Dr. Jack Feldman: Breathing for Mental & Physical Health & Performance
- Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,
where we discuss science and science-based tools
for everyday life.
I'm Andrew Huberman,
and I'm a Professor of Neurobiology and Ophthalmology
at Stanford School of Medicine.
Today, my guest is Dr. Jack Feldman.
Dr. Jack Feldman
is a distinguished Professor of Neurobiology
at the University of California, Los Angeles.
He is known for his pioneering work
on the neuroscience of breathing.
We are all familiar with breathing
and how essential breathing is to life.
We require oxygen, and it is only by breathing
that we can bring oxygen to all the cells
of our brain and body.
However, as the work from Dr. Feldman
and colleagues tells us,
breathing is also fundamental to organ health and function
at an enormous number of other levels.
In fact, how we breathe, including how often we breathe,
the depth of our breathing and the ratio
of inhales to exhales actually predicts how focused we are,
how easily we get into sleep,
how easily we can exit from sleep.
Dr. Feldman gets credit for the discovery
of the two major brain centers that control
the different patterns of breathing.
Today, you'll learn about those brain centers
and the patterns of breathing they control,
and how those different patterns of breathing influence
all aspects of your mental and physical life.
What's especially wonderful about Dr. Feldman and his work
is that it not only points to the critical role
of respiration in disease,
in health and in daily life, but he's also a practitioner.
He understands how to leverage particular aspects
of the breathing process in order to bias the brain
to be in particular states that can benefit us all.
Whether or not you are a person
who already practices breathwork,
or whether or not you're somebody
who simply breathes to stay alive,
by the end of today's discussion
you're going to understand a tremendous amount
about how the breathing system works
and how you can leverage that breathing system
toward particular goals in your life.
Dr. Feldman shares with us
his own particular breathing protocols that he uses,
and he suggests different avenues for exploring respiration
in ways that can allow you, for instance,
to be more focused for work,
to disengage from work and high stress endeavors
to calm down quickly.
And indeed, he explains not only how to do that,
but all the underlying science in ways that will allow
you to customize your own protocols for your needs.
All the guests that we bring on The Huberman Lab Podcast
are considered at the very top of their fields.
Today's guest, Dr. Feldman,
is not only at the top of his field, he founded the field.
Prior to his coming into neuroscience
from the field of physics,
there really wasn't much information
about how the brain controls breathing.
There was a little bit of information,
but we can really credit Dr. Feldman
and his laboratory for identifying
the particular brain areas
that control different patterns of breathing
and how that information can be leveraged towards health,
high performance and for combating disease.
So, today's conversation you're going
to learn a tremendous amount
from the top researcher in this field.
It's a really wonderful and special opportunity
to be able to his knowledge with you,
and I know that you're not only going to enjoy it,
but you are going to learn a tremendous amount.
Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast
is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.
It is however, part of my desire and effort
to bring zero cost to consumer information
about science and science related tools
to the general public.
In keeping with that theme,
I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.
Our first sponsor is Thesis.
Thesis as a company that makes nootropics.
Now, I've talked before on the podcast and elsewhere
about the fact that I don't really like the term nootropics,
which means smart drugs,
because smart means many different things
in many different contexts.
You've got creativity, you've got focus,
you've got task switching.
So, the idea that there will be one pill or one formula
that could make us smarter and better
at all those things at once, just doesn't stand up to logic.
In fact, different chemicals
and different brain systems underlie
our ability to be creative or our ability to task switch
or to be focused.
And that's the basis of Thesis.
Thesis as a company that makes targeted nootropics
for specific outcomes.
In other words, specific nootropics to get your brain
into states that are ideal
for what you're trying to accomplish.
Thesis uses very high quality ingredients,
many of which I've talked about before on the podcast,
such as DHA, Gingko biloba, and phosphatidylserine.
I talked about those in the ADHD podcast.
Those are some of the ingredients
in their so-called Logic formula.
There's a lot of research showing that Gingko biloba
can be very helpful for increasing levels of focus,
and even for people with ADHD.
However, I can't take it.
When I take it, I get really bad headaches
and I know some people who do
and some people who don't get headaches
when they take Gingko biloba.
This is a great example of why nootropics need
to be personalized to the individual.
Thesis gives you the ability
to try different blends over the course of a month
and discover which nootropic formulas work best
for your unique brain chemistry and genetics,
and which ones are best for particular circumstances.
So, they have a formulation for instance,
which is Motivation.
They have another formulation, which has Clarity.
They've got another formulation, which is Logic.
And each of these is formulated specifically to you
and formulated to a specific end point or goal state of mind
for your particular work.
So as a consequence, the formulations that you arrive at
will have a very high probability
of giving you the results that you want.
In addition to that detailed level of personalization,
Thesis takes a step further by offering free consultations
with a brain coach to help you optimize your experience,
and dial in your favorite and best formulas.
I've been using Thesis for close to six months now,
and I can confidently say
that their nootropics have been a total game changer for me.
My favorite of the formulations is their Motivation formula
that they've tailored to me.
When I use that formula,
I have very clear state of mind, I have even energy,
and I use that early in the day until the early afternoon
to get the bulk of my most important work done.
To get your own personalized nootropic starter kit,
you can go online to takethesis.com/huberman,
take a three minute quiz and Thesis
will send you four different formulas
to try in your first month.
That's takethesis.com/huberman and use the code Huberman
at checkout to get 10% off your first order.
Today's episode is also brought
to us by Athletic Greens.
Athletic Greens
is an all-in-one vitamin mineral probiotic drink.
I've been using Athletic Greens since 2012,
and so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast.
The reason I started taking Athletic Greens,
and the reason I still take Athletic Greens
is that it covers all of my vitamin,
mineral and probiotic foundational needs.
There's now a wealth of data showing
that not only do we need vitamins and minerals,
but we also need to have a healthy gut microbiome.
The gut microbiome is a set of nerve connections
that link the microbiota,
literally microbacteria that live in our guts
and that are healthy for us with our brain function.
And our brain is also talking to our gut
in a bi-directional way.
And that conversation is vital for metabolism,
for the endocrine system,
meaning the hormonal system
and for overall mood and cognition.
There's just so much data now pointing to the fact
that we need a healthy gut microbiome
and a healthy brain gut axis as it's called.
By taking Athletic Greens once or twice a day,
I can get the vitamins and minerals
and the probiotics needed
for all those systems to function optimally.
And again, it tastes great, it's great for me.
In fact, if people ask me,
"What's the one supplement that I should take,"
and they can only take one supplement,
I recommend Athletic Greens for all the reasons I mentioned.
If you'd like to try Athletic Greens,
you can go to athleticgreens.com/huberman
to claim a special offer.
They're giving you five free travel packs,
which are these little travel packs to make it easy
to mix up Athletic Greens while you're on the road
or in the car or on the plane, et cetera,
and a year supply of Vitamin D3+K2.
There is also a wealth of evidence showing
that Vitamin D3 is vital to our overall health,
and K2 is important for cardiovascular health
and other systems as well.
Most of us are not getting enough vitamin D3 or K2,
even if we're getting some sunshine.
So again, if you go to athleticgreens.com/huberman,
you get the five free travel packs,
a year supply of Vitamin D3+K2.
Athleticgreens.com/huberman is where you go
to claim that special offer.
Today's episode is also brought to us by Headspace.
Headspace is a meditation app that's supported
by 25 published studies and benefits
from over 600,000 five-star reviews.
I've long been a believer in meditating.
There is so much data now pointing to the fact
that regular meditation leads to reduced stress levels,
heightened levels of focus, better task switching,
and cognitive ability.
It just goes on and on.
I mean, there are literally thousands
of peer reviewed studies now in quality journals pointing
to the benefits of having a regular meditation practice.
The problem with meditation
is many people, including myself,
have struggled with sticking to that practice.
With Headspace, it makes it very easy to start
and continue a meditation protocol.
The reason for that is they have meditations
that are of different lengths and different styles
so you don't get bored of meditation.
And even if you just have five minutes,
there are five minute meditations.
If you've got 20 minutes,
which would be even better,
there are 20 minute meditations.
Ever since I started using Headspace,
I've been consistent about my meditation.
I do meditation anywhere from five to seven times a week
in my best weeks, and sometimes that's drops to three
and then I find with Headspace,
I can quickly get back to doing meditation every day
because of the huge variety
of great meditations that they have.
If you want to try Headspace,
you can go to headspace.com/specialoffer.
And if you do that,
you can get a free one month trial
with Headspace's full library of meditations
for every situation.
That's the best deal offered by Headspace right now.
So again, if you're interested,
go to headspace.com/specialoffer.
One quick mention before we dive
into the conversation with Dr. Feldman.
During today's episode,
we discuss a lot of breathwork practices
and by the end of the episode,
all those will be accessible to you.
However, I'm aware that there are a number
of people out there that want to go even further
into the science and practical tools of breathwork.
And for that reason, I want to mention a resource to you.
There is a cost associated with this resource,
but it's a terrific platform
for learning about breathwork practices
and for building a number of different routines
that you can do, or that you could teach.
It's called Our Breathwork Collective.
I'm not associated with the Breathwork Collective,
but Dr. Feldman is an advisor to the group
and they offer daily live guided breathing sessions
and an on-demand library
that you can practice any time free workshops on breathwork.
And these are really developed by experts in the field,
including Dr. Feldman.
So, as I mentioned, I'm not on their advisory board,
but I do know them and their work
and it is of the utmost quality.
So anyone wanting to learn or teach breathwork
could really benefit from this course, I believe.
If you'd like to learn more,
you can click on the link in the show notes
or visit ourbreathcollective.com/huberman,
and use the code Huberman at checkout.
And if you do that,
they're offering you $10 off the first month.
Again, it's ourbreathcollective.com/huberman
to access the Our Breath Collective.
And now for my conversation with Dr. Jack Feldman.
Thanks for joining me today.
- It's a pleasure to be here, Andrew.
- Yeah, it's been a long time coming.
You're my go-to source for all things respiration.
I mean, I breathe on my own,
but when I want to understand how I breathe
and how the brain and breathing interact,
you're the person I call.
- Well, I'll do my best.
As you know, there's a lot that we don't understand,
which still keeps me employed and engaged,
but we do know a lot.
- Why don't we start off by just talking
about what's involved in generating breath.
And if you would, could you comment on some
of the mechanisms for rhythmic breathing
versus non rhythmic breathing?
- Okay, so on the mechanical side,
which is obvious to everyone,
we want to have air flow in, inhale,
and we need to have air flow out
and the reason we need to do this
is because for body metabolism, we need oxygen.
And when oxygen is utilized through
the aerobic metabolic process, we produce carbon dioxide.
And so, we have to get rid of the carbon dioxide
that we produce in particular
because the carbon dioxide affects
the acid base balance of the blood, the pH,
and all living cells are very sensitive
to what the pH value is,
so your body is very interested in regulating that pH.
So we have to have enough oxygen for our normal metabolism,
and we have to get rid of the CO2 that we produce.
So, how do we generate this air flow?
Well, the air comes into the lungs.
We have to expand the lungs and as the lungs expand,
basically, it's like a balloon that you would pull apart.
The pressure inside that balloon drops
and the air will flow into the balloon.
So we expand, put pressure on the lungs to pull it apart,
that lowers the pressure in the air sacks called alveoli
and air will flow in because pressure outside the body
is higher than pressure inside the body
when you're doing this expansion, when you're inhaling.
What produces that?
Well, the principle muscle is the diaphragm,
which is sitting inside the body just below the lung,
and when you want to inhale,
you basically contract the diaphragm and it pulls it down.
And as it pulls it down,
it's inserting pressure forces on the lung,
the lung wants to expand.
At the same time, the rib cage is going to rotate up and out,
and therefore expanding the cavity, the thoracic cavity.
At the end of inspiration,
under normal conditions when you're aggressed,
you just relax and it's like pulling on a spring.
