Dr. Matt Walker: Improve Sleep to Boost Mood & Emotional Regulation | Huberman Lab Guest Series
[Music]
welcome to the hubman lab guest Series
where I and an expert guest discuss
science and science-based tools for
everyday
life I'm Andrew huberman and I'm a
professor of neurobiology and
Opthalmology at Stanford school of
medicine today marks the fifth episode
in our sixth episode series all about
sleep with expert guest Dr Matthew
Walker today's episode focuses on the
inextricable link between sleep and our
mental health for instance a specific
stage of sleep called rapid eye movement
or REM sleep is critical for removing
the emotional content of our previous
days memories and in doing so provides a
sort of therapy within sleep that allows
us to feel emotionally restored when we
wake the next morning we discuss what
happens when you are deprived of REM
sleep to a small or greater degree and
we discuss how to improve the quality
and quantity of your REM sleep in order
to ensure mental health we also discuss
science-based protocols for reducing
rumination and negative thoughts before
sleep the information shared by Dr
Walker in today's episode is sure to be
critical for anyone that is either
struggling with mental health issues or
who simply wants to bolster their
overall mental health before we begin
I'd like to emphasize that this podcast
is separate from my teaching and
research roles at Stanford it is however
part of my desire and effort to bring
zero cost to Consumer information about
science and science related tools to the
general public in keeping with that
theme I'd like to thank the sponsors of
today's podcast our first sponsor is
eight sleep eight sleep makes Smart
mattress covers with with cooling
Heating and sleep tracking capacity many
times on this podcast we discuss how in
order to fall and stay deeply asleep
your body temperature actually needs to
drop by about 1 to 3° and in order to
wake up feeling maximally refreshed and
energized your body temperature needs to
heat up by about 1 to 3° eight sleep
makes it very easy to control the
temperature of your sleeping environment
so that it's easy to fall and stay
asleep and wake up feeling refreshed I
started sleeping on an eight- Sleep
mattress cover several years ago and it
has completely and positively
transformed my sleep so much so that
when I travel to hotels or airbnbs I
really miss my eight sleep I've even
shipped my eight sleep out to hotels
that I've been staying in because it
improves my sleep that much if you'd
like to try eightsleep you can go to
8sleep.com
huberman to save $150 off their pod 3
cover eights sleep currently ships to
the USA Canada UK select countries in
the EU and Australia again that's
8sleep.com
huberman today's episode is also brought
To Us by element element is an
electrolyte drink that has everything
you need and nothing you don't that
means plenty of the electrolytes
magnesium potassium and sodium and no
sugar as I mentioned before on this
podcast I'm a big fan of salt now I want
to be clear people who already consume a
lot of salt or who have high blood
pressure or who happen to consume a lot
of processed foods that typically
contain salt need to control their salt
intake however if you're somebody who
eats pretty clean and you're somebody
who exercises and you're drinking a lot
of water there's a decent chance that
you could benefit from ingesting more
electrolytes with your liquids the
reason for that is that all the cells in
our body including the nerve cells the
neurons require the electrolytes in
order to function properly so we don't
just want to be hydrated we want to be
hydrated with proper electrolyte levels
with element that's very easy to do what
I do is when I wake up in the morning I
consume about 16 to 32 ounces of water
and I'll dissolve a packet of element in
that water I'll also do the same when I
exercise especially if it's on a hot day
and I'm sweating a lot and sometimes
I'll even have a third element packet
dissolved in water if I'm exercising
really hard or sweating a lot or if I
just noticed that I'm not consuming
enough salt with my food if you'd like
to try element you can go to drink
element spelled LM nt.com huberman to
claim a free element sample pack with
your purchase again that's drink element
lnt.com
huberman today's episode is also brought
To Us by betterhelp betterhelp offers
Professional Therapy with a licensed
therapist carried out entirely online
I've been doing therapy for over 30
years initially it was a requirement for
being let back in school but I decided
to keep up with that therapy because Ed
the therapy has three essential
components which are excellent rapport
with the therapist support from the
therapist and valuable insights from the
therapist that we wouldn't otherwise be
able to arrive at well then it's a
terrific way to improve our mental
landscape both our emotional state and
our behaviors better help allows you to
find a therapist with whom you have
those three key components and it also
makes it very easy to schedule again the
sessions are carried out entirely online
and even if you're extremely busy or
traveling a lot or have a lot of family
and business obligations better help
allows you to access those therapy
sessions regularly so that you can
constantly improve there's just oh so
much data supporting that quality
therapy can improve our mental health
and the overall landscape of our lives
in fact I view quality therapy as
important as physical exercise and I
know many others do as well there
certainly aren't a replacement for one
another but if you're doing physical
exercise meaning resistance training and
cardiovascular training and you're doing
regular quality therapy of the sort that
better help can provide well then you're
essentially doing as much as anyone
possibly can to improve your mental
health and physical health if you'd like
to try betterhelp go to betterhelp.com
huberman to get 10% off your first month
again that's betterhelp.com
huberman and now for my conversation
with Dr Matthew Walker Dr Matthew Walker
welcome back Dr Andrew huberman Delight
to be back during the course of this
series we've of course been talking
about sleep and you've talked about the
biology of sleep ways to improve maybe
even optimize sleep you defined what
optimizing one's sleep actually
is talked about learning and memory
creativity caffeine naps food exercise
and so much more
today I'm excited that you're going to
teach us about the relationship between
sleep and emotion regulation but also
mental health mental health challenges
but I sometimes like to remind people
that mental health includes the word
Health it's not all about mental illness
it's also about how to improve one's
Health as well as ways to combat certain
forms of mental illness or challenges so
to start things off maybe you could just
give us the basics of the relationship
between sleep and emotional states or
one's ability to regulate their own
emotions this is an area of work that
we've been uh interested in and doing a
lot of research on for about 20 or so
years
now and I would say that probably the
most striking statement I can offer
upfront is the following in that 20
years of research we have not been able
to discover a single psychiatric
condition in which sleep is
normal and to me it has taught me
everything that I need to know about
this very intimate bidirectional
relationship between your sleep health
and your mental health and you're right
to emphasize that notion of mental
health because we're not just going to
speak about some of the sort of
challenging aspects of sleep and
psychiatric disorders but we'll speak
about some of the benefits that sleep
can provide when you get it to turn the
tables and we move in the direction not
of Mental Illness but we move in the
direction of mental Wellness so I'm
excited sort of make sure that I I don't
fall prey to
that stepping back still though what
about this relationship between just
sleep and our basic emotional regulation
and our emotional
stability I'm sure everyone has seen the
example or had the example as a parent
of that parent holding a child and the
child is crying and they look at you and
they say well they just didn't sleep
well last night as if there's some
miraculous parental knowledge that bad
sleep the night before equals bad mood
and emotional reactivity and regulation
the next day and some years ago now we
were fascinated by this but we couldn't
really unearth basic science that would
help us explain what was going on and
why that was so clearly the case so we
did an initial study where we took a
group of healthy people no signs of
psychiatric illness or emotional
instability and we gave them a full
night of sleep or we sleep deprive them
and then the next day we put them inside
of a brain scanner and we showed them a
whole range of emotional visual images
is ranging from very neutral all the way
up to quite unpleasant and negative and
we were looking at how the brain was
reacting to those emotional experiences
with versus without sleep and the
structure that we'd initially focused on
was a structure that you've spoken about
before called the amydala and you
actually have one on the left and the
right side of your brain and the amydala
is the centerpiece region for the
generation of emotional reactions both
positive and negative but here we're
focusing on that aversive that negative
aspect and when we looked at that
structure in people who are sleep
deprived what we saw relative to the
people who'd had a full night of sleep
was a 60% 60 60% increase in amydala
[Music]
responsivity under conditions of sleep
deprivation that is quite a striking
amplification in fact we to that date
with all of us that these on sleep and
sleep loss had not quite seen an effect
size within the brain that was that was
that big sorry to interrupt but just to
make sure that everyone's on the same
page so people are being shown images
with varying degrees of emotionality
including images that are known to evoke
negative averse emotions as we call them
in the laboratory um could be feelings
of fear anger disgust revulsion what
whatever negative
veilance was it the case that sleep
deprivation increased the activity in
the amydala to such images by 60% only
for the aversive images or for let's say
a neutral image um presented to somebody
who has had plenty of quality
sleep um let's say it is I'm making up
the units here gives us two out of 10
units of Amal activation this isn't the
way Neuroscience is done but for sake of
discussion is it the case then that that
neutral image would provide a you know a
six out of 10 a level of activation or
was it only for aversive images so the
way we did the analysis first was we
used almost a a correlation approach so
we sort of told the brain Imaging
analysis to say look here are the the
ratings of these pictures and they go
from very neutral to increasingly
negative and aversive and show me what
in the brain is reacting to that curve
that gradient curve and sure enough
you've got the magnitude overall was 60%
but it's a very interesting point that
you make because worthy amydala started
to respond and that res responsivity
started to hook up in the activation and
the sort of aggravation Direction was
much earlier in the curve of
emotionality in other words things that
previously when you've had a good night
of sleep do not feel particularly
emotional started to become rather
emotional when you were not getting
sufficient sleep
so it heightened the sensitivity of the
initial triggering of the emotional
response and then the more emotional it
became the more separate those two sort
of reactivity curves became from the
Amala when you had sleep versus when you
had not sleep or had not slept I should
say to us then the question became well
why why is the amydala so reactive and
controlled when you are absent sleep and
we did another analysis and what we
found was that there was a structure in
your frontal lobe and the frontal lobe
just sits directly sort of if you think
about your eyes and you go directly up
your in your frontal lobe and it was a
particular part of the frontal lobe the
middle part that sits right between your
eyes something that we call the medial
prefrontal cortex and what we found was
that with a night of sleep the med Al
prefrontal cortex was strongly connected
to the amydala why is that important
it's because that part of the your
frontal lobe is very good at acting like
a control rational mechanism on your
deep sort of you know it's not
Neanderthal but your deep emotional
brain centers but without sleep we found
that that connection had been
severed and so it was almost as though
without sleep you become all emotional
gas pedal and too little regulatory
control break and so you couldn't
modulate those emotions anywhere near as
effectively now some people may say well
hang on a second you that was a total
night of sleep deprivation and that's
not really relevant for me because I I
don't sleep enough I know that from all
of the previous episodes that uh I've
gone through here hopefully if you've
listened to them but I'm usually maybe
getting five or 6 hours of sleep is this
really relevant so we started doing that
study we wanted to say Let's do an what
we call an ecological study more of a
real world sleep restriction rather than
total
deprivation and we were about halfway
through that study when a wonderful
Japanese research group essentially
published the study that we were doing
and what was great is that they did it
even in a more rigorous way and
essentially what they were able to do is
replicate exactly what we'd found but
now by putting people on sort of less
than 6 hours of sleep for five nights
and sure enough you got the same
response so that was very clear to us
that there is some sensitivity there's a
reason why you become so
unbuckled emotionally when you are not
getting sufficient sleep it's the reason
that you have almost this sort of
erratic pendulum like sort of
responsivity when you're not getting
sufficient sleep that notion of I just
snap dot dot dot or you apologize and
you say look I am so sorry I just bit
your H off I just haven't been getting
enough sleep and so we could start to
understand what in the brain was
happening when you didn't get sleep it
such an important finding uh for a
couple of reasons that maybe we can
explore um previously on the podcast we
had a guest um doctor he's a
neurosurgeon Matt McDougall he's the
lead neurosurgeon at um neurolink yeah
he came up through Stanford works on
deep brain stimulation Etc and I love
his description of what the prefrontal
cortex does it jbes perfectly with the
way you describe it which is he said the
main function of the prefrontal cortex
is to
say to specific brain areas under
specific contexts so um the sh of course
is a you know his way of describing
neural inhibition so quieting of neural
activity in certain brain circuits under
certain conditions because there are
conditions under which you want your
amydala activation to be very robust you
know fast and um there's time for uh you
