

Time: 2742.38

because it points to specific

Time: 2744.02

actionable items that we can all use

Time: 2746.27

in order to enhance the quality and depth

Time: 2748.88

of social bonds of all kinds.

Time: 2751.29

Now this study involved a very simple type of experiment,

Time: 2754.77

they had people listen to a story,

Time: 2756.48

everybody in the study listened to the same story,

Time: 2759.15

but they listened to that story at different times

Time: 2760.93

and indeed in different locations.

Time: 2762.85

So different people, same story.

Time: 2765.91

And they measured things like heart rate,

Time: 2767.87

they measured breathing, et cetera.

Time: 2769.64

Now, what was the motivation for doing this?

Time: 2771.82

Well, there's a long standing literature

Time: 2774.01

showing that our physiology,

Time: 2776.24

things like our heart rate, our breathing,

Time: 2778.09

our skin conductance,

Time: 2779.04

meaning the amount of sweating,

Time: 2782.22

can be synchronized between individuals,

Time: 2784.86

and that synchronization can occur

Time: 2787.91

according to a variety of different things.

Time: 2788.98

There've been studies that have people look at one another

Time: 2791.54

and they look and actually see that their pupil size

Time: 2794.57

of their eyes starts to synchronize.

Time: 2796.83

People's breathing can synchronize,

Time: 2798.98

people's body temperatures can even start to synchronize,

Time: 2802.53

or at least shifts in body temperature can synchronize.

Time: 2804.68

One person gets cooler, the other person gets cooler.

Time: 2806.85

A lot of this is subconscious,

Time: 2808.57

some of it can be detected by conscious cues

Time: 2810.63

like flushing of the skin,

Time: 2811.7

or actually seeing someone's pupils change.

Time: 2813.76

But actually the pupil reflex is a really good example,

Time: 2816.73

whereby except for rare cases

Time: 2819.458

and certain highly trained individuals,

Time: 2821.61

most people can't control their pupil reflexes

Time: 2823.98

in a very deliberate way,

Time: 2825.34

it's truly a reflex, it's an autonomic reflex.

Time: 2828.95

So there's a lot of literature showing

Time: 2830.47

that within small groups or two people,

Time: 2834.09

these physiological signals can be synchronized.

Time: 2836

What this study found was that when people listen

Time: 2839.22

to the same story, but at different times,

Time: 2841.62

their heart rates start to synchronize.

Time: 2843.8

This is incredible because people are listening

Time: 2845.4

to the story at different times,

Time: 2847.05

but the gaps between their heartbeats become very stereotype

Time: 2849.84

and map almost precisely onto one another.

Time: 2852.7

That's incredible.

Time: 2854.61

Now we also know from an extensive literature

Time: 2857.88

that the quality and perceived depth of a social bond

Time: 2863.22

correlates very strongly

Time: 2865.19

with how much physiological synchronization

Time: 2868.44

there is between individuals.

Time: 2870.6

In other words, when your bodies feel the same,

Time: 2874.53

you tend to feel more bonded to somebody else.

Time: 2877.64

And so this whole thing is a rather circular argument,

Time: 2880.38

when you feel closer to somebody else,

Time: 2882.02

your physiology synchronize,

Time: 2884.09

and the reverse is true as well,

Time: 2886.19

when your physiologies are synchronized,

Time: 2887.81

you feel closer to other people.

Time: 2889.83

This is what I call the concert phenomenon,

Time: 2891.58

if you ever go to see your favorite band,

Time: 2894.28

or you go to a concert that you particularly love,

Time: 2897.12

you often look over at somebody

Time: 2899.18

and you'll see them enjoying the same thing,

Time: 2901.07

and they're often in a similar state as you are,

Time: 2903.47

maybe their sort of like favorite song comes on,

Time: 2905.86

and you actually feel connected to that person.

Time: 2909

You feel like you're in...

Time: 2909.833

Obviously there's a shared experience,

Time: 2911.85

but there's also a shared physiological response

Time: 2914.56

to that experience.

Time: 2915.75

And so this can happen and mass with large groups of people,

Time: 2919.09

or it can happen just between two individuals.

Time: 2921.52

And as the study points out,

Time: 2923.23

it can actually happen between individuals,

Time: 2924.85

without them actually interacting with one another.

Time: 2926.98

When the story they are listening to

Time: 2928.67

is the anchor or the driver of their physiology.

Time: 2931.79

This really points to the fact that the body

Time: 2934.65

and the brain are reciprocally connected.

Time: 2937.14

Yes, indeed, what we think, what we hear,

Time: 2939.14

what we feel drives our physiology,

Time: 2942.26

our heartbeat, our respiration, et cetera,

Time: 2944.55

but our heartbeat and respiration also

Time: 2946.85

are influencing our state of mind.

Time: 2949.29

And in this case,

Time: 2950.123

it's encouraging certain types of social bonds

Time: 2952.94

when our heart rates are synchronized.

Time: 2955.277

And you can leverage this,

Time: 2956.97

how can you leverage this?

Time: 2957.99

Well, let's take a upcoming example of the holidays.

Time: 2961.83

There's a sort of a joke,

Time: 2964.33

I think it was Ram Dass, sort of Buddhist philosopher type

Time: 2969.07

that said, "If you think you're enlightened,

Time: 2971.458

go visit your parents."

Time: 2972.98

And I think what he was referring to

Time: 2974.44

is that some people, not all people,

Time: 2976.35

have challenging relationships with their parents.

Time: 2978.36

We're going to talk about child-parent attachment

Time: 2980.64

and interactions in a few minutes,

Time: 2982.12

but you know, some people have a wonderful relationship

Time: 2985.37

to both their parents and more power to them,

Time: 2987.86

I think that's wonderful, we should all be so lucky.

Time: 2990.94

Many people have challenged relationships

Time: 2992.289

with their parents,

Time: 2993.21

or they have a great relationship with their parents,

Time: 2995.74

but their parents know,

Time: 2997.55

or they know how to drive that dart

Time: 2999.61

right into that particular soft piece of psychological flesh

Time: 3003.08

by saying just the slightest thing,

Time: 3005.1

or even by raising their eyebrow or rolling their eyes

Time: 3007.67

or the tone in which they do something.

Time: 3009.13

This is also true between siblings.

Time: 3011.45

I think many of you can think of examples

Time: 3013.62

where this is true.

Time: 3015.8

Many people when they interact with others,

Time: 3018.57

expect that the mere interaction with the other person

Time: 3022.27

is going to create the sense of bonding.

Time: 3024.75

And often that is the case,

Time: 3026.1

for instance, if are involved in intimate disclosure,

Time: 3029.6

if people enjoy each other's company so much

Time: 3033.72

that just the mere sight of somebody evokes great feelings

Time: 3036.59

and it's mutual, that often can happen.

Time: 3038.89

But in many types of social interactions,

Time: 3041.74

it's not the direct interaction with that person

Time: 3044.552

that makes us feel close to them,

Time: 3046.61

but rather it's shared experience.

Time: 3049.22

And shared experience is shared physiology.

Time: 3052.62

That's the point I'm trying to make by way of this study

Time: 3055.71

about conscious processing of narrative stimuli

Time: 3058.03

synchronize this heart rate of different individuals.

Time: 3060.41

So for instance, if you have a somewhat challenged

Time: 3064.05

or somewhat, let's call it a slight friction

Time: 3067.63

in getting close with somebody,

Time: 3070.58

or it can be a challenging interaction,

Time: 3072.52

oftentimes, it's very useful to focus outward

Time: 3075.8

on some other common narrative, a movie,

Time: 3078.61

oftentimes people will watch a game together.

Time: 3080.67

Actually there's a lot of critique

Time: 3082.5

that people or families will focus outward

Time: 3085.75

too much on external events,

Time: 3087.5

but these external events can be observing the grandchild

Time: 3090.504

and how wonderful they are,

Time: 3092.05

or observing the meal and how wonderful it is.

Time: 3094.51

Or as we commonly see in various traditions,

Time: 3097.75

there's a story that's repeated each year,

Time: 3100.05

certainly in the upcoming holidays,

Time: 3101.45

there's Christmas stories,

Time: 3102.86

there are themes and traditions,

Time: 3105.21

and those themes and traditions

Time: 3106.76

anchor a number of different aspects of our psychology.