You pull down a spring and you let go and relax.
So, you inhale and you exhale.
Inhale, relax, and exhale.
- So, the exhale is passive?
- At rest it's passive.
We'll get into what happens when you need to increase
the amount of air you're bringing in
because your ventilation,
your metabolism goes up like during exercise.
Now the muscles themselves,
skeletal muscles don't do anything unless
the nervous system tells them to do something.
And when the nervous system tells
them to do something, they contract.
So, there are specialized neurons in the spinal cord,
and then above the spinal cord,
the region called the brainstem,
which go to respiratory muscles,
in particular for inspiration in the diaphragm
and the external intercostal muscles in the rib cage.
And they contract.
So, these respiratory muscles these inspiratory muscles
become active and they become active for a period of time,
then they become silent and when they become silent,
the muscles then relax back to their original resting level.
Where does that activity in these neurons
that innovate the muscle, which are called motor neurons,
where does that originate?
Well, this was a question that's been bandied around
for thousands of years,
and when I was a beginning assistant professor,
it was fairly high priority for me
to try and figure that out,
because I wanted to understand
where this rhythm of breathing was coming from
and you couldn't know where it was coming from
until you knew where it was coming from.
And I didn't phrase that properly.
You couldn't understand how it was being done
until you know where to look.
So, we did a lot of experiments,
which I can go into detail and finally found,
there was a region in the brainstem,
that's once again this region sort of above the spinal cord,
which was critical for generating this rhythm.
It's called the pre-Botzinger complex.
And we can talk about how that was named.
This small site, which contains in humans,
a few thousand neurons,
it's located on either side and works in tandem
and every breath begins with neurons
in this region beginning to be active,
and those neurons then connect ultimately
to these motor neurons going to the diaphragm
and to the external intercostals causing them
to be active and causing this inspiratory effort.
When the neurons in the pre-Botzinger complex finish
their burst of activity,
then inspiration stops and then you begin
to exhale because of this passive recoil
of the lung and rib cage.
- Could I just briefly interrupt you to ask
a few quick questions - Of course.
- before we move forward in this very informative answer.
The two questions are,
is there anything known about the activation
of the diaphragm and the intercostal muscles
between the ribs as it relates
to nose versus mouth breathing,
or are they activated in the equivalent way,
regardless of whether or not someone
is breathing through their nose or mouth?
- I don't think we fully have the answer to that.
Clearly there are differences
between nasal and mouth breathing.
At rest the tendency is to do nasal breathing
because the air flows that are necessary
for normal breathing as easily managed
by passing through the nasal cavities.
However, when your ventilation needs to increase
like during exercise, you need to move more air,
you do that through your mouth because the airways
are much larger then,
and therefore you can move much more air,
but at the level of the intercostals and the diaphragm,
their contraction is not,
is almost agnostic to whether or not
the nose and mouth are open.
- Okay, so if I understand correctly,
there's no reason to suspect that there are particular,
perhaps even non overlapping sets of neurons
in pre-Botzinger area of the brainstem
that triggered nasal versus mouth inhales?
- No, I would say that it's not
that the pre-Botzinger complex is not concerned
and cannot influence that,
but it does not appear as if there's any modulation
of whether or not it's where the air is coming from,
whether it's coming through
your nasal passages or through your mouth.
- Great. Thank you.
And then the other question I have
is that these intercostal muscles
between the ribs then move the ribs up and out
if I understand correctly, and the diaphragm,
are those skeletal or as the Brits would say,
skeletal muscles or smooth muscles?
What type of muscle are we talking about here?
- As I said earlier, these are skeletal,
I didn't say there was skeletal muscles,
but they're muscles that need neural input
in order to move.
You talked about smooth muscles.
They are specialized muscles like we have
in the gut and in the heart,
and these are muscles that are capable
of actually contracting and relaxing on their own.
So, the heart beats,
it doesn't need neural input in order to beat.
The neural inputs modulate the strength
of it and the frequency, but they beat on their own.
The skeletal muscles involved
in breathing need neural input.
Now, there are smooth muscles
that have an influence on breathing,
and these are muscles that are lining the airways.
And so, the airways have smooth muscle
and when they become activated,
the smooth muscle can contract or relax,
and when they contract inappropriately
is when you have problems breathing like in asthma.
Asthma is a condition
where you get inappropriate constriction
of the smooth muscles of the airways.
- So, there's no reason to think that in asthma
that the pre-Botzinger or these other neuronal centers
in the brain that activate breathing,
that they are involved or causal for things like asthma?
- As of now, I would say the preponderance of evidence
is that it's not involved,
but we've not really fully investigated that.
- Thank you.
Sorry to break your flow,
but I was terribly interested in knowing answers
to those questions and you provided them, so thank you.
- Now, remind me again, where I was in my-
- We were just landing in pre-Botzinger
and we will return to the naming
of pre-Botzinger because it's a wonderful
and important story really,
that I think people should be aware of.
But maybe you could march us through the brain centers
that you've discovered and others have worked
on as well that control breathing,
pre-Botzinger as well
as related structures. - Okay.
So, when we discovered the pre-Botzinger,
we thought that it was the primary source
of all rhythmic respiratory movements,
both inspiration and expiration.
Their notion of a single source is like day or night.
It's like they're all coming,
they all have the same origin
that the Earth rotates and day follows night
and we thought that the pre-Botzinger complex
would be inhalation, exhalation.
And then in a series of experiments
we did in the early part of 2000,
we discovered that there seemed to be another region
which was dominant in producing expiratory movements,
that is the exhalation.
We had made a fundamental mistake
with the discovery of the pre-Botzinger,
not taking into account
that at rest expiratory muscle activity
or exhalation is passive.
So, if that's the case,
a group of neurons that might generate active expiration,
that is to contract the expiratory muscles,
like the abdominal muscles
or the internal intercostals are just silent.
We just thought it wasn't,
the air was coming from one place,
but we got evidence that in fact,
it may have been coming from another place.
And following up on these experiments,
we discovered that there was a second oscillator
and that oscillator is involved
in generating what we call active expiration.
That is this act of- - If I go [exhales].
- Yeah [exhales], or when you begin to exercise,
you have to go [panting], and actually move that air out.
This group of cells, which is silent at rest
suddenly becomes active to drive those muscles,
and it appears that it's an independent oscillator when-
- Maybe you could just clarify
for people what an oscillator is.
- Okay, an oscillator is something that goes in a cycle.
So, you can have a pendulum
as an oscillator going back and forth.
The Earth is an oscillator because it goes around
and it's day and night.
- Some people's moods are oscillating.
- Oscillating.
And it depends how regular they are.
You can have oscillators that are highly regular
or that are in a watch,
or you can have those that are sporadic or episodic.
Breathing is one of those oscillators
that for life has to be working continuously 24/7.
It starts late in the third trimester
because it has to be working when you're born,
and basically works throughout life and if it stops,
if there's no intervention beyond a few minutes,
it will likely be fatal.
- What is this second oscillator called?
- Well, we found that in a region around the facial nucleus,
so we initially,
when this region was initially identified,
we thought it was involved in sensing carbon dioxide.
It was what we call a central chemo receptor.
That is, we want to keep carbon dioxide levels,
particularly in the brain at a relatively stable level
'cause the brain is extraordinarily sensitive
to changes in pH.
If there's a big shift in carbon dioxide
there'll be a big shift in brain pH,
and that'll throw your brain,
if I can use the technical term, out of whack.
And so, you want to regulate that
and the way to regulate something in the brain
is you have a sensor in the brain.
And others basically identified
that the ventral surface of the brainstem,
that is the part of the brainstem that's on this side,
was critical for that and then we identified
a structure that was near the trapezoid nucleus.
It was not named in any of these noranatomical atlases,
so we just picked the name out of the hat
and we called it the retro trapezoid nucleus.
- I should clarify for people.
When Jack is saying trapezoid,
it doesn't mean the trapezoid muscles.
Trapezoid refers to the shape of this nucleus,
this cluster of neurons.
Parafacial makes me think that this general area
is involved in something related to mouth or face.
Is it an area rich with neurons controlling other parts
of the face, eye blinks,
nose twitches, lip curls, lip smacks?
- If you go back in an evolutionary sense
and a lot of things that are hard to figure out
begin to make sense when you look
at the evolution of the nervous system.
When control of facial muscles,
going back to more primitive creatures
because they had to take things in their mouth for eating,
so we call that the face sort of developed,
the eyes were there, the mouth is there,
these nuclei, the modem that contained the motor neurons,
a lot of the control systems for them developed
in the immediate vicinity.
So, if you think about the face,
there's a lot of sudden nuclei around there
that had various roles
at various different times in evolution.
And at one point in evolution,
the facial muscles were probably very important
in moving fluid in and out of the mouth
and moving air in and out of the mouth,
and so part of these many different sub nuclei
now seems to be in mammals to be involved
in the control of expiratory muscles.
But we have to remember that mammals
are very special when it comes to breathing,
because we're the only class of vertebrates
that have a diaphragm.
If you look at amphibians and reptiles,
they don't have a diaphragm.
And the way they breathe is not by actively inspiring
and passively expiring.
They breathe by actively expiring and passively inspiring
because they don't have a powerful inspiratory muscle.
And somewhere along the line, the diaphragm developed,
and there are lots of theories about how it developed.
I don't think it's particularly clear.
There was something that you can find in alligators
and lizards that could have turned into a muscle
that was the diaphragm.
The amazing thing about the diaphragm
is that it's mechanically extremely efficient.
And what do I mean by that?
Well, if you look at how oxygen gets
from outside the body into the bloodstream,
the critical passage is across the membrane in the lung.
It's called the alveolar capillary membrane.
The alveolus is part of the lung
and the blood runs through capillaries,
which are the smallest tubes in the circulatory system.
And at that point,
oxygen can go from the air filled alveolus into the blood.
- Which is amazing.
I find that amazing,
even though it's just purely mechanical,
the idea we had these little sacks in our lungs,
we inhale and the air goes in and literally
the oxygen can pass into the bloodstream-
- Passes into the bloodstream.
But the rate of which it passes will depend
on the characteristics of the membrane,
what the distance is between the alveolus
and the blood vessel, the capillary.
But the key element is the surface area.
The bigger the surface area,
the more oxygen that can pass through,
it's entirely a passive process.
There's no magic about making oxygen go in.
Now, how do you get a pack,
a large surface area in a small chest?
Well, you start out with one tube, which is the trachea,
the trachea expands.
Now you have two tubes.
Then you have four tubes and it keeps branching.
At some point, at the end of those branches,
you put a little sphere, which is an alveolus,
and that determines what the surface area is going to be.
Now, you then have a mechanical problem.
You have the surface area,
you have to be able to pull it apart.
So, imagine you have a little square of elastic membrane,
it doesn't take a lot of force to pull it apart,
but now if you increase it by 50 times,
you need a lot more force to pull it apart.
So, amphibians who were breathing,
not by compressing the lungs
and then just passively expanding it
weren't able to generate a lot of force,
so they have relatively few branches.
So, if you look at the surface area
that they pack in their lungs relative to their body size,
it's not very impressive.
Whereas when you get to mammals,
the amount of branching that you have
is you have four to 500,000,000 alveoli.
- If we were to take those four to 5,000,000-
- 100,000,000, four to 500,000,000.
- 100,000,000, excuse me, and lay those out flat,
what sort of surface area are we talking about?
- About 70 square meters,
which is about a third the size of a tennis court.
- Wow.
- So you have a membrane inside of you
a third the size of a tennis court
that you actually have to expand every breath.
And you do that without exerting much of a,
you don't feel it,
and that's because you have this amazing muscle,
the diaphragm, which because of its positioning
just by moving two thirds of an inch down
is able to expand that membrane enough
to move air into the lungs.
Now, the at rest, - Wow.
- the volume of air in your lungs
is about two and a half liters.