know protecting oneself maybe even
certain situations for Swift violent
action to protect your family Etc but
the prefrontal cortex seems to be able
to hold it in mind so to speak what the
context is under which that would be
appropriate versus when it would be
inappropriate a great example of that
people can think of if all of a sudden a
gun is pointed in your
face you would want your amydala to
react if it's in the real world but if
you're at the movie theaters and you see
a gun pointed in your face your igula
doesn't really react as much why because
you're prefontal cortex understood the
word that you describe which is
context but in some ways it seems as
though you
become almost regress to this more basic
fundament mental
Elemental you know emotional brain and
the Red Mist descends and you really
can't see much more because your
prefrontal cortex seems to be absent you
become very reflex driven and we don't
want to um go too uh far a tangent of on
prefrontal cortex but one of the most
beautiful descriptions of prefrontal
Cortex I ever heard was also from a
colleague Eric nudson at Stanford who's
now retired does beautiful work on
neuroplasticity and he described how
when people or animals have lesions to
certain regions of the prefrontal cortex
they become stimulus-driven machines
such that you know if you if you go like
this to a you know to a puppy or to Baby
they'll look to the snapping finger but
at some point you know we all learn that
you know there must be a reason for us
to follow the snapping of the fingers in
different locations in space but with a
prefrontal damage people and animals
just become like machines whatever
stimulus is there they Orient to and
this has implications for ADHD Etc one
of the things that I want to um ask
about to take us back to the specific
relationship between sleep reduced
medial prefrontal activity and
emotionality is this feeling when we're
sleep deprived that certain things just
great on us a bit more you know I had
this experience recently unfortunately
there was a night where I didn't get
much sleep at all and then the next day
I was on a phone call and the person I
was talking to I I I'm quite fond of but
they were they had a lot of energy and
they were talking they were kind of
coming at me with a bunch of stuff that
they wanted to and it just felt like you
know it was grading on my system and I
knew because I was sleep deprived that
you know they were entirely well-meaning
and so you just kind of resist but it's
incredible how cold water loud noises uh
requests of our time things like that
become very irritating and they grate on
us yeah when we're sleep deprived
whereas when we're rested it's like oh
yeah okay they're you know talking kind
of faster kind of loud okay somebody is
requesting something else I'll put it in
my list or maybe I'll the Ator or you
know the the uh cold shower that you
know feels like got to get over this
threshold to get into like when you're
rested you're like all right let's do
this right you know maybe even let's go
I I'm excited for it but when you're
tired o it is as if the um the brain is
fighting for any sense of Peace it can
possibly get and that peace is
interrupted by almost anything and
everything it is a grim situation and
we've certainly heard that from you know
patients and individuals it's almost as
though the world that they are
experiencing they look at and they say
you know what you're in an 11 and I need
you at a seven right now it is just too
much and this comes back to that result
that we described that when the amydala
crosses the threshold and says okay
things are getting emotional things are
getting unpleasant I'm going to be
responding
negatively in an angry way or a fearful
way that starts much earlier so the
threshold for triggering your emotional
aversive reaction is much lower and
that's why the person's voice when you
hear it first normally if you had a
great night of sleep you'd say gosh you
know what today I really love your
energy it's it's really it's so
infectious versus a day when you're not
sleeping you just think I just I'm
lifting my earbuds out of my ears I
don't know if I can take this much
longer and so that was where we were
able to manipulate sleep one way which
is to say I dial sleep down and then I
look at the emotional brain and you can
see this ramping up of the emotional
reactivity in these basic kind of gutur
all centers but then we wanted to do the
inverse we wanted to instead see if we
could insert sleep back in in other
words manipulate sleep and dial it back
up could you get a disapp ation in the
emotional
reaction and here we decided to throw a
second ingredient into the equation not
just simply looking at your emotional
reactivity but we wanted to look at
emotional memory now in a previous
episode we've spoken a lot about sleep
and memory but there we were speaking
about really quite neutral memory
textbook like memory fact-based memory
emotional memory is very different and
if I were to ask you you Andrew cast
your mind back to some of your earliest
childhood memories or your team memories
and if anyone listening were to do that
my guess is that almost all of the
memories that you recall are memories of
an emotional nature positive or
negative why is that it's because one of
the functions of emotions when it comes
to memory is to R flag and prioritize
that experience that memory as being
Salient because it's emotional and that
instructs the brain that this
information in particular is very
relevant to us as an organism why
because the rest of the brain is
shouting at me this is emotional so
there is something very privileged and
very special about an emotional memory
like a red flag that tags it for
priority in the brain but something I
started to notice when I would read the
data both the neural data and the
subjective data on emotional memory led
me to get very interested in what
happens with emotional memories over
time because what you will hear is that
if I were to ask you you know recall an
emotional memory just try to remember it
my guess is that now at the time of
recollection much later on you are not
having the same regurgitation of the
same visceral emotional reaction that
you had at the time of the experience
what that sort of turned a light bulb
moment on for me was that somewhere
between the initial experience and the
later recollection of that emotional
memory the brain has done a very clever
trick it has divorced the emotion from
the memory so now when you come to
recollect that emotional memory let's
say days later or even months later in
some ways it is a memory of an emotional
event but it is no longer as powerfully
emotional itself as it was at the time
of the experience right and I started to
wonder is that time or is that time
asleep so we did a study and we had
people experience these emotional
memories sort of essentially make
emotional memories and they were doing
it inside of a scanner and then we gave
them a night of sleep or even a nap and
then we brought them back or we just had
them learn those emotional memories in
the morning and then bring them back
after an identical amount of time to try
to soften those emotional memories but
without sleep and we put them back in
the scanner and we were able to look to
see when you come back later in that
second session is your emotional and you
recollect those experiences and you
relive them is the emotional reactivity
at that second session any different to
the first session and is that different
if that time elapse has contained a full
night of sleep versus you've just been
awake and what we found is that in those
people who remained awake across the day
having had those emotional memories
essentially implanted implanted sounds a
little bit sort of Big Brother I don't
mean it that way but they'd learned them
the amydala was just still as responsive
as they were recalling and reliving and
reexperiencing those emotional memories
but in those people who had the same
amount of time to process the memories
but had had a full night of sleep we saw
this incredible emotional amygdala dep
potentiation and what that taught me was
that the sleeping brain was able
to almost detox the emotional memory it
is think about it like um an
informational orange that the emotional
memory Has This Bitter emotional rind
around it and then you've got the
informational orange in the middle and
what sleep was doing was stripping the
bitter emotional rind off the
informational orange so that then when
you came back the next day again it is
now a memory of an emotional event but
it's no longer triggering that strong
visceral reaction in other words and we
describe this Theory as something called
overnight
forgetting which is that when it comes
to an emotional memory you both sleep to
forget and sleep to remember
respectively which is that you sleep to
remember the information the memory of
the experience but it is no longer
emotional
itself and from there we built a
biological model of exactly how this
works because when we looked at the
Sleep group who'd had that full 8-hour
opportunity we asked the question
because we'd measured their sleep what
is it about that sleep that seems to
provide this form of it's almost
overnight
therapy how is it doing that what stage
of sleep is doing that and sure enough
what we found was that it was REM sleep
rapid eye movement sleep associated with
dreaming and the greater the amount of
REM sleep the greater the amount of
emotional depotentiation the greater the
amount of sort of emotion detox that you
got the next
day and one of the fascinating things
that we didn't um quite mention in uh
the episode where we described what is
sleep and we described the different
stages including RAM and we spoke about
the brain
changes something utterly unique happens
during REM sleep levels of a brain
chemical called noradrenaline are
completely shut off it is the only time
during the 24-hour period when you see
the complete cessation of neur
adrenaline in the brain and of course
neur adrenaline is associated with many
different functions and you've elegantly
described them one of the functions is
that it's associated with emotional
responsivity and the focus and that sort
of strong um sort of emotional energy
and people will know we speak about it
has two names noradrenaline or
norepinephrine same thing us UK but
people of course are familiar with the
sister chemical in the body called
adrenaline upstairs in the brain we can
think about noradrenaline and during REM
sleep neur adrenaline is completely shut
off this stress Associated neurochemical
it's not only associated with stress but
it's associated with lots of things but
stress included is noradrenaline shut
off in the brain and body during uh
rapid eye movement sleep no it's not it
seems to be specifically within the
brain that there is this blockade of
noradrenaline and serot goes down too
whereas another chemical called atile
choline which is another
neurotransmitter that ramps up in the
brain so if there is a brain chemical
that seems to be underlying REM sleep or
dream sleep it seems to be acetycholine
and in fact in some parts of the brain
you can see almost a 30% greater amount
of acetel choline in some brain regions
than when we're awake yet on the other
hand when we think about noradrenaline
and serotonin they are both shut off so
the stress related chemical within the
brain is Switched Off during REM sleep
however if you look at other parts of
the brain the memory related centers of
the brain such as the hippocampus that
we've spoken about before and the
amydala that I just mentioned too those
are very active during REM sleep so we
laid out this biological model that is
almost beautiful that REM sleep is this
perfect condition for emotional
overnight therapy where you can re
activate and re sort of experience and
reprocess those emotional memories but
you're doing it in a neurochemically
quote unquote safe environment that
allows you to strip away the emotion
from the memory I'd like to take a brief
break and acknowledge our sponsor ag1
ag1 is a vitamin mineral probiotic drink
that also contains adaptogens and is
designed to meet all of your
foundational nutritional needs by now
I'm sure you've all heard me say that
I've been taking ag1 since 2012 and
indeed that is true now of course I do
consume regular Whole Foods every day I
strive to get those Foods mostly from
unprocessed or minimally processed
sources however I do find it hard to get
enough servings of fruits and vegetables
each day so with ag1 I ensure that I get
enough of the vitamins minerals
Prebiotic fiber and other things
typically found in fruits or vegetables
and of course I still make sure to eat
fruits and vegetables and in that way
provide a sort of insurance that I'm
getting enough of what I need in
addition the ad adaptogens and other
micronutrients in ag1 really help buffer
against stress and ensure that the cells
and organs and tissues of my body are
getting the things they need people
often ask me that if they were going to
take Just One supplement what that
supplement should be and I always answer
ag1 if you'd like to try ag1 you can go
to drink a1.com huberman to claim a
special offer you'll get five free
travel packs plus a year supply of
vitamin D3 K2 again that's drink a1.com
huberman in some ways it does resemble
behavioral desensitization therapy
whereby under the care of a qualified
you know psychiatrist or psychologist
somebody will um be encouraged to recall
in a great degree of detail some very
difficult maybe even traumatic event um
and through repetition and of course
through the knowledge that there's
support in the immediate environment
that will allow them to um you know
safely move through that experience you
know should their their heart rate go up
they're sweating profusely having
trouble getting the words out there very
unfortunately common features of trauma
and and negative memories but the idea
as I understand is to repeat the recall
many times often in that safe
environment such that eventually what
was initially a really
terrible event remains a terrible event
but the emotional load of that event is
removed from the person's sort of neural
understanding of the event is the way
I've heard it described is what starts
as a tragic traumatic story eventually
becomes a kind of a sad boring story
boring to the person who's saying it
meaning it doesn't evoke as much
autonomic arousal exactly and in some
ways that's the perfect description of
this overnight therapy process
that it becomes a memory that is no
longer triggering an emotional reaction
and in some ways that's what you want if
you go back to my description from an
evolutionary perspective I told you that
one of the functions of emotions is to
red flag and prioritize the memory at
the time of learning to say that it's
important that's a very adaptive process
it helps us prioritize which things we
really should be focusing on and
remembering but it's not not adaptive
for you to hold on to that emotion long
term once you've started and there has
been some suggestion in the literature