Time: 3109.84

They're really wonderful,

Time: 3110.76

they thread through the ages really,

Time: 3113.32

and allow us to link our own experiences up

Time: 3115.96

with previous generations and experiences.

Time: 3119.11

But in addition to that,

Time: 3120.55

they synchronize our physiologies.

Time: 3122.39

And so sometimes it can be useful

Time: 3124.01

rather than expecting others to shift our physiology

Time: 3127

in the way that we wish,

Time: 3128.43

or us shifting their physiologies in the way that we wish,

Time: 3131.67

and then expecting some bond to mushroom out of that

Time: 3134.73

in some beautiful way,

Time: 3136.76

to focus on some external stimulus,

Time: 3139.94

to focus on something that will synchronize

Time: 3141.81

the physiologies of both people,

Time: 3143.11

that can act as a bridge in order to establish social bonds.

Time: 3146.48

And this is not a hack or a workaround

Time: 3148.86

for making terrible relationships good,

Time: 3150.75

this is actually at the seat of what we come away

Time: 3153.77

from a social interaction with

Time: 3155.44

as feeling "Wow, that was a really wonderful time."

Time: 3158.24

Often a really wonderful time can be by virtue

Time: 3160.89

of the specific things that were said,

Time: 3162.48

or the specific things that one engaged in,

Time: 3165.5

but more often than not the final common pathway,

Time: 3168.47

we should say,

Time: 3169.35

of great experiences was a great physiological experience

Time: 3174.01

and a shared physiological experience.

Time: 3177.05

I have a short anecdote that relates to this,

Time: 3178.62

I have an older sibling and she used to say

Time: 3180.58

that when she was in college,

Time: 3181.57

the best dates that she ever went on

Time: 3183.37

were dates where she was asked to go out

Time: 3185.65

and listen to music.

Time: 3186.76

She pointed out, however,

Time: 3188.08

that oftentimes the guys that would ask her out

Time: 3189.908

would take her to jazz clubs,

Time: 3191.4

she always had the theory

Time: 3192.233

that they would ask her to jazz clubs

Time: 3193.66

because at jazz clubs typically you would sit down

Time: 3195.9

and then she had to conclude that they couldn't dance.

Time: 3198.121

My sister likes to dance.

Time: 3199.46

And so anytime someone actually had the nerve

Time: 3201.135

to take her dancing,

Time: 3202.27

those turned out to be particularly,

Time: 3204.78

let's just say, satisfying dates and relationships,

Time: 3206.726

at least they lasted longer,

Time: 3207.915

that's all I know about them,

Time: 3209.14

that's all I want to know about them,

Time: 3210.45

she's my sister after all.

Time: 3212.1

But the theory behind whoever was asking her out

Time: 3215.9

on these dates was it was the right one,

Time: 3217.31

which is that if you want to bond with somebody,

Time: 3219.31

you create a common physiological response

Time: 3221.85

through a common and shared experience.

Time: 3224.51

And that is often a good entryway

Time: 3226.96

into establishing whether or not it's always a question,

Time: 3230.08

whether or not there can be common physiological experience

Time: 3232.34

between two individuals.

Time: 3234.16

Up until now, we've been talking about social bonding

Time: 3236.51

through the lens of neurocircuits

Time: 3238.1

that are already established.

Time: 3239.98

However, early in the episode,

Time: 3241.48

I mentioned that these very neural circuits

Time: 3243.103

that are responsible for social bonding

Time: 3245.14

in adult forms of attachment,

Time: 3246.866

be it romantic or friendship or otherwise

Time: 3250.35

are actually established during development.

Time: 3253.29

One of the more important,

Time: 3254.36

and I think exciting areas of early attachment

Time: 3258.12

as it relates to adult attachment,

Time: 3260.48

comes to us from the work of Allan Schore.

Time: 3263.63

Alan shore, spelled A-L-L-A-N, Schore, S-C-H-O-R-E

Time: 3268.98

is a psychoanalyst who also has deep understanding

Time: 3271.76

of neurobiology of attachment,

Time: 3274.78

both in childhood and in adulthood.

Time: 3278.09

And he's focused a lot on differences

Time: 3280.653

between right brain and left brain forms of attachment.

Time: 3284.85

Now in an early episode of the Huberman Lab Podcast,

Time: 3287.8

I touched into the fact that most of what's discussed

Time: 3290.78

in the general public in sort of pop psychology

Time: 3293.5

and even in some neurobiology courses

Time: 3296.42

about right brain versus left brain,

Time: 3298.637

and one side of the brain being more emotional,

Time: 3300.81

and the other side being more rational is completely wrong.

Time: 3304.34

Okay, most of what I see out there is actually backwards

Time: 3307.891

to the way things actually work.

Time: 3310.14

And while there is some,

Time: 3311.47

what we call lateralization of function,

Time: 3313.93

meaning certain brain functions are handled by neurons

Time: 3317.52

on one side of the brain or the other,

Time: 3320.47

the idea that one side of your brain is emotional,

Time: 3322.38

and the other side of your brain is rational

Time: 3324.26

is just simply not true.

Time: 3326.46

However, the work of Allan Schore

Time: 3328.67

points to some very concrete neural circuits

Time: 3331.41

that do have a lateralization bias,

Time: 3334.73

meaning they are more right brain than left brain,

Time: 3336.82

or more left brain than right brain,

Time: 3338.93

that underlies certain forms of attachment

Time: 3341.44

between child and parents,

Time: 3343.13

in particular child and mother,

Time: 3345.97

and that these right brain-isms, if you will,

Time: 3349.68

and left brain-isms for attachment,

Time: 3352.38

get played out again and again

Time: 3355.33

in our forms of attachment as adults.

Time: 3358.51

So I'd like to talk about that work briefly now,

Time: 3360.82

because I think it really points to a number

Time: 3363.27

of important features of how we establish bonds

Time: 3365.57

and the different routes to establishing bonds.

Time: 3368.57

So within the field of psychoanalysis,

Time: 3370.33

there's been a longstanding discussion, of course,

Time: 3372.34

about the so-called unconscious or subconscious,

Time: 3374.85

the things that we are not aware of.

Time: 3376.76

And I think there's growing evidence

Time: 3378.28

pointing to the fact that at least one major component

Time: 3381.087

of the subconscious or the unconscious

Time: 3383.73

is the so-called autonomic nervous system.

Time: 3385.92

The autonomic nervous system

Time: 3387.33

is the portion of our nervous system

Time: 3388.95

that controls our reflexive breathing,

Time: 3390.65

our heart rate, our skin conductance,

Time: 3392.99

meaning our sweating, pupil size,

Time: 3395.7

it's the aspect of our nervous system

Time: 3398.07

that makes us more alert or more calm.

Time: 3400.23

It's the so-called sympathetic, meaning for alertness,

Time: 3402.86

or parasympathetic branch of the autonomic nervous system,

Time: 3405.38

parasympathetic for more calming responses.

Time: 3408.16

Now what Dr. Schore's work

Time: 3411.11

and the work of others is now showing,

Time: 3413.97

is that early infant-parent,

Time: 3417.15

in particular infant-mother attachment

Time: 3418.86

involves a coordination or synchronization

Time: 3422.75

of these right brain circuits

Time: 3424.94

and these left brain circuits,

Time: 3426.6

as they relate, excuse me,

Time: 3428.47

to the autonomic nervous system.

Time: 3430.77

How does this play out?

Time: 3431.83

Well, it plays out where early on as an infant,

Time: 3434.75

when you're born, you're truly helpless,

Time: 3436.65

you can't feed yourself,

Time: 3437.76

you can't warm yourself, you can't change yourself,

Time: 3439.94

and you certainly can't emulate walk anywhere

Time: 3442.84

to get the things that you need.

Time: 3445.31

All of those functions,

Time: 3447.55

all of those needs rather are met by your primary caretaker.

Time: 3451.43

Typically that's the mother.