Do we need to convert that to quartz?
- No.
- Right, so about two and a half liters.
When you take a breath,
you're taking another 500 milliliters or half a liter.
That's the size maybe of my fist.
So, you're increasing the volume by 20%,
but you're doing that by pulling
on this 70 square meter membrane.
But that's enough to bring enough fresh air
into the lung to mix in with the air that's already there,
that the oxygen levels in your bloodstream
goes from a partial pressure of oxygen,
which is 40 millimeters of mercury
to 100 millimeters of mercury.
So, that's a huge increase in oxygen
and that's enough to sustain normal metabolism.
So, we have this amazing mechanical advantage
by having a diaphragm.
- Do you think that our brains are larger
than that of other mammals in part,
because of the amount of oxygen
that we have been able to bring into our system?
- I would say a key step in the ability
to develop a large brain that has a continuous demand
for oxygen is the diaphragm.
Without a diaphragm, you're an amphibian,
and there's another solution to increasing oxygen uptake
which is the way birds breathe,
but birds have other limitations
and they still can't get brains as big as mammals have.
So, the brain utilizes maybe 20% of all the oxygen
that we intake and it needs to continuously.
The brain doesn't want to be neglected.
So, this puts certain demands on breathing system.
In other words, you can't shut it down for awhile,
which poses other issues.
You're born, and you have to mature.
You have the small body, you have a small lung,
you have a very pliant rib cage,
and now you have to develop into an adult,
which has a stiffer rib cage.
And so, there are changes happening
in your brain and your body,
where the neural control of breathing
has to change on the fly.
It's not like for things like vision
where you have the opportunity to sleep
and while you're sleeping,
the brain is capable of doing things
that are not easy to doing during wakefulness,
like the construction crew comes in during sleep.
The change in breathing has been described
as trying to build an airplane while it's flying.
- Basically what Jack is saying
is that respiration science is more complex
and hardworking than vision science,
which is a direct jab at me that some
of you might've missed, but I definitely did not miss,
and I appreciate that you always take the opportunity
like a good New Yorker to give me a good,
healthy intellectual jab.
A question related to diaphragmatic breathing
versus non diaphragmatic breathing
because the way you describe it,
the diaphragm is always involved
but over the years whether it be
for yoga class or a breathwork thing,
or you hear online that we should
be breathing with our diaphragm,
that rather than lifting our rib cage
when we breathe [inhales] and our chest,
that it is "healthier" in air quotes
or better somehow to have the belly expand when we inhale.
I'm not aware of any particular studies
that have really examined
the direct health benefits of diaphragmatic
versus non diaphragmatic breathing,
but if you don't mind commenting on anything
you're aware of as it relates
to diaphragmatic versus non diaphragmatic breathing,
whether or not people tend
to be diaphragmatic breathers by default, et cetera,
that would be, I think interesting to a number of people.
- Well, I think by default,
we are obligate diaphragm breathers.
There may be pathologies where the diaphragm
is compromised and you have to use other muscles,
and that's a challenge.
Certainly at rest other muscles can take over,
but if you need to increase your ventilation,
the diaphragm is very important.
It would be hard to increase your ventilation otherwise.
- Do you pay attention to whether or not
you are breathing in a manner
where your belly goes out a little bit as you inhale,
because I can do it both ways, right?
I can inhale [inhales], bring my belly in,
or I can inhale [inhales], push my diaphragm and belly out.
Not the diaphragm out,
but and that's interesting, right?
Because it's a completely different muscle set
for each version.
- Well, in the context of things like breath practice,
I'm a bit agnostic about the effects
of some of the different patterns are breathing.
Clearly, some are going to work through different mechanisms,
and we can talk about that,
but at a certain level for example,
whether it's primarily diaphragm
where you move your abdomen or not, I am agnostic about it.
I think that the changes that breathing induces
in emotion and cognition,
I have different ideas about what the influence is
and I don't see that primarily as how,
which particular muscles you're choosing,
but that just could be my own prejudice.
- Okay.
We will return to that as a general theme in a little bit.
I want to ask you about sighing.
One of the many great gifts that you've given us over
the years is an understanding of these things
that we call physiological sighs.
Could you tell us about physiological sighs?
What's known about them,
what your particular interest in them is
and what they're good for?
- A very interesting and important question.
So, everyone has a sense of what a sigh is.
We certainly, when we're emotional,
in some ways we're stressed, we're particularly happy,
[inhales] we'll take a little sigh.
It turns out that we're sighing all the time.
And when I would ask people
who are not particularly knowledgeable
that have read my papers or James Nestor's book
or listened to your podcast,
they're usually off by two orders of magnitude
about how frequently we sigh on the low side.
In other words, they say once an hour, 10 times a day.
We sigh about every five minutes,
and I would encourage anyone who finds that
to be a unbelievable fact,
is to lie down in a quiet room and just breathe normally,
just relax, just let go,
and just pay attention to your breathing
and you'll find that every couple of minutes,
you're [inhales] taking a deep breath and you can't stop it.
It just happens.
Now, why?
Well, we have to go back to the lung again.
The lung has these 500,000,000 alveoli,
and they're very tiny.
They're 200 microns across.
So, they're really, really tiny.
And you can think of them as fluid filled.
They're fluid lined.
And the reason their fluid lined
has to do with the esoterica of the mechanics of that.
It makes it a little easier to stretch
them with this fluid line, which is called surfactant.
And surfactant is important during development,
it is a determining factor in the,
when premature infants are born.
If they have not do not have lung surfactant
it makes it much more challenging to take care of them
than after they have lung surfactant,
which is sometime, if I remember correctly,
in the late second, early third trimester, which it appears.
In any case it's fluid line.
Now, think of a balloon that you would blow up,
but now before you blow it up, fill the balloon with water,
squeeze all the water out and now,
when you squeeze all the water out
you notice the sides of the balloon stick to each other.
Why is that?
Well, that's because water
has what's called surface tension.
And when you have two surfaces of water together,
they actually tend to stick to each other.
Now, when you try and blow that balloon up,
you know that it,
or you'll notice if you've ever done it before,
that the balloon is a little harder to inflate
than if we're dry on the inside.
Why is that?
Because you have to overcome that surface tension.
Well, your alveoli have a tendency to collapse.
There's 500,000,000 in them.
They're not collapsing at a very high rate,
but it's a slow rate that's not trivial.
And when an alveolus collapses it no longer
can receive oxygen or take carbon dioxide out.
It's sort of taken out of the equation.
Now, if you have 500,000,000 in them and you lose 10,
no big deal, but if they keep collapsing,
you can lose a significant part
of the surface area of your lung.
Now, a normal breath is not enough to pop them open,
but if you take [inhales] a deep breath
it pops them open. - Through nose or your mouth?
- Doesn't matter. - Okay.
- Doesn't matter. - Or-
- It just increased that lung volume
'cause you're just pulling on the lungs,
they'll pop open every about every five minutes.
And so, we're doing it every five minutes
in order to maintain the health of our lung.
In the early days of mechanical ventilation,
which was used to treat polio victims
who had weakness of their respiratory muscles,
they'd be put in these big steel tubes
and the way that would work is that the pressure outside
the body would drop.
That would put a expansion pressure on the lungs, excuse me,
on the rib cage.
The rib cage would expand and then the lung would expand.
And then the pressure would go back to normal
and the lung and rib cage would go back to normal.
This was great for getting ventilation,
but there was a relatively high mortality rate.
It was a bit of a mystery.
And one solution was to just give bigger breaths.
They'd give bigger breaths and the mortality rate dropped,
and it wasn't until I think it was the '50s
where they realized that they didn't
have to increase every breath to be big.
What they needed to do
was every so often they to have one big breath.
So, you have a couple of minutes of normal breaths,
and then one big breath just mimicking
the physiological sighs,
and then the mortality rate dropped significantly.
And if you see someone on a ventilator in the hospital,
if you watch every couple of minutes that you see
the membrane move up and down,
every couple of minutes there'll be a super breath
and that pops it open.
So, there are these mechanisms
for these physiological sighs.
So, just like with the collapse of the lungs,
where you need a big pressure to pop it open,
it's the same thing with the alveola.
You need a bigger pressure
and a normal breath is not enough.
So, you have to take a big inhale.
[Jack inhales]
[Jack exhales]
And what nature has done
is instead of requiring us to remember to do it,
it does it automatically.
And it does it about every five minutes.
And one of the questions we asked
is how is this happening?
Why every five minutes?
What's doing it?
And we got into it through a back door.
Typical of the way a lot of science gets done.
This is serendipitous event where you run across a paper
and something clicks and you just, you follow it up.
Sometimes you go down blind ends,
but this turned out to be incredibly productive.
One of the guys in my lab was reading a paper about stress,
and during stress lots of things happen in the body,
one of which is that the hypothalamus,
which is very reactive to body state releases peptides,
which are specialized molecules,
which then circulate throughout the brain and body,
that particular effects usually
to help deal better with the stress.
And one class of the peptides that are released
are called Bombesin related peptides.
And he also realize because he was a breathing guy,
that when you're stressed you sigh more.
So we said, "All right, maybe they're related."
Bombesin is relatively cheap to buy.
We said, Let's buy some Bombesin
and throw it in the brainstem, let's see what happens."
And one of the nice things about some experiments
that we try to design is to fail quickly.
So here we had the idea,
we throw Bombesin in and the Bombesin did nothin',
nothing lost, maybe $50 to buy the Bombesin.
But if it did something it might be of some interest.
So, one afternoon we did the experiment and he comes to me,
he says, I won't quote exactly what he said,
because that might need to be censored,
but he said, "Look at this."
And it was in a rat.
Rats sigh about every two minutes.
They're smaller than we are
and they need to sigh more often.
Their sigh rate went from 20 to 30 per hour to 500 per hour
when you put Bombesin into the pre-Botzinger complex.
- [Andrew] Amazing.
- And the way he did that is he took a very,
very fine glass needle and anesthetized a rat,
and inserted that needle directly
into the pre-Botzinger complex.
So, it wasn't an internalized delivery of the peptide.
It was localized in the pre-Botzinger,
and the sigh rate went through the roof.
- And I would add that that was an important experiment
to deliver the Bombesin directly to that site
because one could have concluded that the injection
of the Bombesin increased sighing
because it increased stress
rather than directly increased sighing.
- Amongst hundreds of other possible interpretations.
So, the precision here is very important,
and that goes back to what I said at the very beginning,
knowing where this is happening allows
you to do the proper investigations.
If we didn't know where the inspiratory rhythm
was originating,
we've never could have done this experiment.
And so, then we did another experiment.
We said, "Okay, what happens if we take the cells
in the pre-Botzinger that are responding to the peptides?
So, neurons will respond to a peptide
because they have specialized receptors for that peptide.
And not every neuron expresses those receptors.
In the pre-Botzinger complex,
it's probably a few hundred out of thousands.
So, we used the technique we had used before,
and this is a technique developed
by Doug Laffey down in San Diego,
where you could take a peptide
and conjugate it with a molecule called saporin.
Saporin is a plant derived molecule,
which is a cousin to ricin.
And many of your listeners may have heard of ricin and-
- It's a ribosomal toxin.
- It's very nasty.
A single stab with an umbrella will kill you,
which is something that supposedly happened
to a Bulgarian diplomat by a Russian operative
on a bridge in London.
He got stabbed and the way ricin works
is it goes inside a cell, crosses the cell membrane,
goes inside the cell, kills the cell,
then it goes to the next cell and then the next cell,
and then the next cell.
It's extremely dangerous.
In fact, it's virtually impossible
to work on in a lab in the United States.
They won't let you touch it. - Ricin?
- Ricin 'cause-
- We've worked with saporin many times.
- Saporin is safe because it doesn't cross cell membranes.
So, you got an injection of saporin,
it won't do anything because it stays outside of cells.