before we were doing this work that
maybe one thing you can do with trauma
and Trauma memories is sleep deprive
individuals the very first night after
the trauma because we knew at the time
sleep is important for memory and what
you would like to do and it's very
similar to that movie Sunshine Spotless
Mind I always forget the Eternal Eternal
Eternal Sunshine of the Spotless Mind
thank you I didn't see the movie but I
hear it's good yeah and what they try to
do is Target in the brain these you know
difficult painful experiences and just
excise them from the brain and that was
the suggestion could you pop those
memories out of the biography of that
individual and save them the trauma I
would argue that's not really what you
want to do because let's say that I am
um I have a trauma experience where I
was walking home at night from The Sleep
laboratory late at night and I was
coming down the kind of an alley to take
a shortcut and someone sticks me up with
a gun maybe some
violence I don't want to remove that
memory I would like to remove the trauma
response associated with that memory but
I would argue for me as an organism it's
still very important for me to remember
that that alley was associated with a
bad experience and I should forego going
down that very same route again I want
to hold on to the memory the information
I want to let go of the emotion I want
to sleep to remember and I want to sleep
to
forget and I'll come on to why I think
that's relevant to PTSD when we perhaps
speak about that condition and it's very
very relevant but coming back to REM
sleep we looked back in the literature
to see if we could find signs that REM
sleep had this relationship with even
just your basic emotional reactivity and
there was some wonderful work by a
gentleman that you will know from
Stanford probably one of the founding
fathers of modern day sleep research a
gentleman called William DeMent yeah who
passed away a few years ago he did um
might have been one of the people who
coined the term rapid eye movement sleep
but I don't think he was the one who
discovered it correct he was not but he
was well up there in terms of
understanding both sort of what its term
was and also what its function
was he Legend as he was very early on
this was probably in the 60s he would
take individuals because we didn't
really have the first published report
of these two types of sleep of REM and
non-rem until they collected the data or
found the data in 1953 it was published
in 1954 so in other words we discovered
that you know even up to then prior to
then we just thought sleep was sleep we
didn't have any knowledge that the these
different stages so in the same year
that um Francis Crick you know un
unveiled this incredible helical
structure that was called a DNA strand
we also discovered the different stages
of sleep but in the 60s then William
DeMent knowing that there were these two
types of sleep and knowing that there
was something that was going on with REM
sleep were people were dreaming and he
would be waking people up from these
different stages and found that it's far
more likely to for people to report a
dream he wondered what the consequence
would be if you selectively deprive
people of this stage of sleep of dream
sleep so he brought individuals into his
laboratory and every time they would go
into REM sleep they would go into the
room they would wake them up have them
do some mathematical problems for 2 or 3
minutes and then put them back asleep
and they go back into nonr and then as
soon as they went back into REM they
would wake them up again and the first
night they would have to go into the
room maybe six or seven times still
brutal for the uh for the person in the
experiment not not too much fun and but
by the end of the 5 days or six days I
think they were going back into the room
something like 17 18 times why because
the people were building up this growing
REM sleep debt and the Brain had such a
hunger for it that by Night five of no
sleep all it wanted to do was rock it
into this thing called REM sleep and
start devouring it with high volume but
that wasn't the interesting part the
interesting part was the consequence to
these subjects they were all well
adjusted perfectly normal individuals by
about day three of selective R sleep
deprivation they started to show signs
of paranoia they started to believe
people were out after them they started
to have hallucinate ations and delusions
and by day
five they were bordering on having you
know aspects of quite severe
psychosis and so what all of this
research has taught us in some ways is
that it's almost as though REM sleep and
again it's
hyperbolic is the difference between
sanity versus Insanity it's the thing
that separates those two and there's a
wonderful quote from an American uh
entrepreneur called e Joseph Cosman and
for all of the years of work that we've
been doing in this field and I I've
spilled so much ink over this including
in the the book he summarized it in a
single
sentence the best bridge between Despair
and hope is a good night of sleep and
that's exactly what the data is
demonstrating in terms of basic
emotional brain
function so a powerful link there and um
I think it's appropriate therefore if we
explore a little bit about what the link
actually consists of um in a way that
will provide people a a kind of a a
compass for when they're feeling a
little bit less emotionally regulated or
if they would like to improve their
levels of emotion regulation uh this is
going to be a little bit of an
exploration but uh you may recall this
is an exploration that you and I had
some years ago when we were talking
about the Rel relationship between rapid
eye movement sleep and
emotionality
and here you've described that the
medial prefrontal cortex normally plays
this kind of
sh role uh this suppressive role over
the amydala under conditions where there
is something to consider is it averse is
it not averse how averse is it right
um but in terms of what we know about
stress and emotion you the autonomic
nervous system this incredible system
that balances sympathetic meaning
alertness arousal sometimes called the
fight ORF flight system and
parasympathetic activation sometimes
called the rest and digest system it's
the balance of the two that dictates
one's emotional state and alertness um
level of stress Etc and I've always
imagined the autonomic nervous system
the sympathetic and parasympathetic
nervous system as sort of a seesaw yeah
but on this seesaw sits us right and we
can move back and forth across this
seesaw but there's an component of the
Seesaw that um in my mental model um
which is the hinge how tight the Seesaw
is meaning how easily or how challenging
it is to tilt the Seesaw to one or the
other side and I don't know if the
mechanism has been discovered but I feel
like what happens under conditions of
REM deprivation or sleep
deprivation that is sleep deprivation
but you've beautifully described how
it's REM deprivation in particular that
can do this that the hinge becomes loose
but the hinge doesn't become loose
toward us becoming more parasympathetic
and relax there's an asymmetry there
it's as if the Seesaw now wants to flop
to sympathetic activation until we're so
exhausted that we just disappear into
sleep so the question is this and maybe
all we have here is is uh opportunity
for speculation but is there any
understanding of what the hinge might be
and how sleep would adjust the tightness
of that hinge and if people are
following this what we're really trying
to get to is you know you described a
neural circuit mechanism within the
brain but is this for instance the
gating of the release of epinephrine
adrenaline and cortisol I mean is that I
could imagine that's regulated by the
brain but when we're deprived of REM
sleep that process becomes less poorly
gated and then we just will punch out a
bunch of adrenaline in response to you
know a phone call from a close friend
that you adore but is their voic is just
a little bit loud and like H this is
rough yeah Etc do we do we understand
the nature of the hinge we do a little
bit and it's something that we started
off trying to test with one specific um
belief and then we were beautifully
course corrected by the data we thought
that the hinge was going to be once you
were sleep deprived and you started to
slide down into that fight or flight
Branch the more sympathetic and away
from the the par sympathetic that the
hinge would get ever tighter the further
into that sympathetic stress related
fight ORF flight dip that you had and
there you would stay it wasn't quite
that simple what we found was that when
I challenge you or put you either under
a very simple cardiovascular challenge
let's say I'm just having you grip a bar
for a long period of time or we have you
under some other maybe even if it's a um
an exercise regiment
when you are in a sleep deprived State
and you are largely inert and not
interacting with the world you actually
are in a more strong parasympathetic
State it's almost as though you do not
want to interact with the world per se
and this comes on to
motivation we and others have found that
one of the earliest and strongest
effects of a lack of sleep is just
absence of motivation I don't want to
interact with the world I don't want to
be social I don't want to learn I don't
want to exert effort I don't want to
exercise I just don't want to do much of
anything however when you provoke me and
you force me to interact or there is a
very strong emotional event that I
experience I go all the way over into
the strongly sympathetic so it's almost
as though we had the prediction that it
was going to be a very tight hinge and
the screw was tightening the more
sympathetic you became it was much more
that you were in this sort of
parasympathetic State this sort of
non-motivational state and the the hinge
was so loose however that even just the
tiniest flick of a challenge whoosh you
went straight over to the sympathetic
there was no sweet spot of a tightening
where you were nicely balancing between
those two states and this comes back to
something else that we found that's you
switching flip-flopping back and forth
between parasympathetic and
sympathetic I spoke about the emotional
reactivity to negative aversive events
but that's only one half of what we call
the affective veilance domain it's not
just that you can have negative
emotional reactions of course you can
have positive emotional
reactions so we did a sister study to
that amigdala study and we asked rather
than showing you increasingly negative
images and how your amigdala would
respond much more strongly to those as
we provoked it we then started to show
you much more positive rewarding images
and because one hypothesis would be that
you just simply slide down the scale and
you move towards more negative and away
from more reward-based
reactivity or you could imagine that
it's both that when you are sleep
deprived you are equally excessively
reactive to both of those domains and
what we found was that it was the latter
that you were very abnormally reactive
overreactive to negative events but you
were equally hyper sensitive to very
reward-based stimuli and this fits
beautifully with what we know from sleep
deprivation you are much more impulsive
you are much more reward seeking you are
much greater in terms of your sensation
seeking and your addiction potential
when you are not getting sufficient
sleep is significantly higher and sure
enough when we looked in the brain many
of these dopamine related circuits that
you've described before were
overactive when you were under slapped
and so I bring this back because it
relates to your seesaw sort of analogy
yes you can think about the Seesaw from
with sleep deprivation from a
sympathetic parasympathetic you can also
think about it from a positive versus
negative veilance and once again our
hypothesis was that you're just going to
slide down into the negative and you're
just going to be less responsive to the
rewarding positive it was the opposite
you were abnormally and excessively
sensitive to both of the those domains
which you could argue is perhaps the
very worst of all adaptive responses
absolutely as an organism you don't want
to be non-reactive emotions are powerful
and we've spoken about the benefits you
need to have emotions to be a
functioning human being or organism in
the world they are designed to
adaptively help us survive but you can't
go to the extremes that's maladaptive
rather than adaptive but that's where
you go when you are sleep to Pride
it's this loose hinge and you become
very very erratically and extremely
reactive from a neural
perspective yeah my understanding is
that um you know sleep deprivation
definitely increases impulsivity and
addictive potential it's um yes so best
worst of Both Worlds in this case and
and given that um now would probably be
an appropriate time to just um cue
people to some of the things that they
can do to improve or maximize their
rapid eye movement sleep this was
covered in detail in episodes one and
two um and to some extent in episodes uh
three and four as well but they're in
reference to um to other things um
learning memory creativity the role of
naps Etc and I'll refer people back to
this um beautiful formula uh q qrt that
it's not just about getting enough sleep
it's about the quantity indeed but also
the quality QQ regularity and timing of
sleep and knowing one's chronotype that
is the best time to go to bed and the
best time to wake up in the morning for
them is going to be critical here and
and I can raise my hand I'll raise both
hands in fact uh metaphorically and say
that when
I've gone to sleep early and woken up
early so for me 8:30 9 p.m. and then
waking up at 4:35 a.m. which for me
matches my chronotype
uh it has served as a powerful
anti-depressant effect and when I've
gotten an equal amount of sleep but
going to bed too late for me that is you
know midnight 1 a.m. and sleeping in
until 8: or 9:00 a.m. um I always carry
a low-level depression um fortunately
not something that needed to be uh
medicated but uh it's it's a striking
effect in the positive direction when
obeying qqr and in in the negative
Direction when not so maybe just for
because we can provide some links to um
those segments in the show note captions
but maybe just um for people that are
here now um if we were going to list out
you know two or three things that one
can do to try and maximize um the
quality and quantity of REM sleep uh
without going on too much of a tangent
but um at the same time we do want to
highlight that addressing that Q qrt
formula um for ourselves is going to be
critical so maybe so for REM sleep um
yeah you know in the domain of exercise
temperature Etc are there any kind of
quick quick bullet points that we can
refer people to I would say just to keep
it high level and and brief the single
best way
cheapest non-pharmacological way that
you can enhance your REM sleep is to
just sleep an extra 15 or 20 minutes
later into the morning don't try to put