Time: 3453.52

Fathers of course play a role also,

Time: 3455.27

but because of breastfeeding or even bottle feeding,

Time: 3458.06

typically mothers play a more prominent role.

Time: 3460.34

I realized there are exceptions,

Time: 3461.43

but that's the general rule.

Time: 3464.74

There are now brain imaging studies

Time: 3466.89

examining the brains of infants

Time: 3469.03

and the brains of mothers as they interact

Time: 3472.16

and showing that the physical contact between the two,

Time: 3475.94

the breathing of the mother and child,

Time: 3478.77

the heart rate of the mother and child,

Time: 3480.8

and indeed the pupil size of the mother and child

Time: 3483.01

are actually actively getting coordinated.

Time: 3485.16

In other words, the mother is regulating

Time: 3487.57

the infant's autonomic nervous system primarily,

Time: 3490.26

and the infant is also regulating

Time: 3491.82

the mother's autonomic nervous system,

Time: 3493.58

a small coup from a baby or a cry,

Time: 3496.23

which is a stress cry from a baby

Time: 3497.66

will definitely regulate the autonomic nervous system

Time: 3499.69

of the mother.

Time: 3500.93

This whole right brain system is directly tapped

Time: 3506.161

into the so-called oxytocin system,

Time: 3508.46

and we'll talk more about oxytocin in a moment.

Time: 3510.47

Oxytocin again, being this peptide hormone

Time: 3512.294

that is involved in social bonds of all kinds,

Time: 3514.832

but that at least in early childhood

Time: 3517.67

is very closely associated with milk let down

Time: 3520.1

and milk production.

Time: 3521.58

There's actually a lot of stimulation

Time: 3523.6

of oxytocin release in the mother by nursing itself,

Time: 3527.06

so physical contact with the nipple,

Time: 3529.562

and by the contact of skin between baby and mother

Time: 3533.76

and their specificity there,

Time: 3535.08

it's not just any baby that can evoke

Time: 3537.93

the most amount of oxytocin release from the mother.

Time: 3541.03

Now, however, there are examples

Time: 3542.63

where just holding a child will evoke oxytocin release

Time: 3545.77

in the non-parent or somebody other than the parent,

Time: 3548.56

I think most people experience that.

Time: 3550.18

That's the new puppy or new baby phenomenon

Time: 3552.62

'cause indeed puppies can invoke oxytocin release as well.

Time: 3556.37

The point is not that oxytocin is only released

Time: 3558.75

in response to the primary relationship,

Time: 3561.186

or the mother and their child,

Time: 3563.44

but rather that the amount of oxytocin

Time: 3566.08

scales with how closely related one is

Time: 3568.313

to that particular child and vice versa.

Time: 3571.29

So there's oxytocin release occurring

Time: 3573.19

in both the child and the mother.

Time: 3575.09

So this right brain system is an emotional

Time: 3578.66

but autonomic system,

Time: 3580.46

it is below our conscious detection.

Time: 3584.42

Now, as we get older,

Time: 3588.16

there's another system that starts to come into play

Time: 3590.79

in parent-child interactions,

Time: 3593.76

and this also comes into play

Time: 3595.3

in sibling interactions and so forth,

Time: 3597.02

and that's the left brain system

Time: 3598.51

as described by Allan Schore.

Time: 3600.25

Now, again, this isn't about emotion versus rationality,

Time: 3603.25

this is about autonomic

Time: 3604.62

versus more conscious forms of bonding.

Time: 3606.74

So on the left brain circuit side,

Time: 3609.85

there is evidence for based on neuroimaging studies,

Time: 3612.57

but also animal studies to support the idea

Time: 3615.09

that on the left brain side of things,

Time: 3617.57

there is a processing more of narratives

Time: 3621.05

that are very concrete, logical narratives, okay.

Time: 3624

And again, I have to zoom out

Time: 3625.42

and just really tamped down the idea

Time: 3628.07

that it's not that one side of the brain is emotional

Time: 3629.976

and the other side is rational,

Time: 3631.529

but rather that these two things are happening in parallel.

Time: 3634.52

And that there's a bit of a dominance

Time: 3636.88

for the left brain circuitry

Time: 3638.72

to be involved in the kinds of bonding

Time: 3641.179

that are associated with prediction and reward.

Time: 3644.22

So good example would be reading to a child every night,

Time: 3648.19

sitting there and reading,

Time: 3649.39

you know, I can recall reading to my niece

Time: 3651.14

and seeing her parents read to her.

Time: 3653.6

And she had no clue whatsoever with what they were saying

Time: 3656.39

because she, well, at least, I don't know,

Time: 3658.017

but she certainly couldn't speak,

Time: 3659.92

but she liked looking at the pictures,

Time: 3661.71

and it was a very predictable sort of interaction.

Time: 3664.64

It was okay, out come the books,

Time: 3666.3

it was usually here's the bath,

Time: 3667.95

then there's the pajamas,

Time: 3669.06

then there's the lights go down,

Time: 3670.45

then out comes the book,

Time: 3671.49

and then there's the interaction between parent and child,

Time: 3673.86

which of course usually also involves physical contact.

Time: 3676.97

So it's not like the right brain system

Time: 3677.987

and the left brain system are operating separately,

Time: 3680.31

they're operating in parallel.

Time: 3681.94

But that sort of prediction and reward

Time: 3684.04

kids like to be read to,

Time: 3685.55

is generally mediated by this left brain system.

Time: 3688

And this carries on as children get older

Time: 3690.14

and as parents take on and evolve their parenting roles.

Time: 3694.36

It's very apparent that healthy social bonding

Time: 3698.98

between children and caretaker

Time: 3702.06

relies on the fact that both this right brain system

Time: 3704.677

and the left brain system are engaged,

Time: 3706.87

that there's a synchronization of autonomic function,

Time: 3709.97

meaning a joining together in actual somatic feeling,

Time: 3714.28

and that there's a synchronization of experience

Time: 3718.44

that's more about some outward or external stimulus,

Time: 3721.3

like reading a book or watching a show together

Time: 3723.49

or enjoying some common experience of a meal together.

Time: 3727.93

And of course, as children get older,

Time: 3729.26

they're able to access more and more cognitively

Time: 3731.088

sophisticated things.

Time: 3732.65

You can watch a movie with them

Time: 3733.86

and they'll make predictions about which characters

Time: 3735.59

are going to show up for instance,

Time: 3737.16

or you can take to a concert

Time: 3739.248

and they can appreciate the concert or play in that concert,

Time: 3742.64

and they appreciate that they're being appreciated.

Time: 3744.417

Okay, so there are a million different,

Time: 3746.11

there's infinite number of examples here,

Time: 3747.93

but the idea is that there are two parallel circuits

Time: 3750.8

that are important for establishing bonds,

Time: 3753.36

and that this is set up very early on in childhood.

Time: 3756.34

And that it's neither emotional nor rational, but both.

Time: 3760.48

Now both of these circuits tap into the circuitry

Time: 3763.23

that we talked about earlier,

Time: 3764.48

where dopamine is released and molecules like serotonin,

Time: 3768.62

which again is a neuromodulator

Time: 3770.14

more associated with feelings of warmth, comfort,

Time: 3773.41

and satisfaction with our immediate surroundings

Time: 3775.79

and possessions rather than seeking of things

Time: 3778.03

and motivation and drive to go look for things,

Time: 3780.25

as is the case with dopamine.

Time: 3781.87

So there's still interactions with those systems,

Time: 3784.46

but the work of Allan Schore has stimulated

Time: 3787.33

a lot of interest in what are the circuits

Time: 3790.67

that underlie this autonomic bonding,

Time: 3793.149

this matching of heart rate and breathing,

Time: 3797.63

and what are the neural circuits

Time: 3799.41

that underlie this bonding or this synchronization

Time: 3802.778

of experience on the kind of left brain side.

Time: 3806.56

And the reason I find this model so attractive

Time: 3809.02

is that it's very clear that healthy child-parent bonds

Time: 3812.688

are established, but not by one or the other

Time: 3816.15

of these right brainer left brain systems, but by both.