- Please, nobody do that,
even though it doesn't cross cell membranes,
please, nobody inject saporin
whether or not you are a operative or otherwise.
- Thank you, Andrew, for protecting me there.
So, but what Doug Laffey figured out
is that when a ligand binds the receptor,
that's when a molecule binds to its receptor,
in many cases that receptor ligand complex
gets pulled inside the cell.
So, it goes from the membrane of the cell inside the cell.
- Sort of like you can't go to the dance alone,
but if you're coupled up, you get in the door.
- That's right.
So, what he figured out is he put saporin to the peptide,
the peptide binds to its receptor, it gets internalized
and then when it's inside the cell,
saporin does the same thing that ricin does.
It kills the cell,
but then it can't go into the next cell.
So, the only cells that get killed,
or the more polite term ablated,
are cells that express that receptor.
So, if you have a big conglomeration of cells,
you have a few thousand and only 50
of them which express that receptor,
then you inject the saporin conjugated
to the ligand for the peptide, and only those 50 cells die.
So, we took Bombesin conjugated the saporin,
inject in the pre-Botzinger complex of rats,
and it took about a couple of days
for the saporin to actually ablate cells.
And what happened is that
the mice started sighing less and less, excuse me,
the rats started sighing less and less and less and less,
and essentially stopped signing.
- So, your student or postdoc, was it?
Murdered these cells, and as a consequence,
the sighing goes away. - Right.
- What was the consequence of eliminating sighing
on the internal state or the behavior of the rats?
Did they, in other words,
if one can't sigh, generate physiological sighs,
what is the consequence on state of mind?
You would imagine that carbon dioxide
would build up more readily
or to higher levels in the bloodstream
and that the animals would be more stressed.
That's a kind of logical extension of the way we set it up.
- It was less benign than that.
When the animals got to the point
where they weren't sighing then,
and we did not determine this,
but the presumption was that their lung function
significantly deteriorated,
and their whole health deteriorated significantly
and we had to sacrifice them.
So, I can't tell you whether they were stressed or not,
but their breathing got to be significantly deteriorated
that we sacrificed them at that point.
Now, we don't know whether that is specifically related
to the fact they didn't sigh
or that there was secondary damage due
to the fact that some cells die,
so we never determined that.
Now, after we did this study,
to be candid, it wasn't a high priority
for us to get this out the door and publish it.
So, it stayed on the shelf.
And then I got a phone call from a graduate student
at Stanford, Kevin Yackle,
who starts askin' me
all these interesting questions about breathing,
and I'm happy to answer them
but at some point it concerned me because he was working
for a renowned biochemist who worked
on lung in drosophila, fruit flies,
Mark Krasnow.
- Yeah, got my next door colleague.
- Right. - Yeah.
- And I said, "Why are you asking me this?"
And he said, "I was an undergraduate at UCLA
and you gave a lecture in my undergraduate class
and I was curious about breathing ever since."
So, that's one of those things which as a professor,
you love to hear that actually
it's something you really affected the life of a student.
- When you birthed the competitor,
but you had only yourself to blame.
- No, I don't look at that as a competitor.
I think that there's enough interesting things to go on.
I know some of our neuroscience colleagues say,
"You can work in my lab, but then when you leave my lab,
you've got to work on something different."
- No one I ever trained would've said that.
It's open field.
You want to work on something, you hop in the mix.
- And, but there are people like that,
neuroscientists like that.
I never felt that-
- I hear that they're breathing apparati
are disrupted and that causes a brain dysfunction
that leads to the behavior you just described.
It's actually not true,
but in terms of the- - So-
- So, before we talk about the beautiful story
with Yackle and Krasnow and Feldman Lab,
I want to just make sure that I understand.
So, if physiological sighs don't happen,
basically breathing overall suffers?
- Well, that would go back to the observations
in polio victims and these iron lungs
where the principal deficit was there was no hyperinflation
of the lungs and many of them deteriorated and died.
- And just to stay on this one more moment
before we moved to what you were about to describe,
we hear often that people will overdose on drugs
of various kinds because they stopped breathing.
So, barbiturates, alcohol combined with barbiturates
is a common cause of death for drug users
and contra-indications of drugs, and these kinds of things.
You hear all the time about celebrities dying
because they combined alcohol with barbiturates.
Is there any evidence that the sighs
that occurred during sleep or during states
of deep, deep relaxation and sedation
that sighs recover the brain?
Because you can imagine that if the sighs don't happen
as a consequence of some drug impacting these brain centers,
that that could be one cause
of basically asphyxiation and death.
- If you look at the progression of any mammal to death,
you find that their breathing slows down,
a death due to natural causes, their breathing slows down,
it will stop, and then they'll gasp.
So, we have the phrase dying gasp [inhales].
Super large breaths.
They're often described as an attempt to auto resuscitate,
that as you take that super deep breath
and that maybe it can kickstart the engine again.
We do not know the degree to such things as gasp
are really sighs that are particularly large.
And so, if you suppress the ability to gasp
in an individual who is subject to an overdose,
then whereas they might been able
to re-arouse their breathing,
if that's prevented, they don't get re-aroused.
So, that is certainly a possibility,
but this has not been investigated.
I mean, one of the things that I'm interested in
is in individuals who have diseases,
which will affect pre-Botzinger complex.
And there's data in Parkinson's disease
and multiple system atrophy,
which is another form of neurodegeneration
where there's loss of neurons in pre-Botzinger.
And the question is, and it also may happen in ALS,
sometimes referred to as Lou Gehrig's disease,
amyotrophic lateral sclerosis.
These individuals often die during sleep.
We have an idea that we have not been able
to get anyone to test is that patients with Parkinson's,
patients with MLS typically breathe
normally during wakefulness.
The disturbances that they have
in breathing is during sleep.
So, Parkinson's patients at the end stages of the disease
often have significant disturbances in their sleep pattern,
but not during wakefulness.
And we think that what could be happening
is that the proximate cause of death is not heart failure,
is that they become apneic.
They stop breathing and don't resuscitate.
And that resuscitation may or may not be due
to an explicit suppression of sighs,
but to an overall suppression
of the whole apparatus of the pre-Botzinger complex.
- Got it. Thank you.
So, Yackle calls you up.
- So, he calls me up and he's, great kid, super smart,
and he tells me about these experiments that he's doing
where he's looking in a database to try
and find out what molecules are enriched
in regions of the brain that are critical for breathing.
So, we and others have mapped out
these regions in the brainstem,
and he was looking at one of these databases
to see what's enriched.
And I said, "That's great.
Will you be willing to sort of share our work together?"
He says, "No, my advisor doesn't want me to do that."
So I said, "Okay," but Kevin's a great kid,
and I enjoy talking to him and he's a smart guy,
and what I found in academia is that
the smartest people only want to hire people smarter
than them and have the preference
to interact with people smarter than them.
The faculty who are not at the highest level
and at every institution, there's a distribution.
One's above the mean, and those below the mean,
those whom below the mean are very threatened by that.
And I saw Kevin as like a shining light,
and I didn't care whether he was going to out-compete me
because whatever he did was going to help me in the field,
so I did whatever I can to help him, to work with Kevin.
So, at one point I got invited
to give grand rounds in neurology at Stanford.
It turns out an undergraduate student who had worked with me
was now head of the training program
for neurologists at Stanford and he invited me.
And at the end of my visit,
I go to Mark Krasnow's office, and Kevin is there,
and a post-doc punctually
who was also working on a project was there.
And towards the end of the conversation,
Mark says to me,
"We found this one molecule
which is highly concentrated
in an important region for breathing."
I said, "Oh, that's great.
What is it?"
And he says, "I can't tell you
because we want to work on it."
So, I'm of course I'm disappointed,
but I realized that the ethic
in some areas of science or the custom
in some areas of science
is that until you get a publication,
you'll be relatively restricted
in sharing the information. - Mark and I
are going to have a chat
when I come back. - Okay, all right.
- Yeah.
- Well, he may remember the story differently,
but I said okay, and as I'm walking out the door,
I remember these experiments I described
to you about Bombesin,
and that was the only unusual molecule we're working.
So, the reason I'm rushing out the door
is I have a flight to catch.
So, I stick my head in and I said,
"Is this molecule related to Bombesin?"
And then I run off, I don't even wait for them to reply.
I can be up for it.
Mark calls me and he says, "Bombesin?
The peptide we found is related to Bombesin.
What does it do?"
And I said, "I'm not telling."
[Andrew laughs]
- Oh my.
I'm so glad I wasn't involved in this collaboration.
- No, no, but that was sort of a tease 'cause I said,
"Well, let's work together on this."
And then we worked together on this.
- It was a prisoner's dilemma at that point, yeah.
So, Kevin Yackle is spectacular, has his own lab at UCF,
and the work that I'm familiar with from Kevin
is worth mentioning now, or I'll ask you to mention it,
which is this reciprocal relationship between brain state,
or we could even say emotional state and breathing.
And I'd love to get your thoughts on how breathing interacts
with other things in the brain.
You've beautifully described
how breathing controls the lungs, the diaphragm,
and the interactions between oxygen
and carbon dioxide and so forth.
But as we know, when we get stressed, our breathing changes.
When we're happy and relaxed, our breathing changes.
But also if we change our breathing,
we, in some sense can adjust our internal state.
What is the relationship between brain state and breathing?
And if you would,
because I know you have a particular love
of one particular aspect of this,
what is the relationship between brain rhythms,
oscillations if you will, and breathing?
- This is a topic which has really intrigued me over
the past decade.
I would say before that I was in my silo,
just interested about how the rhythm
of breathing is generated,
and didn't really pay much attention to this other stuff.
For some reason I got interested in it,
and I think it was triggered by an article
in "The New York Times" about mindfulness.
Now, believe it or not,
although I had lived in California
for 20 years at that time, I never heard of mindfulness.
It's staggering how isolated you can be from the real world.
And I Googled it and there
was a mindfulness institute at UCLA,
and they were giving courses in meditation.
So I said, "Oh, this is great
because I can now see whether or not
the breathing part of meditation has anything
to do with the purported effects of meditation."
So I signed up for the course,
and as I joked to you before, I had two goals.
One was to see whether or not a breathing had an effect,
and the other was to levitate
because I grew up with all these Kung Fu things
and all the monks could levitate
when they meditated, so why not?
We have a motto in the lab,
you can't do anything interesting
if you're afraid of failing,
and if I fail to levitate, well, at least I tried.
And I should tell you now, I still haven't done it yet,
but I haven't given up yet. - Yet.
- Yet. I haven't given up.
So, I took this course in mindfulness
and it became apparent to me
that the breathing part was actually critical.
It wasn't simply a distraction or a focus.
They could have had you move your index finger
to the same effect.
Really we believed that the breathing part was involved.
Now, I'm not an unbiased observer so the question is,
how can I demonstrate that?
I didn't feel competent to do experiments in humans,
and I didn't feel I could design
the right experiments in humans,
but I felt maybe I can study this in rodents.
So we got this idea that we're going
to teach rodents to meditate and that's laughable,
but we said, but if we can,
then we can actually study how this happens.
So, believe it or not,
I was able to get a sort of a startup grant,
an R21 from NCCIH,
that's the National Complementary Medicine Institute.
- A wonderful institute I should mention.
Our government puts major tax dollars toward studies
of things like meditation,
breathwork, supplements, herbs, acupuncture.
This is I think not well-known,
and it's an incredible thing that our government does that,
and I think it deserves a nod and more funding [chuckles].
- I totally agree with you.
I think that it's the kind of thing that many of us,
including many scientists thinks
is to woo woo and unsubstantiated,
but we're learning more and more.
We used to laugh at neuroimmunology,
that the nervous system didn't have anything
to do with the immune system and pain itself
can influence your immune system.
I mean, there are all these things
that we're learning that we use to dismiss,
and I think there's real nuggets to be learned here.
So, they went out on a limb
and they funded this particular project.