if I tell you this is about by the way
this is about the the quantity the your
sleep
opportunity don't try to add that 30
minutes or 20 minutes if your goal is to
increase real sleep at the start of the
night at the front end instead take that
desire that I've offered you of adding
just 20 minutes or 25 minutes of extra
sleep now to the last part of your night
wake up that sort of much later 20 25
minutes later
that's the REM sleep Rich phase so if
people go back and listen to episode one
we'll describe to you exactly how the
different stages of sleep unfold across
the night and they're not evenly
distributed it's not as though you get
just as much REM sleep as well as deep
non-rem sleep in the first half of the
night as you do in the second you get
most of your deep sleep in the first
half and you get most of your REM sleep
in the second half and particularly in
the last quarter of the night and this
leads us to understand that the later
into the morning hours that we go the
greater the hunger preference and The
Taste desire there is of your brain to
start sampling from the finger Buffet of
all of those different stages this thing
called REM sleep and the later that you
sleep into the morning the more of that
REM sleep that you will have and many
people will have experienced this at the
weekend where they have this pattern
that we don't Rec recommend based on the
qqr T um QQ our regularity go to bed at
the same time wake up at the same time
what we see often in society is
something what we call social jetl where
you're short sleeping during the week
and then at the weekend you're out with
friends or you're out sort of on the
town you go to bed late and you wake up
late and maybe you're doing that by 2
hours maybe you're normally in bed by
10: p.m. during the week but now at the
weekends you're going to sleep maybe 12
12:30 and you're waking up 2 3 hours
later on a Saturday and Sunday and then
the problem with that parenthetically is
on Sunday evening you've now got to go
back to work the next day so you have to
push yourself back to 10:30 or 10:00
whereas you were going to bed let's say
at 1 a.m. on Friday and Saturday night
that's a 3-hour Time shift and people
are doing that very frequently that's
the equivalent of you and I flying back
and forth from Los Angeles to New York
every single weekend in terms of oaan
Rhythm and it's brutal on it but this is
separate from this notion of your timing
the final part of the Q qrt and by
pushing your timing a little bit later
into the morning when you wake up you
will experience more REM sleep and as I
said when people sleep later they go to
bed later at night and they wake up much
later in the morning at the weekend I
strongly suspect that if they paid
attention they would say at weekends I
always dream more I always can remember
my dreams and they're more intense it's
not because there's something magical
about how your memory recollection of
Dreams operates on Saturdays and Sundays
it's because you've slept in later
you've gone into that REM sleep Rich
preferential phase in the morning and
therefore you've increased your REM
sleep so I would say that that's
probably the easiest way that you can
start to modulate REM sleep so that's a
terrific do and um I think we can
probably summarize the the top don't as
uh don't drink alcohol because it
abolishes REM sleep alcohol and THC are
both very potent ways that will remove
or obliterate your REM sleep and we
spoke about this in the episode on um on
THC when we discussed this I think just
yesterday I got a very long email and
you know I'm sure you get lots of emails
from um from delightful people in the
public and a Gentleman just saying you
know I was using cannabis for probably
about 7 years and then I watched um or
listen to some of your content and I
stopped and I just had this
explosion of dreams and I was never
recollecting any of my dreams before but
now they came back and goodness were
they Vivid they were Rich they were and
I could not believe it and that's REM
sleep and that's because during the kind
use by way of the THC not the CBD you've
been blocking that REM sleep you've
built up that pressure just as we
described in the dementi these and then
when you finally do take away the agent
that is blocking the generation of REM
sleep the THC all of a sudden your brain
doesn't just go back to having its
standard amount of REM sleep and
dreaming it has that plus it tries to
get back as much of it as it possibly
can by having what we call a REM sleep
rebound and that's why people when they
they stop using they end up having this
intense real sleep by the way to your
point about reward and um addiction
sensitivity with sleep deprivation one
of the things that we um we did in a
collaboration gosh this was years ago
when I was at Harvard with Carl Hart who
I think you I don't know if you colia
yeah Colombia you know him yeah he's
fantastic researcher very interesting
man too um and what we found was that a
lack of sleep was not only predictive of
your addiction potential but when you
went into a clinic to abstain and trying
to come off some of those and here we
were looking at cocaine um addiction a
lack of sleep was a strong predictor of
your abstinence and you falling off the
wagon and going back to you so sleep is
so critical not just for um maintaining
or pushing you away from that addiction
potential but once you are addicted and
you're trying to abstain it gives you
that lift of altitude to try to resist
falling off the wagon and when sleep
gets short that's when you become
vulnerable again probably because your
reward circuitry becomes enhanced and
all of a sudden you just cannot resist
the temptation anymore I want to take a
brief break and acknowledge our sponsor
insid tracker insid tracker is a
personalized nutrition platform that
analyzes data from your blood and DNA a
to help you better understand your body
and help you reach your health goals now
I've long been a believer in getting
regular blood work done for the simple
reason the many of the factors that
impact your immediate and long-term
Health can only be analyzed from a
quality blood test the problem with a
lot of blood tests out there however is
that you get information back about
metabolic factors hormones Etc but you
don't know what to do with that
information with inside tracker they
make it very easy to understand your
results and they also point you to
specific directives that you can follow
in the realm of nutrition exercise
supplementation a even prescription
drugs that can help bring the levels
back into the ranges that are optimal
for you insid tracker also offers insid
Tracker Pro which enables coaches and
health professionals to provide premium
and personalized Services by leveraging
inside trackers analysis and
recommendations with their clients if
you'd like to try insid tracker you can
go to insid tracker.com huberman to get
20% off any of insid tracker's plans
again that's insidetracker
docomo I think the takeaway is very
clear in order to be your best Emotional
Self that is in order to be able to
access positive emotions to their full
amplitude uh motivation um learning as
we also covered in a previous episode
but also to stay out of those um
irritable emotional traps of life um and
to be a regulated person calm and and
joyful person um it stands to reason to
uh minimize alcohol and cannabis use
unless there's some I don't know medical
reason why uh someone should otherwise
but the the real take-home message here
is get as much rapid eye movement sleep
as possible and don't do anything to
inhibit it yeah get as much sleep so
focus on all of those four uh macros of
sleep quantity quality regularity and
timing and notice that if you want to
try to optimize some of those that
emotional reactivity and balance you may
want to slightly over index on your REM
sleep in that regard and one easy cheap
way of doing that if you can lifestyle
permitting an and again of course I
understand everyone is has a life to
live and pressures but that's the way
that if you were to ask me can you do it
and do it simply yes you probably can
great but you touched on trauma a little
bit already but um now would be the
appropriate time I think to talk about
PTSD post-traumatic stress disorder
which um I think we can use the
definition of PTSD and Trauma that the
great Paul kti a former guest on this
podcast who also did series
yeah an incredible man what a what a
what a mind on him and what a generosity
of of sharing information in clear ways
about mental health as he did in the
four episode series on Mental Health
here and he's been on other podcasts as
well wrote A M marvelous book on trauma
Paul defined trauma as some event that
is aversive that changes the way that
our nervous system works such that we
function less well in the future it's
not that every negative event
every uh negative emotion Associated
memory is trauma I think that's a a
misconception um but there are things
that happen to people um or that they
observe happening to other people so
there's you know first person trauma
third person observational trauma Etc
and these can be single events multiple
events you know sadly this stuff happens
um it can be neglect so sometimes it's
the absence of an event right which
becomes the traumatic event that
fundamentally rewire some component of
neur neural circuitry such that we don't
function as well in terms of
relationship to anything work food sex
sleep relationships uh Baseline levels
of emotionality and on and on so what is
the relationship between sleep and
post-traumatic stress disorder
specifically I think some of what we've
covered already certainly touches on
this but PTSD seems to me that it it
might be its own unique case
it is because if you look at the
diagnostic criteria for
PTSD firstly you see sleep disturbance
and as I said right at the top of this
episode there is no major psychiatric
disorder where there isn't some mention
of sleep problems in its diagnostic
criteria but something else was
intriguing about PTSD that compelled me
to think about it and then create a
theory around it it's not just sleep
problems it's also
nightmares and specifically repetitive
nightmares in fact repetitive nightmares
form part of the diagnostic criteria for
you to receive a diagnosis of PTSD
that's how reliable they
are and as I thought more about this
model of overnight therapy this notion
that sleep and particularly REM sleep
provides a form of emotional first
aid PTSD stood out to me as something
think that I had to return to to explain
why because if you think about PTSD and
a veteran it is the perfect example of
the process that I described of
emotional deep potentiation
failing because what I started to
realize is that in PTSD there is this
trauma
experience and then perhaps what's
happening is that sleep the brain goes
back to sleep that night and says okay
please do your elegant trick of
stripping away the emotion from the
memory and it fails so then what happens
the next night the brain comes back and
says I'm sorry but I still got this very
emotionally charged memory please do
your elegant dissipation depotentiation
of the emotion from memory and it fails
again almost like this broken record
that was so indicative of these
repetitive
nightmares and then when you looked at
PTSD I told you that REM sleep is a time
of this remarkable decrease in
noradrenaline but if you look at PTSD
patients they actually have heightened
levels of neur adrenaline and also in
the body adrenaline as well in sleep in
sleep and also when you look just as a
basil State as well so there's something
not quite right with the noradrenaline
story in REM sleep in PTSD patients so I
had just published this paper and I was
up um at a conference in I think it was
Portland and I presented the theory that
or the data that we had on healthy
people and I put forward this theory of
um of PTSD and then later that afternoon
a psychiatrist came on uh the stage
called Murray Raskin and he was working
a lot with PTSD
vets and he describe data which I
couldn't believe it's one of those
moments Andrew where you're at a
scientific
conference and I think it happens maybe
once in a career if you're lucky all of
the hers on the back of my neck stood on
Sharp end because he was saying we've
got this data and we don't quite
understand it we've been treating our
veterans for blood pressure for
hypertension using a generic drug called
prasin and prasin blocked
the um adrenergic response in the body
because you're trying to sort of Tamp
down that sympathetic activation in the
body beta blocker so it's an it's an
alpha
adrenergic antagonist not a beta blocker
so it's not a beta blocker but it's
blocking the adrenergic
system and so and it's a you know it's a
generic pretty cheap drug but it turns
out that it crosses the blood brain
barrier so it doesn't just stay within
the body it goes up into the brain and
he said we don't really understand it
because I've been giving patients this
medication and it works to a degree but
something else happens they come into
the clinic and they say Doc I'm not
having those nightmares anymore they
seem to have gone away and these
patients seem to start to show signs of
resolution so all of a sudden I had had
a model a clinical model that was in
search of data and he had data that was
in search of a theoretical
model I couldn't believe it because it's
exactly what I would predict which is
that if neur adrenaline is too high in
PTSD you're not processing and stripping
the emotion from the memory so it keeps
coming back over and over like this
repetitive nightmare but then if you
block and help bring back down that
level of neur adrenaline to that which
would be seen in a normal healthy person
in other words completely blocking it or
all of a sudden the emotional memory
gets the chance to be processed and you
finally start to get symptom resolution
so we couldn't believe it he flew down
to Berkeley we spent several days
together we went out to dinner we just
could not stop talking he subsequently
did some incredible work in this area
and presin went on to become the um an
FDA approved medication for PTSD and
repetitive nightmares that was approved
by the Veterans Administration Bravo and
so no it's not me it's all all of his
work no you can't no this to me is the
uh scientific collaborative um
conceptual equivalent of the old rees's
um peanut butter cup commercials for
those of us old enough to remember it's
two people running toward one another on
the beach one with a jar of peanut
butter one with a with a piece with a
bar of chocolate and then they crash
into one another and then they both you
know share in the Delight of the the
chocolate peanut butter combination
which is an amazing combination um but
here a far more um important example uh
because it's led to a clinical relief in
in patients with PTSD um so I'm not
making light of that at all but um this
is one of the reasons to go to
Scientific meetings seriously this is
one of the reasons why scientists need
to talk this is one of the reasons to do
podcasts is it is it uh Fosters