Time: 3819.13

And there isn't enough time to go into it right now,

Time: 3822.06

but some of you are probably familiar with this idea

Time: 3824.94

of anxious attached versus avoidant attached

Time: 3827.8

versus there's a kind of dissociative attached

Time: 3830.153

model of infant parent bonding, just briefly.

Time: 3835.1

What's becoming clear from the neuro-biological

Time: 3837.31

imaging studies

Time: 3838.53

is that as people start to advance into adolescence

Time: 3843.2

and adulthood and well into their elderly years,

Time: 3847.77

the same circuits that were active

Time: 3849.73

and established in childhood

Time: 3851.44

are repurposed for other forms of attachment.

Time: 3854.87

And that to have truly complete bonds

Time: 3858.1

with other individuals,

Time: 3859.01

but in particular with romantic partners,

Time: 3861.59

it's important that there be both synchronization

Time: 3864.08

of physiology and synchronization of these more,

Time: 3867.18

I guess we could call them more rational

Time: 3869.25

or predictive type circuits.

Time: 3871.59

So we can leverage this information,

Time: 3874.04

we can start to think about what sorts of bonds

Time: 3876.92

to us feel very enriching and very complete.

Time: 3879.91

We know that we can have,

Time: 3881.19

for instance, an emotional connection with somebody,

Time: 3884.05

but we can also have a cognitive connection with somebody.

Time: 3886.46

I have many colleagues with whom I have deep intellectual

Time: 3890.67

connection and convergence with.

Time: 3892.8

I won't say that I have deep emotional connection

Time: 3896.04

with most of them, a few of them, yes,

Time: 3898.21

but most of them no.

Time: 3900.96

Others in my life, for instance,

Time: 3903.19

I have a deep emotional connection to,

Time: 3905.85

but not a lot of deep cognitive connection to.

Time: 3908.38

A good example would be the connection

Time: 3909.71

that I had with my bulldog,

Time: 3911.088

who unfortunately passed away.

Time: 3912.62

But Costello, we had a very close,

Time: 3915.81

emotional connection, right?

Time: 3917.67

It was based on touch, it was based on our walks,

Time: 3919.54

it was based on fun, it was very autonomic.

Time: 3922.75

We rarely discussed if ever what we were doing,

Time: 3925.91

we had a felt relationship

Time: 3927.94

as opposed to a cognitive relationship.

Time: 3930.04

And while I'm sort of half kidding about that,

Time: 3932.76

as an example, it's a really good example,

Time: 3934.62

it was a very real bond.

Time: 3936.32

And in fact, just as a brief anecdote,

Time: 3938.26

I can remember when Costello was a puppy

Time: 3940.09

and I was entirely responsible for his well-being,

Time: 3944.21

like any parent of any infant,

Time: 3945.8

I lost my appetite for those few weeks

Time: 3948.49

when I was house training him

Time: 3949.81

and I seem to lose all ability

Time: 3952.29

to process any cognitive information.

Time: 3954.1

Now I was also sleep deprived,

Time: 3955.67

but I was entirely focused on the autonomic bond

Time: 3958.892

that we were forming.

Time: 3960.17

And now, thankfully,

Time: 3961.34

that eventually was established pretty quickly.

Time: 3963.81

Basically I went on to just basically feed him,

Time: 3966.125

walk him and do everything for him,

Time: 3968.52

and we had a wonderful relationship.

Time: 3970.79

Now it's very clear that what we're talking about here

Time: 3972.76

is a form of empathy.

Time: 3975.3

Empathy is the ability to feel,

Time: 3978.27

or at least think we feel what others feel.

Time: 3981.62

Because again, as my colleague and the great bioengineer

Time: 3985.18

and psychiatrist at Stanford, Karl Deisseroth has said,

Time: 3988.75

and he was a guest on this podcast.

Time: 3991.42

We really don't know how other people feel,

Time: 3993.48

we just get the sense that perhaps we are feeling

Time: 3995.53

the same thing

Time: 3996.363

or we're feeling something different and we infer,

Time: 3998.15

or we project what they might be thinking.

Time: 4001.4

Empathy is this sense that we are sensing

Time: 4004.356

what other people are sensing, okay.

Time: 4006.207

And there's no real way to verify that

Time: 4008.1

except if you're measuring physiologies,

Time: 4009.7

you could get some insight into that.

Time: 4011.66

In the clinical psychology

Time: 4012.81

and in the neuro-biological literature now,

Time: 4015.97

it's understood that there is both emotional empathy,

Time: 4019.38

like actually feeling what somebody is feeling

Time: 4021.45

and what is now called cognitive empathy.

Time: 4024.74

Cognitive empathy is this idea

Time: 4026.26

that we both see and experience something

Time: 4028.95

the same way at a mental level,

Time: 4031.63

emotional empathy is this idea

Time: 4033.34

that yes, I can feel what you feel

Time: 4035.087

at a visceral sematic or autonomic level.

Time: 4039.83

And it's absolutely clear that strong social bonds

Time: 4043.745

between children and caretaker

Time: 4046.72

involve both emotional empathy,

Time: 4048.54

this autonomic function and cognitive empathy,

Time: 4051.28

that there's a mutual understanding

Time: 4053.41

of how the other person feels

Time: 4056.01

and how the other person thinks

Time: 4058.35

in order to be able to make predictions

Time: 4059.88

about what they're going to do.

Time: 4061.25

It's also very clear based on the emerging literature,

Time: 4064.12

that romantic relationships,

Time: 4065.77

and to some extent, friendships,

Time: 4067.69

although friendships have been explored a bit less

Time: 4070.12

in the literature,

Time: 4071.24

that emotional empathy and cognitive empathy

Time: 4074.13

are both required in order to establish

Time: 4076.7

what we call a trusting social bond.

Time: 4078.92

And there's some beautiful experiments done

Time: 4081.01

using neuroimaging of two individuals playing a trust game,

Time: 4084.02

essentially a game where you're trying to predict

Time: 4085.79

the other person's behavior,

Time: 4086.88

whether or not they will behave in a trustworthy way.

Time: 4089.09

And these experiments tend to use real money,

Time: 4092.189

so there's actually something at stake,

Time: 4094.11

and you can more or less predict

Time: 4095.79

whether or not somebody feels

Time: 4097.65

a lot of trust for somebody else

Time: 4099.24

and whether or not they believe

Time: 4100.45

they will act in a trustworthy manner

Time: 4103.04

based on whether or not they have high levels

Time: 4105.11

of both cognitive empathy and emotional empathy.

Time: 4108.38

So for those of you that are seeking

Time: 4109.72

to establish deeper bonds or bonds of any kind,

Time: 4113.03

it's important that you think

Time: 4114.03

about synchronization of bodily states,

Time: 4116.17

we talked about that earlier,

Time: 4117.7

and synchronization of cognitive states.

Time: 4120.11

Now that doesn't mean you have to agree on everything.

Time: 4121.82

In fact, oftentimes people who feel very close

Time: 4124.92

to one another cognitively and emotionally

Time: 4127.73

argue about all sorts of things

Time: 4128.98

and disagree about a lot of things.

Time: 4130.25

In fact, we probably know,

Time: 4131.549

I certainly know people and couples

Time: 4134.1

that seem to bond through arguing,

Time: 4136.12

which is an interesting phenotype in itself.

Time: 4139.08

But the point isn't that there'd be total convergence

Time: 4141.74

of opinion or stance,

Time: 4143.39

but rather that we understand how the other feels

Time: 4146.51

and we believe that they understand how we feel,

Time: 4149.13

that we understand how the other person thinks

Time: 4151.42

and that they think that we understand how they think.

Time: 4155.26

So it's a reciprocal loop between two people

Time: 4157.58

that involves this cognition and involves emotion,

Time: 4160.57

and it's grounded as Dr. Schore has pointed out

Time: 4164.07

in our earliest forms of attachment.

Time: 4166.29

And that makes perfect sense

Time: 4167.85

because the same sorts of circuits

Time: 4169.51

that are responsible for social homeostasis,

Time: 4172.3

the kind of right brain and left brain circuits

Time: 4173.89

that are responsible for infant-mother attachment,

Time: 4177.26

and then later for more intellectual

Time: 4179.392

or predictive type attachments

Time: 4181.23

between child and caregiver,

Time: 4183.39

are the exact same circuits that we superimpose

Time: 4185.95

into all other types of relationships

Time: 4188.02

throughout the rest of our life.