And now I'm going to leap ahead because for three years
we threw stuff up against the wall that didn't work.
And recently we had a major breakthrough.
We found a protocol by which
we can get mice to breathe slowly,
awake mice to breathe slowly.
I won't tell you.
- Normally they don't breathe slowly.
- No, no.
In other words, whatever their normal breath is,
we could slow it down by a factor of 10
and they're fine doing that.
So, we could do that for,
we did that 30 minutes a day for four weeks, okay?
Like a breath practice.
- Do they levitate?
- We haven't measured that yet [laughs].
I would say a priority,
we haven't seen them floating to the top of that cage,
but we haven't weighed them.
Maybe they weigh less.
Maybe levitation is graded.
And so, maybe if you weigh less
it's sort of partial levitation.
In any case,
we then tested them.
And we had control animals, mice,
where we did everything the same,
except the manipulation we made
did not slow down their breathing.
So, but they went through everything else.
We then put them through a standard for air conditioning,
which we did with my colleague, Michael Fanselow,
who's one of the real gurus of fear.
And we measured a standard test is to put mice
in a condition where they're concerned
they'll receive a shock and their response
is that they freeze,
and the measure of how fearful they are
is how long they freeze.
This is well validated and it's way above my pay grade
to describe the validity of the test, but it's very valid.
The control mice had a freezing time,
which was just the same as ordinary mice would have.
The ones that went
through our protocol froze much, much less.
According to Michael,
the degree to which they showed less freezing
was as much as if there
was a major manipulation in the amygdala,
which is a part of the brain
that's important in fear processing.
It's a staggering change.
The problem we have now is the grant ran out of money,
the postdoc working on it left,
and now we have to try and piece together everything,
but the data is spectacular.
- Well, I think it's,
I'll just pause you for a moment there
because I think that the,
you're talking about a rodent study,
but I think the benefits of doing rodent study
is that you can get deep into mechanism
and for people that might think,
well, we've known that meditation has these benefits,
why do you need to get mechanistic science?
I think that one thing that's important
for people to remember is that first of all,
as many people as one might think
are meditating out there or doing breathwork,
a far, far, far, far greater number
of people are not, right?
I mean, the majority of people don't take any time
to do dedicated breathwork nor meditate.
So, whatever can incentivize people would be wonderful.
But the other thing is that it's never really been clear
to me just how much meditation
is required for a real effect, meaning a practical effect.
People say 30 minutes a day, 20 minutes a day,
once a week, twice a week, same thing with breathwork.
Finding minimum or effective thresholds
for changing neural circuitry
is what I think is the holy grail of all these practices.
And that's only going to be determined
by the sorts of mechanistic studies that you described.
So, this is wonderful.
I do hope the work gets completed and we can talk
about ways that we can ensure that that happens, but-
- But let me add one thing to what you're saying, Andrew.
One of the issues, I think for a lot of people
is that there's a placebo effect.
That is in humans, they can respond to something
even though the mechanism has nothing
to do with what the intervention is.
And so, it's easy to say that the meditative response
has a big component, which is a placebo effect.
My mice don't believe in the placebo effect.
And so, if we could show there's a bonafide effect in mice,
it is convincing in ways that no matter
how many human experiments you did,
the control for the placebo effect
is extremely difficult in humans.
In mice, it's a non-issue.
So, I think that that in of itself
would be an enormous message to send.
- Excellent, and indeed, a better point.
I think a 30 minute a day meditation in these mice,
if I understand correctly, the meditation,
we don't know what they're thinking about, but-
- Well, it's breath practice really.
- Right, so it's breath practice.
So, because presumably they're not thinking
about their third eye center,
lotus position, levitation, whatever it is.
They're not instructed as to what to do,
and if they were, they probably wouldn't do it anyway.
So, 30 minutes a day in which breathing
is deliberately slowed or is slowed relative
to their normal patterns of breathing.
Got it.
What was the frequency of sighing during that 30 minutes?
Unclear? - We don't know yet.
- Oh. - Well, no, we have the data.
We just, we're analyzing that data.
- To be determined, or to be announced at some point.
So, the fear centers are altered in some way
that creates a shorter fear response to a foot shock.
- [Jack] Right.
- What are some other examples
that you are aware of from work in your laboratory
or work in other laboratories for that matter
about interactions between breathing
and brain state or emotional state?
- So, this goes back to our prior conversation.
I sort of went off on a tangent.
I think we need to think separately of the effect
of volitional changes of breathing on emotion
versus the effect of brain state on breathing.
So, the effect of brain state on breathing
like when you're stressed is a affect,
presumably originating in higher centers
if I can use that term affecting breathing.
It's the reciprocal is that when you change breathing,
it affects your emotional state.
I think of those two things as different
and they're ultimately tied together.
So, there's a landmark paper published in the '50s
where they stimulated in the amygdala of cats,
and depending on where they stimulated,
they got profound changes in breathing.
There's like every pattern of breathing
could possibly imagine,
they found the site in the amygdala,
which could produce that.
So, there's clearly a powerful descending effect coming
from the amygdala which is a major site
for processing emotion, fear, stress and whatnot
that can affect breathing.
And clearly we have volitional control over breathing.
So, we have profound effects there.
Now, I should say about emotional control of breathing,
I need to segue into talking about locked-in syndrome.
Locked-in syndrome is a devastating lesion
that happens in a part of the brainstem
where signals that controlled muscles are transmitted.
So the fibers coming from your motor cortex
go down to this part of the brainstem,
which is called the ventral pons.
And if there's a stroke there, it can damage these pathways.
What happens in individuals who have locked-in syndrome
is they lose all volitional movement
except lateral movement of the eyes
and maybe the ability to blink.
The reason they're able to still blink and move their eyes
is that those control centers are rostal, closer to,
are not interrupted.
In other words, the interruption is below that.
They continue to breathe because the centers
for breathing don't require that volitional command.
In any case, they're below that, so they're fine.
So, these people continue to breathe.
Normal intelligence, but they can't move.
There's a great book called
"The Diving Bell and the Butterfly"
about a young man who this happens to,
and he describes his life
and it's a real testament
to the human condition that he does this.
It's a remarkable book, it's a short book.
- Did he write the book by blinking?
Did they translate it?
- He did it by blinking to his caretaker.
It's pretty amazing.
And there was a movie which I've never seen
with Javier Bardem as the protagonist,
but the book I highly recommend this to anyone to read.
So, I had colleagues studying an individual
that had locked-in syndrome and they,
this patient breathed very robotically,
totally consistent, very regular.
They gave the patient a low oxygen mixture to breathe.
Ventilation went up, a CO2 mixture to breathe,
ventilation went up.
So, all the regulatory apparatus for breathing was there.
They asked the patient
to hold his breath or to breathe faster [blows raspberry].
Nothing happened.
Just the patient recognized the command,
but couldn't change it.
Then all of a sudden,
the patient's breathing changed considerably,
and they said to their patient, "What happened?"
They said, "You told a joke and I laughed."
And they went back and whenever they told a joke
that the patient found funny,
the patient's breathing pattern changed.
And you know your breathing pattern when you laugh
is [inhales] you inhale, you go ha, ha, ha, ha.
But it's also very distinctive.
We have some neuroscience colleagues who will go un-named,
who, if you heard them laugh 50 yards away,
you know exactly who they are.
- Yeah, well, I'll name him.
Eric Kandel, - For one.
- has an inspiratory laugh.
He's famous for a [inhaling],
as opposed to a ha, ha.
- Exactly, exactly. - Yeah.
So, it's very stereotyped, but it's maintained
and these people lose volitional control of breathing.
So, there's an emotive component controlling your breathing,
which has nothing to do
with your volitional control,
and it goes down to a different pathway
because it's not disrupted by this locked-in syndrome.
If you look at motor control of the face,
we have the volitional control of the face,
but we also have emotional control of the face,
which most of us can't control.
So, when we look at another person,
we're able to read a lot about
what their emotional state is,
and that's a lot about how primates communicate,
humans communicate and you have people
who are good deceivers.
Probably used car salesman, poker players.
Now poker players have tells,
but many of them now wear dark glasses
because a lot of the tells you blink or whatnot.
- Pupil sizes and stuff. - Pupil size.
Pupil size is a tell, which is an autonomic function,
not a skeletal muscle function,
but we have all these skeletal muscles,
which we're controlling, which give us away.
I've tried to get my imaging friends
to image some of the great actors
that we have in Los Angeles.
- You mean brain imagers.
- Brain imagers, I'm sorry. - Yeah. No, that's all right.
- I mean, yeah, no, brain imagers.
Because I think when I ask you to smile,
I could tell that you're not happy
that you're smiling because I asked you to smile.
I think that you're- - I thought you were
about to crack a joke,
but we're old friends, so, yeah.
- No, I'm not...
When you see a picture like at a birthday or whatnot,
and say cheese, you could tell
that at least half of the people
are not happy to saying cheese,
whereas a great actor when they're able
to dissemble and the fact that they're sad
or they're happy, you believe that they're not faking it.
It's like, that's great acting.
And I don't think everyone could do that.
I think that the individuals who are able to do that
have some connection to the parts
of their motive control system
that the rest of us don't have.
Maybe they develop it through training and maybe not,
but I think that this can be imaged
so I would like to get one of these great actors
in a imager and have them go through that
and then get a normal person,
and see whether or not they can emulate that
and I think you're going to find big differences
in the way they control this emotive thing.
So, this emotive control of the facial muscles,
I think is in large part,
similar to the emotive control of breathing.
So, there's that emotive control,
and there's that volitional control and they're different.
They're different.
Now, you asked me about the Yackle stuff.
The Yackle paper had to do with ascending,
that the effect of breathing on emotion.
What Kevin found was that there was a population
of neurons in the pre-Botzinger complex
that we're always looking at the things
that are projecting ultimately on motor neurons.
He found the population of cells
that projected to locus coeruleus.
Locus coeruleus, excuse me,
is one of those places in the brain
that seems to go everywhere.
- It's like a sprinkler system.
- Exactly, exactly.
And influence mood, and you've had podcasts about this.
I mean, there's a lot of stuff going on with the amygdala,
so, excuse me, the locus coeruleus.
So you get into the locus coeruleus,
you can now spray information
out throughout the entire brain.
He found specific cells that projected
from pre-Botzinger to locus coeruleus,
and that these cells are inspiratory modulated.
Now, it's been known for a long time since the '60s
that if you look in the locus coeruleus of cats
when they're awake,
you find many neurons that have respiratory modulation.
No one paid much attention.
Why bother?
Not why bother paying attention,
but why would the brain bother to have these inputs?
So, what Kevin did with Lindsey Schwarz
in Liqun Luo's lab,
is they killed, ablated,
those cells going to locus coeruleus
from pre-Botzinger and the animals became calmer,
and their EEG levels changed in ways
that are indicative that they became calmer.
- And as I recall, they didn't just become calmer,
but they weren't really capable of high arousal states.
They were kind of flat.
- I don't think we really pursued that in the paper.
And so, we'd have to ask John Huguenard about that,
but I- - He's on the other side
of my lab so we'll ask him.
But nonetheless,
that beautifully illustrates
how there is a bi-directional control, right?
Of emotion- - Well, that's ascending.
- Well, no, the two stories of the locked-in syndrome,
plus the Yackle paper shows
that emotional states influence breathing
and breathing influences emotional states,
which, but you mentioned inspiration,
which I always call inhalation, but people will follow.
No, that's fine.
Those are interchangeable.
People can follow that.
There's some interesting papers from Noam Sobel's group
and from a number of other groups
that as we inhale or right after we inhale [inhales],
the brain is actually more alert and capable
of storing information than during exhales,
which I find incredible but it also makes sense.
I'm able to see things far better when my eyes are open
than when my eyelids are closed, for that matter.
- Maybe, right?
I mean, I don't doubt, Noam's work is great.