hybridization of ideas which um is
Central to to new discoveries um and in
this case a clinical Discovery I I have
a question about um this notion of
blocking norepinephrine um in the brain
and body um you know on the one hand it
seems that during rapid eye movement
sleep we know we're paralyzed um or we
are paralyzed that's a fact of rapid eye
movement sleep the um the brain is
recalling memories often in in great
detail sometimes through symbolic
representation SpaceTime is disrupted
it's either faster or slower so because
you're dreaming um and
it seems that there's something powerful
about being able to replay the
memories and yet divorce them from
certain
neurochemical uh release in the brain
and body to essentially uncouple them
and then to me it makes perfect sense
why taking a a drug that would reduce
the amount of sympathetic arousal in
sleep would help especially PTSD because
you said with
PTSD sort of an invasion of the
noradrenaline response into rapid ey
Moon sleep that is inappropriate so does
that mean that rapid eye movement sleep
in people with PTSD is not truly rapid
eye movement sleep it's as if it's been
abolished and replaced with something
that's kind of pseudo waking stress
invaded you know it's like a zombie REM
um and it's not good is that is that is
that correct it seems to be an and you
can look at this in terms of the
electrical activity of REM sleep the
electrical brain waves of R sleep in
these patients and you're right it
doesn't seem to be of the same
electrical quality but what was
interesting in Marie Raskin studies when
he started to treat patients with the
pricin and Tamp down that
noradrenaline one of the other things
that returned back to normality was not
just that the symptoms dissipated their
REM sleep started to return with a
greater amount and so I think it fits
very well with this notion that whatever
REM sleep that was going on may not
necessarily have been electrical or
neurochemically identical to normative
REM sleep but when you assisted the
system with a chemical to bring it back
into normality REM sleep was gifted back
to the brain and emotional resolution
started to unfold now I should note that
there have been um a number of studies
that have replicated the finding some
Studies have not though and so we still
need to understand exactly why this is
the case and there are other therapies
that we'll probably discuss in a later
episode on dreaming that are as if not
more effective than that drug therapy
for repetitive nightmares that is a
psychological intervention rather than a
pharmacological intervention and that
seems to be very effective too in 2017
as my laboratory was starting to explore
some studies on humans on fear and
Trauma I visited a a trauma Addiction
Center on the East Coast um the guy who
runs it will be a guest on this podcast
in the future
an amazingly talented uh trauma and
addiction therapist by the name of Ryan
Suave and there it
was that I learned about Yoga Nidra
non-sleep deep rest and here's why they
were taking heroin addicts gambling
addicts sex addicts alcoholics people
with what they're call behavioral
process addictions and substance abuse
addictions um and every morning after
they woke up the first thing that they
would do was 1 hour of non-sleep deep
bre you know placing people into this
linal state and I asked why and um and
Ryan said this is especially important
to do with um addicts when they arrive
in inpatient recovery in the first week
and um even more so in the first three
days because typically they are badly
sleep
deprived and in addition to that many of
them are just not good at getting and
staying asleep at night without the use
of pharmacology or in some cases their
behavioral addictions depending on what
it what it was
and so it was a a kind of a
self-directed relaxation training of
sorts uh first thing in the morning that
in addition um perhaps could compensate
partially for some of the sleep
deprivation that they no doubt were
experiencing when they arrived it's also
novel environment and sleeping novel
environments can be challenging so uh
there is as far as I know no uh
randomized control Trials of of this
practice uh yet but there are a good
number of um clinics and treatment
centers that are now employing non-sleep
deep breast AKA Yoga Nidra um for 30
minutes to an hour first thing upon
waking um as one of the you know core
components of treatment for helping
people get and stay sober I think it's
absolutely fascinating because that
morning time period as well can be for
those who are struggling with sleep
especially difficult and you and I have
spoken on this podcast series about sort
of Awakening at night or later in the
morning when you really want to be
asleep and it's just a struggle to get
back and as you noted there he was
saying they often come in underslept and
my suspicion is that they're probably
getting sleep at the front end in part
because they're heavily medicated but
self-medicating in terms of helping
their sleep but then of course because
they are asleep they can't continue to
medicate so which part of sleep is
fragile it's those morning hours and
therefore if you have something that is
a compensatory tool that is not going to
be the trigger of saying just get back
into bed get under those sheets and
sleep you know sleep doesn't work like
that sleep is not something that we do
sleep is something that arrives to us
with us and if it's not you can't force
it and it's not it's a little bit like
someone's name in fact quite the
opposite that the harder you try to
remember the further you push sleep away
and when you stop it all of a sudden
comes back but I love this idea of
inserting something like that as a
compensatory tool and that's why I think
you and I have discussed um openly here
in fact on this series at some point
we're going to collaborate and we're
going to look to see exactly what is
happening electrically at High Fidelity
mapping inside of the brain when we are
going through these linal states and
what is the benefit of that is it a very
similar benefit for sleep and it's
fascinating because it's Poss possible
that what we find at the level of the
brain is that it's not sleep like it's
something else like maybe it's just a
Lial State like and what's also
interesting is that it provides
seemingly many of the benefits of sleep
but it's not sleep in other words you
can arrive at the same destination of
mental and physical health through two
different routs one thing called Sleep
one thing called these lional states or
they both op operate on the same Highway
in terms of mechanistic transaction
benefits there's so much that we
need we could stay here all all night
and all day hopefully not all night well
well we will absolutely do those studies
and um because I think that people are
in desperate need of zero cost tools to
try and um access the the the
replenishment and Recovery that comes
from sleep and when sleep is available
to us when we can access it that's going
to be the best option there's no
question but then some of these tools um
in theory and in practice provide a a
portal to get better at sleeping as well
so I was going to say one of the other
things I I'd be fascinated for us to do
is not just look at that model of what
happens in the morning but can we use
that for people who have the opposite
insomnia problem which is that I can't
fall asleep and we spoke about this in a
previous episode of tools and techniques
and methods to help you fall asleep
could this be one of them where you just
start to help move yourself into this
Lial State you take the stress off one
of the things I hear so much at the
center when people come in and they say
I've just I always struggle to sleep and
you go into depth and it's because they
their mind starts to roll it X through
that anxiety of what I need to do and
what I should do but also then the the
later it gets and the the absent the
Sleep becomes the more stress they get
not just about the next day The more
stress that they get about this thing
called not being able to fall asleep and
if there's something a practice that
you've taught someone that says that's
okay I know this place and I know this
situation and there's a tool I have and
it's
called this ainal state and if you were
to train people on that sort of that
method is it a way that they finally can
then cast themselves
off and it's the it's the bridge not
necessarily just between Despair and
hope but the bridge between wakefulness
and sleep so put it at the back end at
the end of the day rather than the front
end a lot for us to discover there and
um you know at risk of of uh being
hyperbolic I mean what what would be
more useful than a zeroc cost
non-pharmacologic tool for people to get
um the rest and restoration they need
and to get better at getting the
ultimate form of rest and restoration
which is sleep yeah and it you know I
love the Paradox of it that non-sleep
deep rest allows you to go into sleep
deep rest right it is after all a
transition or Lial State maybe this will
become the the stage before stage one of
sleep who knows who knows we Define off
stage in criteria that's right okay so
speaking of challenges sleeping because
of one's concern AKA anxiety about the
importance of sleep what about the
relationship between sleep and anxiety
meaning many people in the world
experience low-level anxiety or have a
low threshold to what could be a
full-blown anxiety or panic attack but
more often than not is this feeling of
being tired and wired or um having a a
quick uh you know prepulse startle as we
call it in our business a nerd speak for
um you know kind of a reactive um to
input anxiety and I don't think there's
any clean definition between anxiety
stress um and PTSD these run along a
Continuum and they PSD is an anxiety
disorder it's one of many right they
these things braid together in a way
that it would be a waste of our time to
try and disentangle those um but many
people have anxiety that is anywhere
from minor to debilitating um but that
is separate from PTSD although people
with PTSD can have
anxiety so what do we know about the
relationship between
sleep and anxiety and perhaps we could
frame this in the context of the
qqr you know um I'll just toss out a
question that perhaps highlights what I
mean is it possible that somebody's
getting eight hours of sleep a night
which for them meets their quantity
requirement in the um the quality
is relatively high but it's not as high
as it could be because the regularity
and timing of their sleep isn't great is
that person going to be more prone to
anxiety than somebody who's really
matched to their chronotype and is still
getting enough sleep no one's done the
head-to-head comparison where you kind
of do the how I going do the Coke Pepsi
Dr Pepper Sprite QQ RT challenge between
all of those what we do know is that if
you look at each one independently qqr T
quantity quality regularity timing if
any one of those is off it's very
difficult not to see a coexisting anxiet
disorder or increase in anxiety or a
mood disorder and I think to me anxiety
is part of that class of a broader class
that I would call mood
disorders it's relevant that we make
that distinction at least in my eyes and
I know some people may disagree because
mood and anxiety are different than
emotions and many of us Clump them
together the way I think about the
difference is the following time scale
emot ions are short punctate events that
usually last anywhere from seconds to
too many minutes mood States however
like anxiety or depression those operate
on a slightly different time scale from
minutes to hours to months to years and
so it's very unlikely that we can
experience an emotional reaction that
from a sort of a chronometry point of
view lack lasts for 2 years but you can
certainly see someone who has a mood
State abnormality of depression that
lasts for several years or who has been
chronically anxious for several years
and I'll come back to why I think that
distinction is is relevant for a second
to your point though about the
relationship with sleep here again it's
a very strong bidirectional relationship
and I would say that probably in the
last eight or nine years we've been
doing a considerable amount of work can
sleep in anxiety rather than just sleep
in basic emotional
reactivity what we found is it's very
strongly
bidirectional that if you have anxiety
it's very difficult to sleep and if you
are having difficulty sleeping it's very
likely that you will increase your
anxiety but before we really unpacked
that we started with a a very basic
study similar to those we've described
we took a group of people and we were
very careful to make sure that they had
um completely normative levels of
anxiety they showed no signs of an
anxiety related disorder and by the way
anxiety disorders are it seems one of if
not the most common of all psychiatric
conditions just to put it in context for
people listening and these individuals
no signs of anxiety disorders whatsoever
they were normative and then we had them
go through a full night of sleep or we
then sleep deprived them and the next
day we were measuring their anxiety and
in those people who were sleep deprived
we were actually measuring the level of
anxiety every hour so we could almost
get this timelapse photography of what
happened to the anxiety state as it
unfolded across the sleep deprivation
period it wasn't a linear response that
the more and more hours that you were
awake Beyond
16 the more exponential
that rise in anxiety became so it wasn't
simply a linear dose response curve it
was an exponential meaning that there
was this hockey shaped swing up and in
fact by the next morning compared to
when you'd had a full night of sleep
those individuals were so anxious that
almost 50% of the participants in that
group who had no signs of anxiety before
had a level of anxiety that was so
strong that they would reach the
diagnostic threshold for having an
anxiety disorder and that was simply by
way of the absence of
sleep but again that brought me back to
this notion of this is a good
experimental tool for us scientists to
understand what is the benefit of sleep
when it's present and the absence of
sleep when it's not by taking sleep
completely out of the equation by way of
total deprivation but of course that's
not real life so we did a slightly
different study
here what we did was we tracked
individuals essentially in the wild as
it were just going about their daily
lives and we had different sleep
tracking monitor uh monitoring equipment
on them so we were tracking their sleep
from one night to the next to the next
to the next and from one day to the next
to the next we were tracking their level
of anxiety and what we found here was
that even small perturbations in their
sleep from one night to the next to the
next
accurately predicted the increase or
decrease in their anxiety from one day
to the next to the next what was the
critical ingredient here well in the
first experiment I'd essentially
manipulated both quantity and quality
the two qqs of the qqr had removed the