Time: 4189.84

And I should just mention that for those of you

Time: 4192.24

that might be thinking that you had a less than satisfactory

Time: 4197.64

infant-caretaker interaction or form of attachment,

Time: 4201.67

you are not alone.

Time: 4202.75

And in fact, much of the work that Dr Schore focuses on

Time: 4205.76

is about how those early circumstances

Time: 4208.46

can be understood and rewired

Time: 4210.57

toward the development of healthy adult attachment.

Time: 4213.15

And if you want to check out his work,

Time: 4215.05

he's actually got a few YouTube videos out there,

Time: 4218.24

again, it's Allan Schore, spelled S-C-H-O-R-E,

Time: 4221.39

I'd love to get him as a guest on the podcast.

Time: 4223.39

He also has a book it's called "Right Brain Psychotherapy."

Time: 4226.3

And it's an excellent book,

Time: 4227.44

it's actually pretty accessible

Time: 4228.59

even if you don't have a background

Time: 4229.73

in biology or psychology,

Time: 4232.4

I found it to be very interesting,

Time: 4234.29

there are a lot of excellent references.

Time: 4236.09

And again, if you're listening Dr. Schore

Time: 4237.837

or, you know, Allan Schore,

Time: 4238.95

we'd love to get you on the podcast.

Time: 4241.04

One of the key themes to understand

Time: 4242.82

about biological processes

Time: 4244.63

is that they often work on short timescales

Time: 4247.003

and longer timescales.

Time: 4248.76

And up until now, we've mainly been talking

Time: 4250.45

about the stuff that happens on short timescales.

Time: 4252.79

So the kind of synchronization of heart rate

Time: 4254.7

or activation of a given set of neurons

Time: 4256.81

that dumps some dopamine

Time: 4257.83

and causes us to seek out more social interaction

Time: 4261.13

or less for instance.

Time: 4262.82

But every biological circuit and function

Time: 4265.78

needs to have longstanding effects as well.

Time: 4268.8

And typically when you're thinking

Time: 4269.97

about longstanding effects in the brain and body,

Time: 4272.41

you start looking towards the hormone system.

Time: 4274.37

It's not always the case,

Time: 4275.35

but more often than not

Time: 4276.98

neuro-transmitters and neuromodulators are pretty quick,

Time: 4279.95

whereas hormones have longer lasting effects.

Time: 4282.18

In fact, a lot of hormones can actually travel

Time: 4283.827

to the nucleus of a cell

Time: 4285.28

and actually change which genes are expressed.

Time: 4288.1

So if ever there was a hormone or hormone-like molecule

Time: 4292.19

that's associated with social bonding, it's oxytocin,

Time: 4295.97

and oxytocin has gotten a ton of interest

Time: 4299.2

in the popular press.

Time: 4300.47

I don't know why that is,

Time: 4301.56

but perhaps it's because of all the incredible things

Time: 4303.58

that oxytocin is associated with.

Time: 4306.27

And it is indeed a lot of things.

Time: 4308.76

So for instance, oxytocin is released in the brain

Time: 4312.52

and binds to receptors in different locations in the body,

Time: 4315.6

and the moment you hear different locations

Time: 4317.23

in the body receptors, you should think,

Time: 4319.97

well, it's going to have lots of different effects.

Time: 4321.71

And indeed it does.

Time: 4322.87

Oxytocin is involved in orgasm,

Time: 4325.84

it's involved in social recognition.

Time: 4328.53

That's right, when you see people

Time: 4330

that you consider your people,

Time: 4331.94

your team, your group, your friends, oxytocin is released.

Time: 4336.41

Even if you don't come into physical contact with them.

Time: 4339.7

Oxytocin is also associated with pair bonding,

Time: 4342.22

the feeling that they are your person,

Time: 4344.504

and that you are their person,

Time: 4346.317

is the common language people use.

Time: 4348.63

It's also associated with honesty.

Time: 4351.49

Believe it or not, there are experiments

Time: 4352.75

that show that if people receive oxytocin

Time: 4355.236

through an inhalation spray,

Time: 4357.35

that they will be more honest

Time: 4358.64

and forthcoming about certain things.

Time: 4360.88

And the oxytocin system and variants in the oxytocin system

Time: 4364.359

have also been associated with autism

Time: 4366.287

and various autism spectrum disorders.

Time: 4368.71

So there's a huge range of behaviors

Time: 4370.42

that's involved in because you have receptors

Time: 4372.43

for oxytocin in lots of different brain structures

Time: 4374.77

and areas of the body that do different things.

Time: 4376.84

However, there's some very consistent effects of oxytocin

Time: 4379.64

that are worth just listing off,

Time: 4381.03

and then I'm going to talk about two separate pathways

Time: 4384.06

by which oxytocin can manifest its effects,

Time: 4386.772

and how you can actually regulate oxytocin

Time: 4389.43

in ways that are interesting and perhaps useful as well.

Time: 4393.27

First of all, oxytocin is involved in the milk,

Time: 4396.19

let down reflex, lactation.

Time: 4398.43

This makes perfect sense,

Time: 4399.46

there needs to be a cue by which

Time: 4401.76

the suckling on the nipple of the infant

Time: 4404.6

causes the release or let down of milk,

Time: 4407.09

and milk let down and lactation

Time: 4408.46

is controlled by prolactin, another hormone,

Time: 4410.5

but also oxytocin.

Time: 4413.21

Oxytocin is also involved in uterine contraction

Time: 4416.23

during childbirth,

Time: 4418.35

it's involved in cervical dilation

Time: 4419.94

to allow the baby to pass out of the birth canal.

Time: 4423.55

So it's involved in induction of breastfeeding and of labor,

Time: 4427.7

which is remarkable and especially remarkable

Time: 4430.23

given that in males or at least in some male animals

Time: 4432.62

and in some male humans,

Time: 4433.78

and I do want to say some, and I'll get back to this,

Time: 4435.88

it can be involved in the erection response,

Time: 4438.2

it can be involved in the orgasm response

Time: 4440.12

in both males and females.

Time: 4441.46

Although there there's a very interesting difference.

Time: 4443.81

There's a little bit of controversy about this,

Time: 4446.54

but it does appear that in females,

Time: 4450.55

sexual and orgasm cause the release of oxytocin,

Time: 4455.169

whereas in males, sexual stimulation

Time: 4457.63

does not cause the release of oxytocin,

Time: 4459.377

but rather a different molecule vasopressin

Time: 4462.35

is triggered by sexual stimulation,

Time: 4464.97

but orgasm does trigger the release of oxytocin in males,

Time: 4467.87

but with a delay of about 30 minutes.

Time: 4471.09

Why that is in the specific function of that is not clear,

Time: 4474.17

but it does seem the oxytocin is involved

Time: 4476.48

in the sexual response in both males and females.

Time: 4479.42

The main types of interactions that release oxytocin

Time: 4482.51

at high levels are first of all,

Time: 4485.28

that the interaction between individuals

Time: 4487.642

that see each other as very closely associated, right?

Time: 4492.63

So a infant and mother are very closely associated

Time: 4495.327

whether or not it's an adopted infant or not.

Time: 4497.55

Oftentimes they are in close contact,

Time: 4499.05

oftentimes they are from the very body of the other.

Time: 4501.9

And so the amount or the amplitude of oxytocin

Time: 4504.36

released tends to scale

Time: 4506

with how closely associated individuals are

Time: 4508.43

just the sight of one's baby or smell of one's baby

Time: 4511.56

can evoke oxytocin release and vice versa from the mother.

Time: 4515.08

Physical contact, even more so in romantic partners.

Time: 4519.38

Physical contact,

Time: 4520.68

even the sight of a picture of a partner

Time: 4523.01

can evoke oxytocin release and sexual desire also trust.

Time: 4527.67

So there's this whole collection of psychological

Time: 4529.67

and physiological things that are packaged

Time: 4531.91

into the oxytocin system.