Let me backtrack a bit because I want people
to understand that when we're talking
about breathing affecting emotional cognitive state,
it's not simply coming from pre-Botzinger.
There are at least, well, there are several other sites
and let me sort of describe,
I need to sort of go through that.
One is olfaction.
So, when you're breathing, normal breathing,
you're inhaling and exhaling.
This is creating signals coming from the nasal mucosa
that is going back into the olfactory bulb.
That's respiratory modulating.
And the olfactory bulb has a profound influence
and projections through many parts of the brain.
So, there's a signal arising from this rhythmic moving
of air in and out of the nose
that's going into the brain that has contained in it
a respiratory modulation.
So, that signal is there.
The brain doesn't have to be using it,
but when it's the discriminating owner and whatnot,
that's riding on a oscillation,
which is respiratory related.
Another potential source is the vagus nerve.
The vagus nerve is a major nerve,
which is containing efferents from all of the viscera.
- Efferents just being- - A signal.
- Signals to. - Yes.
Signals from the viscera.
It also has signals coming from the brainstem down,
which are called efferents,
but it's getting major signals from the lung, from the gut.
And this is going up into the brain stem.
So, it's there.
There are very powerful receptors in the lung
that are responding to the lung volume.
The lungs stretch.
- So, bareovers?
Oh, sorry.
We have a number of, - They're pressure receptors.
- Like the PA0 receptors of this year's Nobel Prize, yeah.
- Yeah.
So, they're responding to the expansion
and relaxation in the lung.
And so, if you record from the vagus nerve,
you'll see that there's a huge respiratory modulation due
to the mechanical changes in the lung.
Now, why that is of interest is that
for some forms of refractory depression,
electrical stimulation of the vagus nerve
can provide tremendous relief.
Why this is the case still remains to be determined,
but it's clear that signals in the vagus nerve,
at least artificial signals in the vagus nerve
can have a positive effect on reducing depression.
So, it's not a leap to think
that under normal circumstances,
that that rhythm coming in from the vagus nerve
is playing a role in normal processing.
Okay, let me continue.
Carbon dioxide and oxygen levels.
Now, under normal circumstances,
your oxygen levels are fine.
And unless you go to altitude,
they don't really change very much,
but your CO2 levels can change quite a bit
with even a relatively small change
in your overall breathing.
That's going to change your pH level.
I have a colleague, Alicia Maurette,
who has working with patients
who are anxious and many of them hyperventilate.
And as a result of that hyperventilation,
their carbon dioxide levels are low.
And she has developed a therapeutic treatment
where she trains these people
to breathe slower and to restore
their CO2 levels back to normal,
and she gets relief in their anxiety.
So, CO2 levels, which are not going to affect brain function
on a breath by breath level,
although it does fluctuate breath by breath,
but it's sort of this continuous background can change,
and if it's changed chronically,
we know that highly elevated levels of CO2
can produce panic attacks.
And we don't know the degree to that gets exacerbated
by people who have a panic attack,
the agree to which their ambient CO2 levels
are affecting their degree of discomfort.
- What about people who are, tend to be too calm,
meaning that they're feeling sleepy,
they're under breathing as opposed to over-breathing?
Is there any knowledge of what the status
of CO2 is in their system?
- I don't know which doesn't mean there's no knowledge,
but I'm unaware but that's blissfully unaware.
I've not looked at that literature, so I don't know.
- And I have a feeling, I mean, most people, or excuse me,
most of the scientific literature around breathing
in humans that I'm aware of relates
to these stressed states because they're
a little bit easier to study in the lab,
whereas people feeling under-stimulated
or exhausted all the time,
it's a complicated thing to measure.
I mean, you can do it, but it's not as-
- Well, CO2's easy to measure.
- But in terms of sort of the measures for feeling fatigue,
they're somewhat indirect,
whereas stress we can get a pulse rates
in HRV and things of that sort.
- Well, I'd imagine that these devices
that we're all wearing will soon be able to measure,
well, now they can measure oxygen levels,
oxygen saturation. - Just amazing.
- Yeah, but oxygen will pretty much stay
above 90% unless there's some pathology
or you go to altitude.
But CO2 levels vary quite a bit and in fact,
because they vary, your body is so sensitive,
the control of breathing,
like how much you breathe per minute
is determined in a very sensitive way
by the CO2 level.
So, even a small change in your CO2
will have a significant effect on your ventilation.
So, this is another thing
that not only changes the ventilation,
but affects your brain state.
Now, another thing that could affect breathing,
or how breathing practice can affect
your emotional state is simply descending command
because breathing practice involves volitional control
of your breathing,
and therefore there's a signal
that's originating somewhere in your motor cortex.
That is not, of course,
that's going to go down to pre-Botzinger,
but it's also going to send off collaterals to other places.
Those collaterals could obviously influence
your emotional state.
So, we have quite a few different potential sources.
None of them that are exclusive.
There's an interesting paper,
which shows that if you block nasal breathing,
you still don't see breathing
related oscillations in the brain.
And this is where I think the mechanism
is occurring is that these breathing
related oscillations in the brain,
they are playing a role in signal processing.
And maybe, should I talk a little bit about the role
that oscillations may be playing in signal processing?
- Definitely, but before you do,
I just want to ask you a intermediate question.
We've talked a lot about inhalation,
inspiration and exhalation.
What about breath holds?
In apnea, for instance, people are holding their breath,
whether or not it's conscious or unconscious,
they're holding their breath.
What's known about breath holds in terms
of how it might interact with brain state or oxygen CO2,
and I'm particularly interested in how breath holds
with lungs empty versus breath holds
with lungs full might differ
in terms of their impact on the brain.
I'm not aware of any studies on this looking
at a mechanistic level,
but I find it really interesting
and even if there are no studies,
I'd love if you care to speculate.
- Well, one of the breath practices
that intrigued me is where you basically hyperventilate
for a minute and then hold your breath
for as long as you can.
- Tummo style, - Yeah, brief shots of air.
- Wim Hof style or,
we call it in the laboratory,
because frankly before Tummo, and before Wim,
it was referred to as cyclic hyperventilation.
So, it was basically [panting], right?
Followed by a breath hold and that breath hold
could be done with lungs full or lungs empty.
- Right, yeah.
So, I had a long talk with some colleagues
about what they might think inline mechanisms are,
particularly for the breath hold.
And I certainly envisioned that there's
a component with respect to the presence
or absence of that rhythmicity in your cortex,
which is having effect.
But the other thing with the hyperventilation,
hypoventilation or the apnea
is your CO2 levels are going from low to high.
- Anytime you're holding your breath.
- Anytime you hold your breath, okay?
And those are going to have a profound influence.
Now, I have to talk about episodic hypoxia
because there's a lot of work going on
particularly with Gordon Mitchell
at the University of Florida
is doing some extraordinary work on episodic hypoxia.
So, in the '80s,
David Millhorn did some really intriguing work.
If I ask you to hold your breath, excuse me.
If I gave you a low oxygen mixture for a couple of minutes,
your breathing level would go up 'cause you want to
have more oxygen. - You're starving for air.
- [Andrew and Jack] Yeah.
- No, you're starving for oxygen.
- All right. - Okay?
And for a couple of minutes, you'd go up.
You can reach some steady state level.
Not so hypoxic that you can't reach an equilibrium.
And then I give you room air again,
the ventilation quickly relaxes back down to normal.
If on the other hand, I gave you three minutes of hypoxia,
five minutes of normoxia, three minutes of hypoxia,
five minutes of normoxia, three minutes of hypoxia,
five minutes in normoxia-
- Normoxia being normal air. - Normal air.
Your ventilation goes up, down,
up, down, up, down, up, down.
After the last episode,
your breathing comes down and doesn't continue to come down,
but rises again and stays up for hours, okay?
This is well validated now.
This was originally done in animals,
but in humans all the time,
it seems to have profound benefit
on motor function and cognitive function.
- In what direction?
- Positive, positive.
I've often toyed with the idea of getting an 8% oxygen,
don't do this, listeners,
getting an 8% oxygen tank by my desk
when I'm writing a grant and doing like in "Blue Velvet"
and going through the episodic hypoxia
to improve my cognitive functioning,
'cause certainly could use improvement
when I'm writing grants.
- But you could do this without the low oxygen.
I mean, you could do this through breathwork, presumably?
- It's hard to lower your oxygen enough.
Okay?
In the experimental studies,
they typically use 8% oxygen.
It's hard to hold your breath long enough.
And there is another difference here,
that is what's happening to your CO2 levels.
When you hold your breath,
your oxygen levels are dropping,
your CO2 levels are going up.
When you're doing episodic hypoxia,
your CO2 levels are going to stay pretty normal
because you're still breathing,
it's just the oxygen levels are gone.
- So, unlike normal conditions, which you described before,
where oxygen is relatively constant and CO2
is fluctuating depending on emotional state
and activity and things of that sort,
in episodic hypoxia, CO2 is relatively constant,
but you're varying the oxygen level coming
into the system quite a bit.
- I would say it's relatively,
I would say CO2 is relatively constant,
but it's not going to go in a direction
which is going to be significantly far from normal.
Whereas when you're holding your breath,
you're going to become both hypoxic
and hypercapnic at the same time.
- We should explain to people what hypoxic
and hypercapnic are because we
haven't done it. - Okay, hypoxic is just
the technical term for low levels of oxygen,
hyper, or you could hypoxic, low, hyper is high.
So, hyperoxia or hypocapnia, low CO2 or hypercapnia,
your highest levels of CO2.
So, when you're, in episodic hypoxia, if anything,
you're going to become hypocapnic, not hypercapnic.
And that could play an influence in this.
One example that I remember,
and Gordon will have to forgive me if I'm misquoting this,
is they had a patient who had a stroke
and had weakness and ankle flection.
That is, excuse me, ankle extension, to extend the ankle.
And so, they had the patient
in a seat where they can measure ankle extension,
and then they measured it
and then they exposed the patient
to episodic hypoxia and they measured again,
the strength of the ankle extension went way up.
And so, Gordon is looking at this,
they're looking at this now for spinal cord rehab.
- And I imagine for all sorts of neuromuscular performance,
it could be beneficial.
- Gordon is looking into athletic performance.
We have a project which we haven't been able
to push to the next level to do golf.
So, I find- - Why golf?
'Cause you love golf?
- Well, it's because it's motor performance coordination.
So, it's not simply running as fast as you can.
It's coordination, it's concentration,
it's a whole variety of things.
And so, the idea would be to get a group of golfers
and give them the placebo control,
so they don't know whether they're breathing a gas mixture,
which is just normal air or a hypoxic gas mixture,
although they may be able to figure
it out based on their response.
Do it under controlled circumstances that do it into a net,
measure their length of their drives,
their dispersion and whatnot,
and see what happens.
Look, if we could find that this works for golfers,
forget about cognitive function.
We could sell this for unbelievable amounts of money.
- That sounds like a terrible idea.
[Jack laughs]
- By the way, I'm not serious about selling it, but-
- I know you're joking.
I mean, maybe people should know
that you are joking about that.
No, I think that anything that can improve cognitive
and neuromuscular performance is going to be of interest
for a wide range of both pathologic states
like injury, TBI, et cetera.
I mean, one of the most frequent questions
I get is about recovery from concussion
or traumatic brain injury.
A lot of people think sports, they think football,
they think rugby, they think hockey.
But if you look at the statistics on traumatic brain injury,
most of it is construction workers,
car crashes, bicycle accidents.
I mean it, the sports part of it is a tiny,
tiny minuscule fraction of the total amount
of traumatic brain injury out there.
I think these protocols tested in the context of golf
would be very interesting because of the constraints
of the measures as you mentioned,
and it could be exported to a number of things.
I want to just try and imagine whether or not
there is any kind of breathing pattern or breathwork,
just to be direct about it,
that even partially mimics what you described
in terms of episodic hypoxia.
I've done a lot of Tummo,
Wim Hof cyclic hyperventilation type breathing before.