quantity of sleep and also they had no
quality of sleep why because they had no
quantity of sleep but when we looked at
that day to day to day night to night
toight study it wasn't quantity that was
the best predictor it wasn't shortening
of quantity that determined next day
increases in anxiety it was quality the
worse the quality was night to night to
night the worse their anxiety
became so that started to lead us to
think a lot more about what is it
regarding the quality of sleep that
seemed to offer when it was present what
I would describe as an angio itic
benefit in other words it's lessening
anxiety a lack of sleep is an anxiogenic
it's going to produce anxiety what in
sleep is
anxiolytic we started off with a
hypothesis that was profoundly incorrect
we thought well for emotions which are
these short bursts of um of affective
state it was REM sleep that seemed to be
the Principal ingredient well wouldn't
that be the case for mood States well
here with anxi it wasn't it was deep
non-rm sleep and we couldn't get away
from it and so what we found was that
when we looked at the sleep in the
laboratory and asked what was predictive
from the night before so you measure
your anxiety the night before and then
we measure it the next morning and
basically we calculate a change score
has your anxiety the next morning
increased stayed the same or decreased
and then we correlate that with the
different stages and what we found was
that the electrical quality of your deep
nonrem sleep was very much
predictive of your dissipation of
anxiety
overnight and this helped me realize
gosh it's much more complex these are
beautiful surprises you get from
research when you you have like you have
this hypothesis and you look at you see
REM sleep no signal of predictive
relationship with anxiety and I say of
course because I'm idiotic rerun the
analysis just go back to Raw data and
you know the r sleep signal was so
strong rerun the analysis and you get
exactly the same result it's deep non-rm
sleep great okay then what is that deep
non-rm sleep doing to help dissipate the
anxiety but here again was a commonality
with emotion what we found is that the
greater the amount of Deep non-rm Sleep
the greater the re-engagement of your
frontal lobe was the next day and that
was predicting the dissipation of your
anxiety the next morning so we really
started to understand this sort of
critical bidirectional relationship but
it was a very complex one that yes
anxiety can disrupt your sleep and yes
disrupted sleep can predict your next
day anxiety but it wasn't the same stage
of sleep that we thought before it was
the the opposite it was deep non-rm
sleep what we've come to realize is that
deep nonr sleep in part seems to be
almost shifting you from that
sympathetic State over to the
parasympathetic state it seems to engage
that nice rest and digest it seems to
reduce your heart rate it seems to drop
levels of cortisol and we think that
perhaps is a resetting brain body
literally an embodied mechanism by way
of Deep non-r Sleep helping you just
relieve that anxiety pressure so it does
come back to your question which is yes
quantity if I manipulated quality if I
manipulate regularity or timing
manipulate any one of those I can change
your anxiety but the story coming
through here if anything was that it
wasn't quantity it was quality I told
you that from one night to the next to
the next the quality of your sleep that
we were measuring was predictive of your
anxiety and then when we bring you into
the laboratory and we look at the
electrical activity of your brain I also
mentioned in that episode on the first
episode another way we measure quality
is not just subjectively what's going on
or objectively what is the efficiency of
your sleep is it filled with lots of
Awakenings which was the measure that we
used in the day-to-day study and night
to night study but we looked at the
electrical quality of your sleep once
again it was quality that was predicting
it it's something about getting good
continuous sleep that is replete with
this deep non-rem electrical brain
activity that provides an angiolytic
benefit to your brain the next day and I
think it's strategic CU so many of us
deal with
anxiety and some of us would prefer not
to necessarily be on medication or even
look to that well here again is a
strategic tool think about your sleep it
really does seem to be a buffer for
anxiety along those lines maybe you can
just recap a few of the things covered
in previous episodes that are known to
improve the quality and quantity of Deep
nonm Sleep
um I can think of a couple but I'm
you're the expert here um not rush to
judgment on that one well I think it's
an established fact uh which is why
you're here um so let's
um perhaps list a few of those off in
the domains of you know exercise
temperature Etc what what what would you
place in that you know is there top
three like three greatest hits for uh
for improving deep non-rem sleep for uh
because of its important relationship to
anxiety management or reducing anxiety I
think there are the first thing I would
tell you is that regularity is going to
be key here when you are giving your
brain the signals of regularity it
understands exactly how to instigate
that that deep sleep and that's one of
the two qualitative measures of sleep
that I spoke about so QQ the quality the
second Q I spoke about it's regarding
the continuity of your sleep and the
electrical quality of your sleep
regularity is probably best for the
continuity of your sleep if you're very
irregular with the timing of your sleep
your brain almost doesn't know you know
are we on are We off are we on and we
off and your sleep can become quite
fragmented because it's confused based
on regularity when you give it
regularity sleep starts to become more
stable more stable means that it's less
likely to be littered with Awakenings
meaning that it's better quality of
sleep in terms of electrical quality of
sleep we did mention this um in a prior
episode when we spoke about sort of food
and exercise exercise seems to be one of
those things that's very good at
improving the quality of your deep sleep
and here I'm talking about the
electrical quality of your deep sleep
try to make sure that you're physically
active to a degree and and I think this
is a protocol and I think it's a
meaningful protocol but to go so to the
extreme where I would to say you need to
do at least 32 and a half minutes on a
spin bike at this wattage or you know we
can't prescribe quite at that point you
know scientific prescription not MediCal
and so I would say exercise is one then
we spoke about another which was
temperature and we said that getting
your bedroom cool seems to be a way to
promote the increase in deep sleep so
these are two dos which is get regular
get cool the don'ts we've already spoken
a little bit about too one of the things
that I probably didn't mention enough
with alcohol not only does it seem to
compromise your rapid eye movement sleep
but it will fragment your sleep it will
make your sleep more unstable and an
indirect consequence of that is alcohol
is going to be in highest concentrations
in your system after drinking in the
evening with sleep in the first four to
5 hours now that depends on how quickly
you metabolize it and how much you've
had but let's assume some degree of
standardization in other words I said
that alcohol will not just block your M
sleep it will fragment your sleep makes
your sleep more vulnerable to you waking
up well you're especially vulnerable in
the first four or so hours because
that's when alcohol concentration is
highest in your system and therefore the
first 4 hours can also fall prey to the
greatest culling of your sleep quality
and if you're removing or restricting
some of that quality in the first four
hours what type of sleep are you
principally restricting you're
restricting deep sleep because we've
said deep sleep comes in the first half
dream sleep REM sleep in the second half
so don'ts would be try to stay away from
you know excessive alcohol in the
evening we also know that Al alcohol is
associated
with longer term chronic anxiety and the
tragedy is that it's often used as a way
to blunt the anxiety because alcohol is
a sedative and it can help just
alleviate take you know take the edge
off but it's a short-term quote unquote
win for a long-term loss because overall
it will increase anxiety levels so I
would say those are some dos and perhaps
a don't if you want to try to optimize
your sleep quality quality including the
Integrity of your sleep and also the
electrical quality of your sleep
terrific I think um because so many
people struggle with anxiety ranging
from mild to severe anxiety the tips you
just provided are going to be immensely
beneficial and in addition to that the
previous four episodes that we've
recorded for this series each and all
include tools that is protocols for
improving the Q qrt aspects of sleep so
um all the more reason for people to uh
dig into those and and to glean the the
gems that you've laid out for people
because they're they really are very
actionable and um you know most all
perhaps even all of the tools that we've
discussed in those episodes are zero
cost they require a little bit of time
investment some um thought and
consideration but they're not really
that difficult to implement they just
require a little bit of um being one's
own scientist of self and being your own
own Corner when it comes to sleep and
another lowcost method that we um
mentioned was not just temperature in
terms of keeping your room cool but warm
bath or shower before bed I mentioned
improved sleep but one of the things
that improves most is deep non-r sleep
so there's another technique get your
room cool to go into but warm up to cool
down to fall asleep which then keeps you
cool so that you stay asleep and you'll
get more deep sleep very
fantastic although it's a terribly um uh
unhappy
topic suicide is a is an important topic
for us to to cover here um you know I
can think of few things more tragic than
suicide um and yet sadly it U it
accompanies certain uh psychiatric
conditions I think um people with manic
bipolar have a 20 to 30 times greater um
probability of suicide than others but
you know suicide accompanies major
depression anxiety PTSD you know again
it's it's um a tough topic to to get
into um but an important one to get into
what is the relationship between
suicidality and sleep um and I suppose
we could look at this from the
perspective of to what degree does sleep
deprivation um correlate with suicide or
attempted suicides and what sort of inoc
ulatory effects does um sleep provide
towards
suicide unfortunately we don't know much
about the second part of the question
which is how can sleep be used as a risk
mitigating tool when you know that there
is the risk of suicide in place um
there's been a number of people who are
doing this work including my colleague
Allison Harvey again at uh the
University of uh of California Berkeley
and Sher Johnson who's also there too I
would say though that the first question
is is quite answerable which is what do
we know firstly about how a lack of
sleep can impact suicide some of the
earliest data that we found were
associational relationships what we
found is that short sleep or poor
quality of sleep predicted three things
it
predicted suicidal ideation meaning that
you had thoughts of
suicide bad sleep seemed to predict
suicide attempts and then tragically
more recent data a lack of sleep
predicts suicide
completion and what makes me think more
causely about it and we've been trying
to get some grants and we've failed to
do so so far to do more of this work
because I'm I'm just so compelled by it
and you're right it's one of the most
tragic
situations those sleep relationships
aren't simply happening at the same
moment in time what I mean is that the
sleep disturbance that we see
precedes the onset of having suicidal
thoughts it precedes the onset of
suicide attempt and it precedes the
suicide completion so what this has been
teaching me is I've been looking at the
data and we've looked at a little bit of
our own data sleep disruption when it
comes to suicide is almost the canary in
the coal mine it's almost like a tragic
crystal ball that when you see that
sleep start starting to
dismantle it is a foreshadowing sign of
a very dark series of events that will
unfold in other words could we now start
to think and this is one of the things
that we want to do is sleep a biomarker
is sleep disruption I should say a
biomarker for upcoming suicide risk
before it begins the idea of finding a
biomarker or collection of biomarkers
for SU side I think is one of the more
important missions of Neuroscience Ai
and mental health uh generally there's a
brilliant young researcher up at the
University of Washington named Sam
golden he's um spent a lot of his career
studying animal models of um aggression
um and rage um and of course some forms
of suicide are thought to be forms of
self-directed aggression and rage it
makes sense um some forms of suicide
perhaps are different I don't think we
quite understand what suicide represents
in the brain just yet and I think
they're having spoken to Paul kti and
and others about suicide um it's clear
that there are unfortunately many paths
to Suicide there's in one brain State
nonetheless um Sam's laboratory has been
developing tools that help people with
Suicidal Tendencies or people who have
had suicidal ideation or plans in the
past um with these AI based tools where
it detects changes in their
voice in their sleep patterns and in a
few other metrics that I don't recall
that together become very good
predictors of um later suicidal ideation
so the idea here is that um people who
are prone to Suicide um often don't
realize that they're drifting that way
until it can sadly be too late so um
the point here is uh a biomarkers are
key B these biomarkers are being
developed uh C AI is critical but that
according to Sam um changes in sleep
patterns is absolutely Central to these
algorithms for allowing people to detect
their own um potential for suicide I
think it's it's critical and we've
thought about this when we started to
see these sleep signals that were
preemptive that were almost precognitive
in the sense of of prediction
occurred to me that we're at the stage
of
technological
Evolution that if we get consent to many
individuals who become
suicidal are they are interested in some
degree of
support and we often but of course some
people will just recoil and go into
themselves and that's that's when things
can get very problematic as well but it
would require some degree of consenting
that if you have a history of suicide
adiation in the past what if you were to
be able to consent and say I would like
to risk mitigate and you have a wearable
like a watch and that watch is connected
to your phone and there is a signal that
can come from your watch that dials a
series of phone numbers in order of
preference and when your watch starts to
detect that your sleep has this one the