Time: 4534.13

It's not just a one way system.

Time: 4536.61

Now, a lot of people out there

Time: 4538.16

have written to me asking about inhaling oxytocin,

Time: 4541.35

asking whether or not that can actually increase

Time: 4543.76

the depth or rate of pair bonding.

Time: 4546.14

And there does seem to be some evidence for that.

Time: 4548.41

Now, I think in most places, oxytocin is prescription,

Time: 4551.27

although it might be over-the-counter and others,

Time: 4553.76

I don't know, you have to check where you are

Time: 4555.55

as far as I know you can't just go out

Time: 4556.93

and buy oxytocin nasal spray,

Time: 4558.85

although you may be able to,

Time: 4559.83

forgive me, I'm naive to that point.

Time: 4561.91

But it's interesting to note that some drugs

Time: 4565.478

that are being used in clinical trials

Time: 4567.69

for things like trauma,

Time: 4568.86

and are also used in clinical therapeutic settings

Time: 4571.54

for increasing bonding, in particular MDMA,

Time: 4574.58

also called ecstasy,

Time: 4576.86

increase dopamine and serotonin, we know this,

Time: 4579.84

dopamine and serotonin have a vast number of effects

Time: 4582.657

throughout the brain and body

Time: 4583.94

that I've talked about some of them today

Time: 4585.28

and another podcast.

Time: 4586.66

But one of the lesser appreciated effects of MDMA

Time: 4591.07

is that it causes huge increases,

Time: 4593.24

massive increases in the amount of oxytocin

Time: 4595.62

that's released into the brain and body.

Time: 4598.16

And MDMA-assisted psychotherapy while still illegal,

Time: 4602.21

as far as I know,

Time: 4603.12

certainly in the United States,

Time: 4604.09

but in most places throughout the world,

Time: 4605.83

is being explored in clinical trials,

Time: 4607.84

not just for trauma,

Time: 4608.93

not just for depression, not just for eating disorders,

Time: 4611.02

but also for reestablishing

Time: 4613.41

what seemed to be fractured or challenged bonds

Time: 4616.51

between romantic partners.

Time: 4617.94

And while most of the attention has been focused

Time: 4620.13

on the dopaminergic and serotonergic

Time: 4622.831

aspects of the MDMA response,

Time: 4625.57

it's clear to me, based on my read of the literature,

Time: 4628.09

that the enormously elevated oxytocin

Time: 4630.95

that occurs during the consumption of MDMA

Time: 4634.43

is part of the reason why people

Time: 4636.46

experience during the MDMA session and post MDMA session,

Time: 4640.85

a much greater degree and depth of kinship

Time: 4644.66

or feeling of connection with that person.

Time: 4646.75

And it's important to point out

Time: 4648.42

that that feeling of connection is of the autonomic type

Time: 4651.78

that I was referring to earlier, al Allan Schore's work.

Time: 4655.08

That it's not of the, oh, we think about things

Time: 4656.906

the exact same way,

Time: 4658.26

we agree on everything now,

Time: 4659.6

it's more of that their physiologies are synchronized.

Time: 4663.31

So much so that even in individuals within a couple

Time: 4667.21

where one does a therapeutic session and the other does not,

Time: 4670.92

they still both feel quite more bonded to the other.

Time: 4674.09

Now, oftentimes in the clinical therapeutic setting,

Time: 4676.61

both members of a couple or romantic partnership,

Time: 4679.39

whatever form it may take or consuming MDMA

Time: 4683.86

and then thereby experiencing elevated oxytocin

Time: 4687.679

and this enhanced sense of bonding,

Time: 4689.63

and again, it's this autonomic bonding,

Time: 4691.45

but it's so powerful.

Time: 4693.7

Meaning the oxytocin response is so powerful

Time: 4695.88

that it doesn't even require that both individuals

Time: 4698.87

experienced this user inflection and oxytocin,

Time: 4701.14

and that's because one person's physiology

Time: 4703.297

is influencing the other,

Time: 4704.94

and oxytocin is this kind of bridging signal

Time: 4708.41

that occurs in both nervous systems,

Time: 4710.13

synchronizes things like heartbeat,

Time: 4711.81

obviously it's associated with touch.

Time: 4713.843

And so if people are touching

Time: 4715.5

or people are engaging in the sorts of behaviors

Time: 4717.16

that I mentioned earlier,

Time: 4717.993

that can increase oxytocin further,

Time: 4720.74

that's going to further increase the depth of the bond.

Time: 4723.31

But the point here is that there's actually a hormonal glue

Time: 4727.17

between individuals, okay, infant and mother,

Time: 4730.13

friends, teammates, romantic partners, and so on.

Time: 4733.8

And that hormonal glue is oxytocin.

Time: 4736.85

Now people vary in the extent to which they feel

Time: 4740.61

or have the capacity to feel bonded to anyone.

Time: 4744.38

And it is now generally understood

Time: 4747.32

that some of that variation

Time: 4749.41

might depend on variations in oxytocin receptors

Time: 4752.88

or what are called gene polymorphisms for oxytocin.

Time: 4756.04

Genes can have a number of different sequences in them,

Time: 4758.53

they're nucleotide sequences,

Time: 4759.75

we won't go into genetics right now,

Time: 4761.31

As and Gs and Cs and Ts in various combinations

Time: 4763.81

are what make up the genes.

Time: 4764.79

Genes are transcribed into RNA,

Time: 4767.54

RNA is translated into proteins that affect cells, okay?

Time: 4773.01

The oxytocin gene encodes for oxytocin

Time: 4775.81

and variants in that gene change the amount

Time: 4778.7

and function of oxytocin.

Time: 4781.61

There's a really interesting study

Time: 4783.82

published just this last year in a relatively new journal,

Time: 4786.68

the journal has a kind of a unusual name it's Heliyon,

Time: 4789.84

I think it's Heliyon and not hellion,

Time: 4791.4

but Heliyon, H-E-L-I-Y-O-N.

Time: 4793.69

This is a Cell Press journal,

Time: 4795.45

as far as I can tell, it's a very solid journal,

Time: 4797.84

certainly the Cell Press label is very stringent.

Time: 4801.49

And this paper is entitled,

Time: 4804.1

The Relation Between Oxytocin Receptor

Time: 4806.062

Gene Polymorphisms,

Time: 4807.75

which just means changes in genes or variations in genes,

Time: 4812.15

Adult Attachment and Instagram Sociability,

Time: 4815.35

and Exploratory Analysis.

Time: 4816.69

This is a really wild study,

Time: 4818.04

but I liked the study, It's very thorough.

Time: 4819.88

First author, last name, Carollo, C-A-R-O-L-L-O.

Time: 4823.96

And what they found was that by analyzing

Time: 4827.89

the genetics of different individuals

Time: 4829.98

who are on social media,

Time: 4832.13

and looking at how many people those individuals follow

Time: 4836.85

and how many people follow them,

Time: 4839.74

and what they come up with

Time: 4841.13

is a so-called social desirability index,

Time: 4844.12

they were able to correlate in a very straightforward way

Time: 4847.85

that people that carry certain variants in the oxytocin

Time: 4851.58

and oxytocin receptor genes actually seek out more online,

Time: 4856.29

social Instagram interactions.

Time: 4858.9

So some people I know, I won't name their names,

Time: 4862.01

only follow, you know, anywhere from zero to six accounts,

Time: 4865.7

other people follow thousands of accounts

Time: 4868.79

and they take the ratio of how many accounts people follow

Time: 4871.8

versus how many followers they have,

Time: 4873.74

arguably not a perfect measure,

Time: 4875.28

but a nice one in the sense that you can do this

Time: 4877.44

in a completely unbiased way

Time: 4879.27

with many, many thousands of subjects.

Time: 4881.65

And then they were able to get genomic analysis

Time: 4884.42

from a number of these subjects.

Time: 4885.84

And it turns out that people who have,

Time: 4889.14

let's say higher levels of oxytocin function

Time: 4893.08

or potential levels of oxytocin function,

Time: 4897.06

actively seek out more social interactions on social media.