My lab studies this in humans,
and what we find is that
if people do cyclic hyperventilation,
so for about a minute, then exhale,
hold their breath for 15 to 60 seconds,
depending on what they can do,
and just keep repeating that for about five minutes,
it seems to me that it at least partially mimics
the state that you're talking about
because afterwards people report heightened levels
of alertness, lower levels of kind of triggering due
to stressful events.
They feel comfortable at a higher level
of autonomic arousal, cognitive focus,
a number of improvements that are pretty impressive
that any practitioner of Wim Hof or Tummo
will be familiar with.
Is that pattern of breathing even,
can we say that it maps to what you're describing
in some general sense?
- Well, the expert in this would be Gordon Mitchell.
I would say it moves in that direction,
but it's not as extreme because I don't think
you can get down to the levels
of hypoxia that they do clinically.
I know that our pals at Our Breath Collective actually
just bought a machine because you buy
a machine that does this.
- [Andrew] I see.
- And they bought it and they're going to
do their own self testing to see whether or not
this has any effect on anything that they can measure.
Of course, you have to be concerned
about self-experimentation,
but I applaud their curiosity and going after it.
- Hyperbaric chambers.
I hear a lot nowadays about hyperbaric chambers.
People are buying 'em and using 'em,
and what are your thoughts on hyperbaric chambers
as it relates to any of the-
- Hyper or hypo?
- Hyperbaric chambers.
- Oh, so you're not talkin' about altitude?
- [Andrew] No.
- I don't really have much to say.
I mean, your oxygen levels would probably go up a little bit
and that could have a beneficial effect,
but that's way outside my area of comfort.
- 2022 I think is going to be the year of two things
I keep hearing a lot about,
which is the deliberate use of high salt intake
for performance increasing blood volume, et cetera,
and hyperbaric chambers seem to be catching on much
in the same way that ice baths were
and saunas seemed to be right now but anyway,
a prediction we can return to at some point.
I want to ask you about some of the studies
that I've seen out there exploring
how deliberately restricting one's breathing
to nasal breathing can do things like improve memory.
There's a couple of papers in "Journal of Neuroscience,"
which is a respectable journal in our field,
one looking at olfactory memory.
So, that kind of made sense because you can smell
things better through your nose than your mouth,
unless you're some sort of elk or something where they can,
presumably they have some sense
of smell in their mouth as well.
But humans generally smell with their nose.
That wasn't terribly surprising,
but there was a companion study that showed
that the hippocampus, an area involved in encoding memories
in one form or another was more active if you will
and memory and recall was better
when people learned information while nasal breathing,
as opposed to mouth breathing.
Does that make sense from any mechanistic perspective?
- Well, given that there's a major pathway going
from the olfactory system into the brain
and you cut that and not one
from any receptors in the mouth,
the degree of respiratory modulation
you're going to see throughout the forebrain
is going to be less with mouth breathing
than nose breathing.
So, it's certainly plausible.
I think there are a lot of experiments
that need to be done to distinguish
between the two that is the nasal component
and the non nasal component
of these breathing related signals.
But there's a tendency sometimes when you have
a strong effect to be exclusive,
and I think what's going on here is that there
are many inputs that can have an effect.
Now, whether they're parceled,
that some effect this part of behavior
and some effect that part of behavior remains
to be investigated.
There's certainly a strong olfactory component.
My interest is trying to follow the central component
'cause we know that there's
a strong central component in this.
In fact, there's a strong central projection
to the olfactory bulb.
So, regardless of whether or not there's any air flowing
in and out of the nose,
there's a respiratory input into the olfactory bulb,
which combines with the respiratory modulated signals
coming from the sensory receptors.
- Interesting.
And as long as we are poking around, forgive the pun,
the nose, what about one nostril versus the other nostril?
I know it sounds a little crazy to imagine,
but there have been theories in yoga traditions
and others that breathing through one nostril
somehow activates certain brain centers,
maybe hemispherically one side of the brain versus the other
or that right nostril and left nostril breathing
might differ in terms of the levels
of alertness or calmness they produce.
I'm not aware of any mechanistic data on that,
but if there's anything worthwhile
about right nostril versus left nostril breathing
that you're aware of, I'd love to know.
- Well, it certainly plausible.
I don't know of any data demonstrating it,
except the anecdotal reports of the,
as you know the brain is highly lateralized
and we have speech on one side
and a dominant hand that's on one side.
And so, the notion that if you have this huge signal coming
from the olfactory system and it,
to some degree is lateralized,
is not perfectly symmetrical.
That is one side is not going evenly to both sides,
then you can imagine that once
the signal gets distributed in a way that's not uniform,
that the effectiveness or the response
is going to be particular to the cortex
in which either the signal still remains
or the signal is removed from.
- I see.
What are some of the other features of our brain and body,
be it blinking, or eye movements,
or ability to encode sounds,
or any features of the way that we function
and move and perceive things
that are coordinated with breathing in some interesting way?
- Thank you for that question.
Almost everything.
So, we have, for example, on the autonomic side,
we have respiratory sinus arrhythmia.
That is during expiration the heart slows down.
Your pupils oscillate with the respiratory cycle.
I don't know what the functional basis for that is,
but they do oscillate with the respiratory cycle.
- When we inhale our pupils constrict,
presumably 'cause you there's an increase
in heart rate and sympathetic tone,
I would think of constriction
and I'm guessing as you relax the pupil will get,
and you exhale the pupil will get bigger-
- I think you're right,
but I always get the valance of that-
- Yeah, well, it's counterintuitive
because people wouldn't think that when the pupils get,
I mean, it depends.
I mean, well, you can get very alert and aroused
in that for stress or for good reasons,
and the peoples get wider,
but your visual field narrows and then the opposite is true.
Anyway, I guess the idea is that the pupils
are changing size and therefore the aperture
of your visual window is changing
in coordination with breathing.
- Okay.
Your fear response changes with the respiratory cycle.
- Can you tell us more about that?
- Well, there's a paper by Solano,
which I think showed rather clearly
that if you show individuals fearful faces
that their measured response of fearfulness changes
between inspiration and expiration.
I don't know why, but it does.
Your reaction time changes.
So you talk about blinking.
The reaction time changes
between inspiration and expiration.
If I ask you to punch something that time
will change between inspiration and expiration.
In fact, I don't know the degree
to which martial artists exploit that.
You watch the breathing pattern and your opponent
will actually move slower
during one cycle compared to the other.
- Meaning as they're, in which direction?
If they're exhaling, they can punch faster?
- I have to say, I don't keep a table of which
is which direction things move in
'cause I'm out of the martial arts field now.
- My vague understanding is that exhales on strikes
is the more typical way to do that,
and so as people strike, they exhale.
In many- - As you exhale,
but there are other components to striking
because you want to stiffen your rib cage,
you want to make a Valsalva maneuver.
So that's both an inspiration and expiration.
It's at the same time.
So, I don't know enough about,
when you say during expiration,
I would assume that when you make your strike,
you're actually sort of wanting to stiffen here,
which is a Valsalva like maneuver.
- And oftentimes they'll clench
their fist at the last moment.
Anyway, there's a whole set of motor things
that we can talk to some fighters.
We know people who know fighters, so what we can ask them.
Interesting.
What are some other things that are modulated by breathing?
- I think anything anyone looks at seems
to have a breathing component
because it's all over your brain,
and it's hard to imagine it not being effective.
Now, whether it's incidental or just background
and doesn't really have
any behavioral advantage is possible.
In other cases, it might have a behavioral advantage.
I mean, the big, this eye-opening thing
for me probably a decade ago,
was digging into literature
and seeing how much of cortical activity
and subcortical activity
had a respiratory modulator component to it.
And I think a lot of my colleagues who were studying cortex
are oblivious to this, and they find,
I heard a talk the other day
from a person who will go un-named,
who found a lot of things correlated
with a particular movement.
And I think it all, when I looked, I said, gee,
that's the list of things that are respiratory modulated.
And rather than it being correlated
to the movement they we're looking at,
I think the movement they were looking at
was modulated by breathing, as was everything else.
So, there wasn't that the movement itself
was driving that correlation.
It was that they were all correlated to something else,
which is the breathing movement
and whether or not that is behaviorally relevant,
or behaviorally something you can exploit, I don't know.
- I suspect you're right,
that breathing is if not the foundational driver of many,
if not all of these things
that it's at least one of the foundational drivers.
- It's in the background, it's in the brain,
and oscillations play an important part in brain function.
And they vary in frequency from maybe 100 hertz down to,
well, we can get to circadian and sort of monthly cycles,
but breathing occupies
a rather unusual place in all that because...
So, let me talk about what people think
the oscillation's is doing, particularly faster ones.
They're important in coordinating signals across neurons.
Just like in a computer, a computer steps.
So, a computer knows when information is coming
from another part of a computer
so that it was originated at a particular time.
And so, that the screen by step-by-step thing
is important in computer control.
Now, the brain is not a digital device,
and it's an analog device,
but when I have a signal that's coming in my ear and my eye,
which is Andrew Huberman speaking
and I'm looking at his face, I see that as a whole,
but the signal is coming into different parts of my brain.
How do I unify that?
Well, my neurons are very sensitive to changes
in signals arriving by fractions of a millisecond.
So, I know we're sure that those signals
coming in represent the same signal.
Well, if I have throughout my brain
and isolation and the signals ride on that oscillation,
let's say the peak of the oscillation,
I can then have a much better handle
on the road of timing and say,
"Those two signals came in at the same time.
They may relate to the same object and ah ha,
I see you as one unified thing spouting talking."
And so, these oscillations come
in many different frequency ranges
and are important in memory formation
and all sorts of things.
I don't think people pay much attention to breathing
because it's relatively slow to the range
when you think about milliseconds,
but we have important things that are thought
to be important in cognitive function,
which are a few cycles per second
to 20, 30, 40, 50 cycles per second.
Breathing in humans is maybe .2 cycles per second,
every five seconds.
Although in rodents they're up to four per second,
which is pretty fast.
So, but breathing has one thing which is special,
that is you can readily change it.
So, the degree to which the brain is using
that slow signal for anything,
if that becomes part of its normal signal processing,
you now change it.
That signal processing has to change.
And as that signal processing changes,
acutely there's a change.
So, you asked about breath practice,
how long do you have to do it?
Well, a single breath will change your state.
You're nervous, you take a deep breath
and it seems to help relax,
so- - Or a sigh.
- Call it what you will, call it what you will.
It seems to work.
Now, it doesn't have a permanent change,
but when I'm getting up to bat
or getting up to the first tee or getting
to give a big talk or coming to do a podcast,
I get a little bit anxious, a deep breath,
or a few deep breaths are tremendously effective
in calming one down.
And so, you can get a transient disruption,
but on the other hand, let's take something like depression.
I think it's, you can envision depression
as activities sort of going around in a circuit.
And because it's continuous in the nervous system,
as signals keep repeating,
they tend to get stronger and they can get so strong,
you can't break them.
So, you can imagine the depression
being something going on and on and on,
and you can't break it.
And so, we have trouble
when we get for certain levels of depression,
I mean, all of us get depressed at some point,
but if it's not continuous, it's not long lasting,
we're able to break it.
But if it's long lasting and very deep, we can't break it.
So, the question is how do we break it?
Well, there are extreme measures to break it.
We could do electroconvulsive shock.
We shock the whole brain.
That's disrupting activity in the whole brain.
And when this circuit starts to get back together again,
it's been disruptive.
And we know that the brain,
when signals get disrupted a little bit,
we can weaken the connections and weakening
the connections of this then
in this circuit involved in depression,
we may get some relief and electroconvulsive shock
does work for relieving many kinds of depression.
That's pretty heroic.
Focal deep brain stimulation does the same thing,
but more localized or transcranial stimulation.
You're disrupting a network.
And while it's getting back together,
it may weaken some of the connections.