things we really want to understand is
what is the specific signature of sleep
abnormalities it's not just that your
sleep gets short but is it that your
sleep gets long and then short and then
long and then short but it constantly
has poor quality of sleep and the
regularity is all over the place but the
chronotype timing is still in place what
sort of specific pattern of those things
is the Hallmark that is most predictive
of suicide let's say that I can come up
with that algorithm finally and then we
can implement it into a watch or a
tracking device of some sort and when it
starts to see that pattern it's
constantly pattern matching and it
starts to see that across whatever
number of days we say if you see this
across six nights or across 13 nights
this is serious it then triggers that
phone to send a message to those
individuals who the designated support
carers and those people then reach out
and start to say how are you doing would
you like to have a phone call can I come
over can I make you some food and I'd
love to have a chat with you can you
find a way to bootstrap a condition
where you constantly then otherwise
become asocial or antisocial and lose
all support network so that would be the
the sort of the grandiose idea the other
thing that's very interesting is that we
could measure their activity and their
wakefulness at night and the reason I
bring this up is some great work by
Michael peus and U Michael grandner
who've looked at
suicide um both attempts and suicide
completion across the 24-hour period
it's not constant it's not that you see
suicide ideation and suicide attempts
and completion in a distributed manner
equally across the 24-hour period when
do they principally occur they occur in
the late middle of the night and there's
this almost a four to five hour period
you know somewhere on average and again
it's just an average somewhere between
let's say 1 a.m. and 4 a.m. which it
turns out to be right at the lowest dip
of your Cadian Rhythm and it could be
Cadian Rhythm but I also think that
there's something about of course the
night ESS when no one else is around
around and it is just you bad point
number one second as we've spoken about
before on this episode negative thoughts
are 10 times worse in the darkness of
night than they are in the light of
day and third at that point if you're
awake you're not asleep and we know
sleep is providing this balast to your
mental health so on all three of those
counts you see this very strong spike in
suicide ideation suicide attempt and
also suicide completion in this
bewitching hour in the middle of the
night there's a final piece in the
suicide story though that is only just
emerging if you are not getting
sufficient sleep you are somewhere
between two to three times more likely
to go into that suicidal state which is
a very significant number however when
people started to measure another factor
of sleep and particularly dream sleep
which was the dream content itself it
became even more predictive and we've
not really seen this very much in
psychiatric conditions but what they
found was that instead of using your
sleep disruption or your lack of sleep
as a predictor of your suicide risk we
use nightmares as a predictor of your
suicide risk that predictive value that
risk went from about two to three times
more likely to somewhere between 5 to8
times more likely there is something
special going on with bad dreams and
specifically nightmares that is even
more predictive than this physiological
thing that we call Sleep itself and
we'll probably come on to maybe some of
the reasons why dreaming and
particularly nightmares in the next
episode on dreaming could explain
exactly why that is but it's a new
finding I don't think we can say much
more about it now but it is one of the
most I think novel findings in the
psychiatric sleep story that now dreams
have come above and beyond simply sleep
itself as a predictor of mental illness
and specifically a form that will take
your life tragically very
quickly when I think about depression
I immediately associate that with
excessive amounts of
sleep after all it's called depression
but what is the real link between major
depression which is you know the
classical um you know signs of you know
mala's um one of the Hallmark features
also being a uh a lack of optimism about
the future or Andor ability to sense
into the future that's what it's not the
only criteria um when Dr Carl daero the
great neuroscientist that he is was on
this podcast and of course he's also a
practicing clinical psychiatrist I when
we were talking about depression he
mentioned that another Hallmark of major
depression is people waking up at 2: or
3:00 a.m. and not being able to fall
back asleep um this just seems like a
recipe for disaster all around um that
the very condition that you're trying to
perhaps amarate with additional sleep is
preventing you from sleeping it's like I
couldn't imagine very cruel is a whole a
whole lot of things more diabolical um
in terms of the sleep sleep science so
what's the relationship between sleep
and depression and how how should one um
untangle that like seemingly gordi and
not it
is like the other conditions
bidirectional that depression can
disrupt sleep very much and disrupted
sleep can trigger depression depression
is interesting by the way some people
have conceptualized it as being
different to anxiety based in some ways
on memory which is that when you think
about anxiety people consider anxiety a
disorder of the future that you are
constantly worried about what's coming
up in the future I I didn't do this
today so I need to do that tomorrow and
then I've got that other thing next week
or I'm fearful of going out to see them
tomorrow I just I'm fearful of taking
that flight tomorrow it seems to be so
much about prospective future whereas
other people have suggested depression
is the opposite it's about rumination of
the past I went through this event I had
this bereavement I had this painful
divorce I just can't get over my past
now I don't necessarily know if that's
entirely true but it it is interesting
in the sense that both of those abnormal
prospection worry of the future and
abnormal
retrospection sort of ruminating on the
past seem to disrupt sleep before um you
continue I just wanted to drill into
that that idea just a little bit because
I think it's a really interesting one
worth exploring again I'm no
psychiatrist but I have heard and I've
experienced the I've had a depression I
think it's my understanding is it's
normal for um people to experience a
major depressive episode at some point
in their lives could be situationally
triggered or not but that um for others
Unfortunately they have repeating major
depressive episodes and hopefully some
people go through life never having had
a depressive episode but as I recall
that one of the more Salient um thought
patterns was that I used to have
something that somehow was lost and I
couldn't quite figure out what it was it
was this curring feeling of right like
things were on track and then they got
off track but not being able to to tack
the uh progression from on track to off
track to one particular event it was
this sort of sense that like I had
something that them was lost now
fortunately for me it eventually lifted
uh and you know it didn't get um
dangerously bad um but I I've had some
close friends who've gone through
individual or several major depressions
and I I hear this like this idea that
they had they had it or they think
something was there that then they lost
so I think I I agree with well if you
think about the word that you just used
had is about right it's it's about it's
the past right right it's past tense
right and then if if we apply the
criteria that is is indeed part of the
criteria for determining if somebody has
major depression which is a lack of
optimistic outlook on the future one can
see how one could be very much stuck in
the present and focused on the past and
you know just stuck in that spin cycle
um anyway we're not here to um to uh
decide what depression is or isn't in
every case but I I'm I'm not a
psychiatrist either right but but I
think this distinction between anxiety
being about the future in a way that
disrupts one's present and depression
being about often the past in a way that
disrupts one's sense of the present and
the future um makes a lot of sense it's
just a a nice um not nice it's
unfortunate but it's a um it could be a
useful a useful contextualization yeah
thank you and I to your question though
about sleep it's it's been a little bit
interesting with depression firstly what
we know is that depression will disrupt
your sleep and make your sleep shorter
and it comes back to um your comment
from uh from Carl from Carl di we often
see that patients will have problems
staying asleep they wake up in the
middle of the night they can't get back
to sleep it's
problematic and therefore their sleep
duration and their Sleep Quality
decrease however on the other hand there
is an interesting question by the way of
can you get too much sleep which I
should probably come back as there's a
whole episode to do on that probably but
one of the places where we see quote
unquote too much sleep is in the
depression literature and it's a
condition that we call hypersomnia in
other words increased or excessive
degrees of sleep hypersomnia here but a
a great PhD student at Berkeley looked
at the data um Kate Kaplan who's a
fantastic um cognitive behavioral
therapist now and a clinical
psychologist looked a little bit at the
data and others have looked at this
too when you examine what people were
asking those patients where there is
this conclusion that patients with
depression can sleep too long really
what they were asking in those studies
was what time you go to bed and what
time do you wake up and there what you
clearly find is that people with
depression will be in bed for
significantly longer periods of time and
the inference there and you could argue
almost the conflation is that if you're
in bed for longer then you're sleeping
for longer and therefore depression is a
condition of
hypersomnia but when people looked at
this a little bit more in a nuanced way
and asked a different question what time
did you go to sleep and what time did
you wake up that hypersomnia phenomenon
is nowhere near as strong as you would
have been led to believe otherwise from
the what time did you go to bed and what
time did you wake up and I think part of
the reason comes back to depression as a
condition when you think about
depression one of the aspects one of the
features is that you're depressed to the
point where you just don't want to
interact with the world and what better
place to spend if that's your mentality
than this thing called bed I just don't
want to get out of bed I'm just going to
stay here and lie in bed I'm awake I'm
not asleep and so we don't quite know
yet if depression is a condition that is
associated with long sleep we certainly
Know It's associated with short sleep
and disrupted sleep or that is
masquerading as this thing called
hypersomnia but when you really look at
the data it's not quite so clear
that was the first peculiarity in
depression that there could be this
Paradox of yes long sleep but also not
enough sleep too short sleep one of the
earliest findings in depression and
sleep and has been quite well replicated
is a change in REM sleep but now it
wasn't necessarily that individuals who
had depression slept or had excessive
amounts of REM sleep they had a little
bit more what was interesting is that
when that REM sleep emerged during the
night was much earlier and in the first
episode I was telling you that when you
your head hits the pillow you go down to
the light stages of nonr then into the
deeper stages and then maybe after about
50 60 70 80 minutes you'll pop up and
you'll have your short REM sleep period
but that first REM sleep period in
people with depression seem to have been
called up by the brain abnormally or not
much earlier so it's what we call REM
sleep latency from the moment that you
fell asleep what is the time what is the
latency of the first arrival of REM
sleep and that REM sleep latency was
significantly shorter in those people
with depression Ram sleep was arriving
earlier now it's hard because you can
argue and this is these are the most
dangerous hypotheses you can argue both
sides of it you can say well perhaps
that's because Matt you also spoke to me
that REM sleep may be important for some
aspects of the emotional brain and when
you are depressed the brain knows that
REM sleep is required and it calls it up
on the menu of the series of dishes that
you're going to be served earlier on in
the night because it's needed more
significantly the other and that's the
adaptive theory the other is the
maladaptive Theory which is that
arriving with your REM sleep too early
does not do your brain good things and
therefore it's some abnormality of
emotional
processing the data that's interesting
there is that if you look at some
anti-depressants many of them will
either delay the onset of REM sleep or
they will reduce it significantly now
there's a huge debate about the efficacy
and the utility of anti-depressants and
I I don't have a horse in that race and
I don't know know enough about that
literature to comment I would simply say
though that it's at least intriguing to
me that some
medications that are commonly prescribed
as anti-depressants will alter
specifically rem's sleep and push it
later or try to reduce it down and that
would fit with the maladaptive
hypothesis that this arrival of REM
sleep so early in depression and perhaps
having a little too much REM sleep isn't
optimal and when you push back against
that with pharmacology I.E
anti-depressants you seem to get some
degree of resolution or reduction in the
depression
symptomatology again I don't think we've
we clearly understand
that the another strange thing that is
has been often cited to me many times
about sleep and depression is a
literature that suggests that if you
deprive people of sleep which time and
again in this episode we've said leads
to bad outcomes for mental health it
does exactly the opposite in depression
that if you sleep deprive a depressed
patient you get a resolution of the
depression and that is the claim that's
often made to me now it is a very clear
set of data in the literature but there
are two potential concerns with it the
first concern is that not all patients
respond to sleep deprivation in fact if
you look at the data it's somewhere
between 30 to 55% of patients will be
responders to sleep depravation the
other proportion of those patients don't
respond or if anything get worse when
you sleep deprive them and then the
question is well how would you know and
right now and there have been some brain
Imaging studies some pet studies done
way back at UC irine U and other
locations where they were trying to say
is there something about the metabolic
activity of your brain that can predict
if you're a responder or not sleep
deprivation because at least then we