Time: 4901.03

So this, I think represents an important first

Time: 4903.35

in the area of how social media and data from social media

Time: 4906.43

are starting to merge with biological data

Time: 4909.09

in terms of predicting how avidly people will seek out

Time: 4912.53

social interactions of an online type.

Time: 4915.18

And nowadays, we hear a lot about how online,

Time: 4917.979

we are connected but we're not really...

Time: 4920.56

What is it? We're communicating

Time: 4921.81

but we're not connected

Time: 4922.72

or the connections aren't real.

Time: 4924.25

I think we're going to need to revisit that.

Time: 4926.09

While I'm certainly a believer in the idea

Time: 4928.01

that face-to-face communication

Time: 4929.56

and common interactions with people

Time: 4932.41

standing in the same space

Time: 4934.13

or playing sports together, enjoying music together,

Time: 4936.66

enjoying meals together is vitally important,

Time: 4938.93

there's an entire generation,

Time: 4941.04

or several generations of people

Time: 4943.05

that are coming up who much of their social

Time: 4945.72

interaction has been online.

Time: 4947.32

And if you think about it,

Time: 4948.58

all of the things that we've spelled out earlier

Time: 4951.02

about common mental narrative,

Time: 4953.6

this left-brain system al Allan Schore,

Time: 4955.79

or autonomic bonding or synchronization of heartbeats

Time: 4959.94

according to common stories,

Time: 4961.27

all that is happening in online social interactions.

Time: 4964.5

When a thousand of us

Time: 4965.79

look at the exact same Instagram post,

Time: 4968.4

yes, we will have a thousand independent responses to that,

Time: 4971.97

but chances are many of us have a similar or same response

Time: 4974.94

based on the data that we talked about earlier

Time: 4976.88

in synchronization of heartbeats.

Time: 4978.38

And so we are socially bonded with other people

Time: 4981.37

through social media,

Time: 4982.68

and it's very apparent that the oxytocin system

Time: 4985.53

is playing some role in that.

Time: 4987.66

And this, if we zoom out makes perfect sense,

Time: 4990.155

because again, dopamine, serotonin, prolactin, oxytocin,

Time: 4995.43

none of these systems were placed in us

Time: 4998.67

or are organized within us in order to encourage specific

Time: 5003.1

and only specific types of social interactions.

Time: 5005.87

The one that we can say is absolutely critical

Time: 5008.06

is the child-parent interaction, right?

Time: 5010.38

Because children simply can't take care of themselves,

Time: 5012.15

they need a caretaker.

Time: 5013.21

I should have said caretaker, not parent.

Time: 5015.15

But infants, if they're not taken care of will die.

Time: 5018.57

But beyond that, we have evolved

Time: 5021.21

or come to realize many different types

Time: 5023.61

of social interactions,

Time: 5024.75

and online interactions nowadays are very, very common.

Time: 5027.814

I'm certainly involved in them,

Time: 5029.27

I'm guessing you're involved in them as well,

Time: 5030.64

we're involved in one right now, for example.

Time: 5033.71

The oxytocin system is absolutely threaded through

Time: 5036.87

and largely responsible for those types

Time: 5039.49

of social bonds as well.

Time: 5041.36

And incidentally, "Oxytocin" is the name

Time: 5043.56

of the fifth song on Billie Eilish's

Time: 5045

second album, "Happier Than Ever."

Time: 5047.13

So we've covered a lot about the biology

Time: 5049.21

and indeed the neural circuitry and neurochemistry

Time: 5051.46

and neuroendocrinology of social bonding.

Time: 5055.26

I want to make sure that I highlight the key features

Time: 5057.76

that go into any and all of your social bonds.

Time: 5061.18

First of all, all social bonds have the potential

Time: 5064.35

to include both what we call emotional empathy

Time: 5067.28

and cognitive empathy.

Time: 5068.66

And so if you are interested in establishing

Time: 5071.09

and deepening social bonds of any kind,

Time: 5074.03

it's important that you put some effort

Time: 5076.57

towards this thing that we call emotional empathy,

Time: 5079.41

which is really about sharing autonomic experience.

Time: 5082.87

Now, depending on the relationship

Time: 5084.01

that will take on different contexts,

Time: 5085.3

what's appropriate in one type of bond

Time: 5087.06

is not going to be appropriate in another type of bond.

Time: 5089.35

Physical contact for instance,

Time: 5090.78

is appropriate for certain types of bonds

Time: 5092.709

and not for others.

Time: 5094.64

Nonetheless, emotional empathy

Time: 5097.61

and the synchronization of autonomic function,

Time: 5099.81

heart rate breathing, et cetera,

Time: 5102.11

can be best accomplished by paying attention

Time: 5103.86

to external events in particular narrative story music

Time: 5107.48

and perhaps sports or other types of experience

Time: 5110.07

as an external stimulus to drive synchrony

Time: 5112.81

of those internal states.

Time: 5115.23

The other aspect of forming deep bonds is cognitive empathy.

Time: 5119

Again, cognitive empathy is not about agreeing on things

Time: 5122.567

or viewing things the exact same way,

Time: 5125.06

it's about really gaining understanding

Time: 5127.43

of how somebody else thinks about something,

Time: 5130.14

really paying attention to that,

Time: 5131.83

and then paying attention to how you think about

Time: 5135.22

and feel about something.

Time: 5136.77

So that's what cognitive empathy is.

Time: 5138.33

So emotional and cognitive empathy together

Time: 5140.507

are what make up these really robust bonds of various kinds.

Time: 5145.1

Now we also talked about introversion and extroversion,

Time: 5148.05

and I'd like to try and dismantle the common misperceptions

Time: 5151.14

about introversion extroversion

Time: 5153.01

because when we look at the neural circuitry, as you recall,

Time: 5155.71

introverts are not people

Time: 5157.55

that don't like social interaction,

Time: 5159.74

it's just that they feel filled up or sated

Time: 5163.4

by less social interaction than would be an extrovert.

Time: 5167.61

And that's because at least according to the social

Time: 5170.22

homeostasis circuit model,

Time: 5171.7

they actually get more dopamine

Time: 5173.65

from less social interaction.

Time: 5175.463

Okay, it's like somebody who is sated

Time: 5177.41

by less amount of food.

Time: 5179.35

Okay, it doesn't mean they don't have the same appetite,

Time: 5181.68

it just means that they get more from less.

Time: 5184.41

Whereas extroverts get less dopamine release

Time: 5188.8

from an equivalent amount of social interaction.

Time: 5191.35

And of course these aren't precise measurements,

Time: 5193.21

but on the whole extroverts need more social interaction,

Time: 5196.56

more frequent, more long-lasting, et cetera,

Time: 5199.51

in order to achieve that dopamine threshold,

Time: 5201.56

because again, dopamine is driving

Time: 5203.84

that craving of social interaction.

Time: 5206.46

And once it's met, then people don't feel

Time: 5208.55

like they have to seek social interaction as much.

Time: 5211.23

So for those of you that feel as if you're an introvert

Time: 5213.7

or extrovert or that know introverts and extroverts,

Time: 5217.13

it's not about how verbal people are,

Time: 5219.74

it's not about how much they seek out

Time: 5221.4

social interactions per se,

Time: 5223.46

it's about how much social interaction

Time: 5225.29

is enough for the given person.

Time: 5228.16

Now, the whole reason for providing this framework,

Time: 5230.25

this biological circuitry, et cetera,

Time: 5232.84

is not to simply put a reductionist view

Time: 5235.79

on things that you already realized and knew,

Time: 5238.24

but rather to give you some leverage points

Time: 5240.25

to understand how is it that you form social bonds?

Time: 5243.38

How is it that you might be challenged

Time: 5244.67

in forming certain types of social bonds?

Time: 5246.62

And to think about entry points,

Time: 5248.54

to both establishing

Time: 5249.75

and reinforcing social bonds of different kinds.

Time: 5252.66

Hopefully it will also give you insight into why breakups,

Time: 5256.69

whether it be between friendships or romantic partners

Time: 5258.85

can be so painful.

Time: 5260.65

A breakup of any kind involves both a breaking

Time: 5263.24

of that emotional empathy and that cognitive empathy.

Time: 5266.36

And indeed it has a neuro-biological

Time: 5269.201

and hormonal underpinning, right?

Time: 5271.18

We go into some sense, a social isolation,

Time: 5274.35

even if we're surrounded by other types of people,

Time: 5276.81

if one of our major sources of oxytocin

Time: 5279.62

or one of our major sources of dopamine

Time: 5281.91

suddenly is not around,

Time: 5283.77

that is incredibly devastating to a nervous system.

Time: 5286.77

And to borrow from the great psychologist

Time: 5288.968

and neurobiologist Lisa Feldman Barrett,

Time: 5291.64

who says, you know, we are not just individuals,

Time: 5294.25

we are nervous systems influencing other nervous systems

Time: 5297.131

and their nervous systems are influencing us.

Time: 5299.38

I think that's the right way to think about it.

Time: 5301.44

So it should come as no surprise

Time: 5303.42

that breakups of various kinds are very challenging

Time: 5306.44

regardless of what underlied that breakup,

Time: 5309.67

whether or not somebody's moving

Time: 5310.73

or an actual decision of one person

Time: 5312.35

to leave the relationship or both, et cetera.

Time: 5315.35

On the more positive side, largely biological,

Time: 5319.19

but to some extent, psychological view of social bonding

Time: 5323.33

will also allow you to orient in this vast landscape

Time: 5326.561

that we call social bonds.

Time: 5328.7

To understand why it is perhaps that you seek out

Time: 5331.17

so many online interactions.

Time: 5332.52

Maybe you have the oxytocin polymorphism

Time: 5335.92

that causes you to want more, follow more accounts,

Time: 5338.7

or interact more with people and comment more,

Time: 5341.24

respond to comments, who knows?

Time: 5344.98

I'm also hoping that it will allow you to get a lens

Time: 5347.64

into how you can strengthen the social bonds

Time: 5349.76

that you want to strengthen,

Time: 5351.11

and to establish new social bonds

Time: 5352.76

that you want to establish.

Time: 5354.65

None of this has meant to manipulate

Time: 5356.83

or leverage social bonds that wouldn't otherwise form,

Time: 5359.97

to the contrary, it's about identifying

Time: 5362.24

what are the specific routes

Time: 5364.52

by which social bonds are created

Time: 5367.18

and allowing you, I hope, to work with people

Time: 5371.81

that you feel challenged in forming social bonds with,

Time: 5374.57

or maybe deciding to completely divorce

Time: 5376.58

from those social bonds entirely

Time: 5378.19

because there's absolutely no hope

Time: 5379.37

of ever forming emotional or cognitive empathy.

Time: 5381.96

I certainly acknowledge that that could be the case too.

Time: 5384.25

So there's both a light and a dark and a gray zone

Time: 5386.57

to this entire thing that we call social bonding.

Time: 5388.66

What is not graded, but is absolute, as they say,

Time: 5392.56

is that social bonds are vitally important

Time: 5394.94

to us as a species,

Time: 5396.36

whether or not they are at a distance over social media,

Time: 5398.87

whether or not they are in close proximity,

Time: 5400.39

actual physical contact.

Time: 5402.51

Today, what I've really tried to illustrate

Time: 5404.04

is that there are a common set of biological, neurochemical

Time: 5408.28

and hormonal underpinnings to what we call social bonding.

Time: 5411.93

And so while it is complex and it is subjective,

Time: 5415.049

it involves the hierarchies,

Time: 5416.71

it involves our previous upbringing,

Time: 5419.25

it involves our goals, et cetera,

Time: 5422.05

it is not infinitely complex,

Time: 5424.3

and in that sense, it is tractable.

Time: 5426.87

Hopefully I've offered you some leavers

Time: 5429.182

or some entry points under which you can both understand

Time: 5431.47

and move towards social bonds

Time: 5433.14

that would be more satisfying and more gratifying for you.

Time: 5436.75

That's certainly one of the goals.

Time: 5438.02

The other one is that hopefully if you are a clinician

Time: 5440.56

or simply the friend that people go to,

Time: 5442.48

or the family member that people go to

Time: 5443.82

when they are challenged

Time: 5445.09

through various challenges and social bonds,

Time: 5447.62

that you can start to perhaps pass along

Time: 5449.88

some of the information as a way of people understanding

Time: 5453.832

what they're going through as they are breaking up,

Time: 5456.48

but also as they are falling in love,

Time: 5458.24

as they are forming attachments,

Time: 5459.9

and as they are being challenged with attachments.

Time: 5461.82

That's my hope, and especially as you head

Time: 5464.36

into the holidays and end of year,

Time: 5466.69

but also as it continues into 2022,

Time: 5469.91

I would hope that you would take this knowledge

Time: 5471.45

and apply it in any of the ways

Time: 5472.86

that you feel are meaningful and adaptive for you.

Time: 5475.86

If you're learning from and/or enjoying this podcast,

Time: 5478.35

please subscribe to our YouTube channel,

Time: 5480.52

that really helps us.

Time: 5481.43

In addition, please put comments in the comment section

Time: 5484.49

on YouTube, if you have them,

Time: 5486.18

and if you have suggestions for future podcast guests

Time: 5488.99

that you'd like us to host on the Huberman Lab Podcast,

Time: 5491.15

please put those in the comment section as well.

Time: 5492.98

We do eventually read all the comments.

Time: 5496.08

In addition, please subscribe to the Huberman Lab Podcast

Time: 5498.57

on Apple and/or Spotify.

Time: 5500.2

And on Apple, you have the opportunity to leave us

Time: 5502.66

up to a five star review and a comment if you like.

Time: 5505.33

Please also check out our sponsors

Time: 5506.82

mentioned at the beginning of the podcast,

Time: 5508.53

that's perhaps the best way to support this podcast.

Time: 5511.12

And we have a Patreon, it's patreon.com/andrewhuberman.

Time: 5514.79

And there you can support the podcast

Time: 5516.52

at any level that you like.

Time: 5518.13

We didn't talk about supplements on today's episode

Time: 5520.06

of the Huberman Lab Podcast, but on many episodes we do.

Time: 5523.65

While supplements aren't necessary for everybody,

Time: 5525.65

many people derive tremendous benefit from them,

Time: 5527.99

for things like enhancing the depth and quality of sleep,

Time: 5530.9

for things like focus, immune system, et cetera.

Time: 5534.67

If you'd like to see the supplements that I take,

Time: 5536.67

you can go to thorne.com/u/huberman.

Time: 5540.86

The reason we partnered with Thorne

Time: 5542.63

is because thorn has the highest levels of stringency

Time: 5544.87

in terms of the quality of the ingredients

Time: 5547.15

that they include in their supplements

Time: 5548.87

and the precision of the amounts

Time: 5550.54

of the supplements that they include.

Time: 5552.06

This is not true for a lot of other

Time: 5553.39

supplement brands out there.

Time: 5555.61

Thorne is partnered with the Mayo Clinic,

Time: 5557.46

with all the major sports teams,

Time: 5558.61

so there's tremendous confidence in their stringency.

Time: 5561.23

Again, if you go to thorne.com/u/huberman,

Time: 5564.83

you can see all the supplements that I take,

Time: 5566.33

you can get 20% off any of those supplements.

Time: 5568.51

And if you navigate deeper into the Thorne site,

Time: 5570.198

through that portal,

Time: 5571.45

you can also get 20% off any of the other supplements

Time: 5574.08

that Thorne makes.

Time: 5575.22

If you're not already following the Huberman Lab

Time: 5577.07

on Instagram and Twitter, please do so.

Time: 5579.67

on Instagram, I regularly teach short snippets

Time: 5582.57

about neuroscience and neuroscience-related tools,

Time: 5584.68

some of that information overlaps

Time: 5586.32

with what's covered on the podcast, often it does not.

Time: 5589.57

So check us out at Huberman Lab

Time: 5591.1

on Instagram and on Twitter.

Time: 5593.37

And last, but certainly not least,

Time: 5595.43

thank you for your interest in science.

Time: 5597.601

[upbeat music]

Copyright © 2024. All rights reserved.