would know who should we push through
this quote unquote treatment and who
should we not because it's going to be
bad for them that's the first issue and
we don't have a clear understanding the
second issue is that as soon as those
patients with depression sleep after the
deprivation the anti-depressant benefit
goes away and they go right back to
being depressed again so yes it's a
mechanistic interesting process what is
it about sleep deprivation that could
alleviate depression and I'll explain
why I think it
can but it's not a sustainable one it's
not a clinically viable
one why would it have that effect if it
does well you and I discussed earlier in
this episode that when you are sleep
deprived not only does your emotional
brain become much more responsive to
negative things also becomes much more
responsive to rewarding
positive things and one of the
interesting things that I think people
mistake about depression they just think
that when I'm depressed I have sad mood
I have negative mood that's not entirely
true one of the principal features of
depression is something that we call
anhedonia which is an absence of having
the ability to have honic responses in
other words you can't get pleasure from
normally pleasurable things it's not an
issue about sliding down to the negative
it's the absence of being able to
experience the positive that puts you on
a track towards depression and what you
and I discussed earlier in this episode
is some of the work that we've been
doing where when you sleep deprive
individuals but you show them very
rewarding based stimuli they become much
more reward sensitive and perhaps this
is why patients will respond to sleep
deprivation with depression because
they're too far away from that positive
end of the spectrum they're not reward
sensitive enough they don't get a
positive good feeling now if you're
someone who is healthy and you're sleep
deprived you go too far in the reward
direction and you become vulnerable to
reward and sensation seeking but if
you're depressed and you're shifted to
sort of away from that and sleep
deprivation brings you back closer to a
normative reward-based
reactivity maybe that's the reason why
you get this anti-press present benefit
and why when you start sleeping again
you take away that enhanced reward
sensitivity and you lose the
anti-depressant benefit so I think we
still don't know enough
about depression and sleep yet if you
were to ask me of the four quantity
quality regularity and timing which
would be
ideal I would say all four are
definitely players but timing may have
some of the best evidence because it's
not just about sleep when it comes to
depression it's also about your
circadian rhythm that if you are not
aligned with your natural chronotype
your natural 24-hour Rhythm Cadian
misalignment when you fall out of
synchrony with your natural chronotype
is a strong predictor of depression so
if there is an actionable item first it
would be to say from a big picture
perspective understand that sleep is one
of
the least
painful available options for you as a
no cost to try to stabilize your mental
health now I'm not suggesting that all
psychiatric conditions are Sleep
Disorders that's not true and I'm not
suggesting that you should stop simply
at the place of getting your sleep
straight to help with your mental
conditions not at all I am saying
however that if you do get your sleep
straight it's only going to help and may
help quite a significant amount based on
the data but when it comes to depression
I would say of those four qqr T there's
very strong emerging data that Cadian
misalignment not matching your
chronotype to the time when you are
sleeping and the time you are awake is
one of the strongest factors so if you
want to say I can't do all of them Matt
I can't do all of this Q qrt nonsense
just tell me one of them to start with I
would say don't worry we'll get to the
three others let's just start with
getting your timing right let's
understand what type you are take the um
go online you can take one of these
tests the um meq the morningness
eveningness questionnaire you can just
Google it it's free you can we'll
provide a link to it in the show note
caption that's great um understand what
type you are and then try to understand
based on what time I'm currently
Awakening sleep is it matched is it
mismatched and if it's mismatched try to
see what you can do with your lifestyle
accommodating of course to match that
things will more than likely start there
getting
better along those lines um and if I may
uh I'd like to just mention a recent
study that I think dovetails with what
you just said beautifully and seems
highly actionable to me this was a study
published in um nature mental health
which is a relatively new Journal um but
it involved exploring the light exposure
and dark exposure patterns of I believe
it was more than 80,000 individuals I'll
have to go back and check that but what
was interesting is that um when they
looked at light exposure in particular
sunlight exposure and they looked at
Darkness exposure across the 24-hour
schedule what they concluded was that
there was a near linear relationship
between the amount of light that one
gets in the morning and throughout the
day and reduction in mental
health challenges at the in terms of
depression PTSD there were a few others
some of the effects were less robust for
certain psychiatric conditions than they
were for say depression what was equally
interesting is that Darkness the absence
of light turned out to be as important a
variable as light during the day Made
Simple
if people tended to be in dim or dark
light at night they experien reductions
in their suicidal depressive anxiety and
PTSD symptoms independent of how much
light they were getting during the day
so what this says is get as much light
as one can possibly and safely get in
their eyes by the way in the morning and
throughout the day and then do one's
very best to be in very dim or dark
environments at night yeah and even goes
so far as to say that if you didn't get
sunlight during the day then you would
be especially well off being in a very
dark environment at night and it's
independent so don't worry yes it's
always good to get that daylight but
what that paper also teaches us is that
because those things can be independent
you can still get some benefit even if
though you've not made it good on your
daylight during the day getting that
Darkness at night is still going to be
beneficial and I should probably resolve
what some people may think of as
confusing we spoke about for example
suicide risk and it being highest in
that in the the depths of the Darkness
at night I think what's clear from that
paper comes on to one of the fundamental
conventional tips that we spoke about in
how to optimize your sleep not just an
unconventional but the conventional
which was I told you we are a dark
deprived society and we need Darkness at
night to help keep our sleep regular so
the sort of the the r in the Q
qrt and I think there in that paper the
inference of course is that if you're
getting dark at night it's going to give
you a nice sleep onset signal so that
you are asleep at night in the darkness
and that sleep at night in the darkness
provides this beneficial you know sort
of not immunization but at least
pallative help to certain psychiatric
conditions we're not suggesting that
Darkness at night if you're awake at
night however is beneficial that seems
to be not beneficial um but it was such
a a great paper and very elegant in how
it dissected the independent nature of
these things which fits very well with I
think your mission in part in life both
as a scientist and as an educator which
is how can I curate information gather
it together and give you some type of
actionable boots on the ground feet in
the trenches advice as to what to do it
was a great paper um so thank you for
bringing it up yeah I only wish I had
done that study but I'm so glad that
others did one thing that's been helpful
to me to um encourage uh more darkness
and dim light at night uh for myself in
my home environment is to think about uh
artificial photons coming from
artificial sources as sort of um empty
calories
and how sunlight provided one isn't
getting a burn and you know people
debate uh you know how best to do that
physical barrier everyone agrees on
certain sunscreens are are safer than
others some are very safe some are
perhaps less safe in any event the point
is
that trying to make one's home
environment dark at night is in my mind
now uh akin to trying to avoid eating
sugary uh you know non-nutritious
calories at night as well it just lends
itself to a a um just overall feelings
of well-being improved sleep and of
course improved daytime wakefulness and
then getting sunlight even on uh through
cloud cover in one's eyes early in the
day and as much as as safely possible
throughout the day and if one can't get
sunlight getting light from Bright
artificial sources um seems to be the
best alternative um but I think there's
this asymmetry of Light Dark requirement
in the same way that I think most
everyone agrees that eating during one's
active hours of the day is going to be
the best way to go as opposed to eating
during the Ina less active hours of of
the late night and um and certainly
prior to sleep such a good point and
since it's only you and I here and no
one else watching and witnessing this I
am thoroughly going to steal that phrase
of junk light and help educate people
because that's a perfect description
it's like empty photons yeah you've all
heard of junk food well there's
something called Junk light and if you
get you know your whole foods during the
day just like you get your whole kind of
encompassed light during the day that's
great but then if you start binging on
junk light at night it's profoundly
deleterious to your sleep and everything
that sleep depends on it's lovely so
when people hear me in future um public
spheres talking about junk light you
know where it came from I will give you
full credit it's a delightful statement
because I I may have I may have lifted
from somebody else inadvertently um we
all stand on the shoulders of other
Giants that's right or other Twitter
accounts or something like that well I
place myself firly uh underneath a
pedestal but yes we all try to stand on
those shoulders of giants well wherever
you place yourself the the information
that that uh emerges from you and that
emerged today is absolutely spectacular
uh you know I can't think of topics U
more interesting and important then
emotion regulation anxiety PTSD suicide
sadly depress all all these things are
are tragic challenges that but they are
a real part of of Life some argue even
more so nowadays perhaps even because of
um the Advent of so much artificial
light and smartphone use in the middle
of the night who knows it's um I think
it's reasonable to assume it's at least
one variable today you've provided a ton
of depth of understanding about why
sleep and these mental health and
emotional states are linked just a
really clear logical framework for both
the non-rem sleep and and REM sleep and
how it impacts mood and and reactivity
during the daytime and also some really
actionable tools um to improve one's
mental health and emotion emotionality
excuse me um and in addition to that
we'll refer people back to episodes 1
two 3 and four all of which include
tools to improve every aspect of sleep
and to really really nail down the QQ
art T that quality do you put quality
first or quantity quantity quantity I'm
just making sure that qqr t to really
nail down the quantity quality
regularity and timing of sleep um we can
no longer consider sleep just uh 6 to
eight hours or get your nine hours or
get your um get your seven hours clearly
there are other variables involved and
you've made those variables very clear
to us and you've given us the road map
to plug in the best variables for
ourselves so thank you Matt ever so much
thank you for allowing me to both um
voice and narrate the important story of
sleep in mental health it's something
I'm immensely passionate about both from
a personal perspective but also from a
professional um research perspective
thank you for this opportunity well
again thank you Matt and I'm very much
looking forward to the sixth installment
in this series on sleep which is about a
topic that everybody is fascinated with
which is dreaming I know you're going to
tell us about dreams and what they mean
perhaps what they don't mean uh we'll
get into dream interpretation of all
things lucid dreaming and much much more
so I really look forward to that
discussion in episode 6 thank you for
joining me for today's episode with Dr
Matthew Walker to learn more about Dr
Walker's research and to learn more
about his book and his social media
handles please see the links in our show
note captions if you're learning from
Andor enjoying this podcast please
subscribe to our YouTube channel that's
a terrific zero cost way to support us
in addition please subscribe to the
podcast on both Spotify and apple and on
both Spotify and apple you can leave us
up to a festar review please also check
out the sponsors at the beginning and
throughout today's episode that's the
best way to support this podcast if you
have any questions for me or comments
about the podcast or topics or guests
that you'd like me to feature on the
hubman Lab podcast please put those in
the comment section on YouTube I do read
all the comments on many episodes of The
hubman Lab podcast we discuss
supplements while supplements aren't
necessary for everybody many people
derive tremendous benefit from them for
things like improving sleep for hormone
support and for Focus to learn more
about the supplements discussed on the
huberman loud podcast go to live
momentus spelled o us that's Liv
mous.com
huberman if you're not already following
me on social media I'm hubman lab on all
social media platforms so that's
Instagram X LinkedIn Facebook and
threads and on all those platforms I
discuss science and science related
tools some of which overlaps with the
content of the hubman Lab podcast
but much of which is distinct from the
content covered on the hubman Lab
podcast so again it's hubman lab on all
social media platforms if you haven't
already subscribe to our neural network
newsletter our neural network newsletter
is a zeroc cost newsletter that provides
podcast summaries as well as protocols
in the form of brief one to three-page
PDFs that cover everything from
neuroplasticity and learning to sleep to
deliberate cold exposure and deliberate
heat exposure we have a foundational
Fitness protocol and much more all of
which again is completely zero cost you
simply go to to hubman lab.com go to the
menu tab scroll down to newsletter and
by supplying your email you can
subscribe I want to point out that we do
not share your email with anybody thank
you once again for joining me for
today's discussion all about sleep with
Dr Matthew Walker and last but certainly
not least thank you for your interest in
science
[Music]