If breathing is playing some role in this circuit,
and now, instead of doing like a one second shock,
I do 30 minutes of disruption by doing slow breathing
or other breathing practice,
those circuits begin to break down a little bit,
and I get some relief.
And if I continue to do it before the circuit
can then build back up again,
I gradually can wear that circuit down.
I sort of liken this,
I tell people it's like walking around on a dirt path.
You build a rut, the rut gets so deep
you can't get out of it.
And what breathing is doing is sort of filling
in the right rut bit by bit
to the point that you can climb out of that rut.
And that is because the breathing signal
is playing some role in the way the circuit works,
and then when you disrupt that,
the circuit gets a little thrown off kilter,
and as you know when circuits get thrown off,
the nervous system tries to adjust in some way or another,
and it turns out at least for breathing
for some evolutionary reason, or just by happenstance,
it seems to improve our emotional function,
and our cognitive function.
And we're very fortunate that that's the case.
- It's a terrific segue into what I want to ask you next,
and this is part of a set of questions
I want to make sure we touch on before we wrap up,
which is what do you do with all this knowledge
in terms of a breathing practice?
You mentioned that one breath can shift
your brain state and that itself can be powerful.
I think that's absolutely true.
You've also talked about 30 minute breathwork practices,
which is 30 minutes of breathwork,
is a pretty serious commitment I think, but it's doable.
Certainly a zero cost,
except for the time in most cases.
What do you see out there in the landscape of breathwork
that's being done that you like, and why do you like it?
What do you think you,
or what would you like to see more of
in terms of exploration of breathwork and what do you do?
- Well, I'm a relatively new convert to breathwork.
Through my own investigation of it,
I became convinced that it's real,
and I'm basically a beginner in terms of my own practice.
And I like to keep things simple,
and I think I've discussed this before.
I liken it to someone who's a couch potato
who was told they got to begin to exercise.
You don't go out and run a marathon.
So, couch potato, you say,
okay, get up and walk for five minutes and 10 minutes.
And then, okay, now you're walking for a longer period.
You'll begin to run, and then you reach a point,
you say, well, gee, I'm interested in this sport.
And there may be particular kinds of practices
that you can use that could be helpful
in optimizing performance of that sport.
I'm not there yet.
I find I get tremendous benefit
by relatively short periods
between five and maybe 20 minutes
of doing box breathing.
It's very simple to do.
I have a simple app, which helps me keep the timing.
- Do you recall which app it is?
Is it the Apnea Trainer?
Is that the one?
- Well, I was using Calm for a long time,
but I let my subscription lapse
and I have another one whose name I don't remember but it's,
so it's very simple and it works for me.
I'm now trying this Tummo, because I'm just curious
and exploring it because it may be acting
for a different way and I want to see
if I respond differently.
So, I don't have a particular point of view.
Now, I have friends and colleagues
who are into particular styles like Wim Hof.
And I think what he's doing is great
and getting people who are interested.
I think the notion is that I would like
to see more people exploring this and to some degree,
as you point out, 30 minutes a day,
some of the breath patterns that some of these stars
like Wim Hof are a little intimidating to newbies.
And so, I would like to see something very simple,
that what I tell my friends is, look,
just try it five or 10 minutes.
See if you feel better, do it for a few days.
If you don't like it, stop it, it doesn't cost anything.
And invariably, they find that it's helpful.
I will often interrupt my day
to take five or 10 minutes.
Like, if I find that I'm lagging...
I think there's some pretty good data
that your performance after lunch declines.
And so, very often what I'll do after lunch,
which I didn't do today is take five or 10 minutes
and just sort of breath practice.
- And lately, what does that breath practice look like?
- It's just box breathing for five to 10 minutes.
- And the duration of your inhales and holds
and exhales and holds is set by the app?
Is that right?
- Well, I do five seconds.
- So, five seconds inhale, five second hold,
five second, exhale, five second hold.
- Yeah, and sometimes I'll do doubles.
I'll do 10 seconds just because I get bored.
It's just, I feel like doing it and it's very helpful.
I mean...
Now, that's not the only thing I do
with respect to trying to maintain my sanity and my health.
- No, I can imagine there'd be a number of things,
although, because you seem very sane and very healthy,
I in fact, know that you are both of those things.
- Right, you suspect that I am.
- I suspect that there's data.
Awhile back we had a conversation, a casual conversation,
but you said something that really stuck in my mind,
which is that it might be
that the specific pattern of breathwork
that one does is not as important
as experiencing transitions between states based
on deliberate breathwork or something to that extent,
which I interpreted to mean that if I were
to do box breathing with five second in,
five seconds hold, five second exhale,
five second hold for a couple of days,
or maybe even a couple of minutes and then switch
to 10 seconds or then switch to Tummo,
that there's something powerful perhaps
in the transitions and realizing the relationship
between different patterns of breathing
in those transitions,
much in the same way that you can get
into one of these cars at an amusement park
that just goes at a constant rate and then stops.
Very different than learning how to shift gears.
I used to drive a manual.
I still can so I'm thinking about a manual transmission,
but even with an automatic transmission,
you start to get a sense of how the vehicle behaves
under different conditions.
And I thought that was a beautiful seed
for a potential breathwork practice
that at least to my awareness,
nobody has really formalized,
which is that you introduce some variability
within the practice that's somewhat random
in order to be able to sense the relationship
between different speeds and depths of inhales,
exhales and holds and so forth.
And essentially, it's like driving around the track,
but with obstacles at different rates
and breaking and restarting and things of that sort,
that's how you learn how to drive.
What do you think about that and if you like it enough,
can we call it the Feldman protocol?
- Oh, please [laughs].
I was asked in this BBC interview once why
didn't I name it the Feldman complex,
instead of pre-Botzinger complex?
- You said I already have a Feldman complex.
- Well, it sounds like a psychiatric disorder,
but I think the primary effect is this disruptive effect,
which I described,
but the particular responses may clearly vary depending
on what that disruption is.
I don't know of any particular data,
which are as in well controlled experiments,
which can actually work through the different types
of breathing patterns or simply with a box pattern,
just varying the durations.
I mean, pranayama is sort of similar,
but the amount of time you spend
going around the box is different.
So, I don't really have much to say about this.
I mean, this is why we need better controlled experiments
in humans and I think this is where being able
to study in rodents where you can have
a wide range of perturbations
while you're doing more invasive studies
to really get down as to which regions are affected,
how was the signal processing disrupted,
which is still a hypothesis,
but how it's disrupted could tell us a lot
about maybe there's a resonant point
at which there's an optimal effect
when you take a particular breathing practice.
And then when we talked about the fact
that different breathing practices
could be affecting the outcomes
through different pathways.
You have the olfactory pathway, you have a central pathway,
you have a vagal pathway, you have a descending pathway,
how different practices may change
the summation of those things
because I think all those things are probably involved,
and we're just beginning to scratch the surface.
And I just hope that we can get serious neuroscientists
and psychologists to do the right experiments
to get at this because I think
there's a lot of value to human health here.
- I do too, and it's one of the reasons my lab
has shifted to these sorts of things in humans.
I'm delighted that you're continuing
to do the hardcore mechanistic work in mice
and probably do work in humans already as well,
if you're not already.
And there are other groups, Epel Lab at UCF,
and a number of, I'm starting to see some papers out there
about respiration in humans a little bit,
some more brain imaging.
I can't help but ask about a somewhat unrelated topic,
but it is important in light of this conversation
because you're here,
and one of the things that I really enjoy
about conversations with you as it relates
to health and neuroscience and so forth is that,
you're one of the few colleagues I have
who openly admits to exploring supplementation.
I'm a long time supplement fan.
I think there's power in compounds,
both prescription, non-prescription, natural, synthesized.
I don't use these haphazardly,
but I think there's certainly power in them.
And one of the places where you and I converge
is in terms of our interest in the nervous system
and supplementation is vis-a-vis magnesium.
Now, I've talked endlessly on the podcast
and elsewhere about magnesium for sake of sleep,
and improving transit transitions
to sleep and so forth.
But you have a somewhat different interest in magnesium
as it relates to cognitive function
and durability of cognitive function.
Would you mind just sharing with us a little bit
about what that interest is, where it stems from,
and because it's The Huberman Podcast,
and we often talk about supplementation,
what you do with that information.
- So, I need to disclose
that I am a scientific advisor
to a company called Neurocentria,
which my graduate student, Guosong Liu is CEO.
So that said, I can give you some background.
Guosong, although when he was in my lab worked on breathing,
had a deep interest in learning and memory.
And when he left my lab,
he went to work for it
with a renowned learning memory guy at Stanford, Dick Chen.
And when he finished there,
he was hired by Susumu Tonegawa at MIT.
- Who also knows a thing or two about memory.
I'm teasing.
Susumu Tonegawa has a Nobel for his work on immunoglobulins,
but then is a world-class memory researcher.
- Yeah, and more.
- He's many things.
- And Guosong had very curious, very bright guy,
and he was interested in how signals
between neurons get strengthened,
which is called long-term potentiation or LTP.
And one of the questions that arose
was if I have inputs to a neuron and I get LTP,
is the LTP bigger if the signal is bigger
or the noise is less?
So, we can imagine that when we're listening to something,
if it's louder, we can hear it better.
Or if this less noise, we can hear it better.
And he wanted to investigate this.
So I did this in tissue culture of hippocampal neurons,
and what he found was that if he lowered
the background activity in all of the neurons,
that the LTP he elicited got stronger,
and the way he did that was increasing
the level of magnesium in the bathing solution.
This gets into some esoteric electrophysiology,
but basically there's a background level
of noise in all neurons,
and that part of it is regulated by the degree
of magnesium in the extracellular bath.
- And you mean electrical noise.
- Electrical noise, I'm sorry, electrical noise.
And if you, in what's called the physiological range,
which is between 0.8 and 1.2 millimolar,
which don't worry about the number.
- I can't believe you remember the millimolar
of the magnesium.
- Well, I'm always frightened that I get,
I say micro or femto or something,
I go off by several orders of magnitude,
but so in that physiological range,
there's a big difference in the amount of noise
in a neuron between 0.8 and 1.2 millimolar.
So, he played around with the magnesium,
and he found out that when the magnesium was elevated,
it was more LTP.
All right, that's an observation in a tissue culture.
- Right, and I should just mention
that more LTP essentially translates
to more neuro-plasticity,
more rewiring of connections in essence.
- So, he tested this in mice and basically,
he offered them a,
he had control mice,
which got a normal diet and one that had,
one enriched in magnesium,
and the ones that lived enriched with magnesium
had higher cognitive function, lived longer,
everything you'd want in some magic pill,
those mice did that.
Excuse me, rats.
The problem was that you couldn't imagine taking this
into humans because most magnesium salts
don't passively get from the gut into the bloodstream,
into the brain.
They pass via what's called a transporter.
Transporter is something in a membrane
that grabs a magnesium molecule or atom,
and pulls it into the other side.
So if you're imagining you have magnesium in your gut,
you have transporters that pull the magnesium
in the gut into the bloodstream.
Well, if you take a normal magnesium supplement
that you can buy at the pharmacy,
it doesn't cross the gut very easily.
And if you would take enough
of it to get it in your bloodstream,
you start getting diarrhea.
So it's not a good way to go.
- Well, it is a good way to go.
I couldn't help myself.
- [laughs] Well said.
So, he worked with this brilliant chemist, Fay Mow,
and Fay looked at a whole range of magnesium compounds
and he found that magnesium threonate
was much more effective in crossing the gut blood barrier.
Now, they didn't realize at the time,
but threonate is a metabolite of vitamin C,
and there's lots of threonate in your body.
So magnesium threonate would appear to be safe
and maybe part of the role
or now they believe it's part of the role
of the threonate is that it supercharges
the transporter to get the magnesium in.
And remember, you need a transporter at the gut
into the brain and into cells.
So, they gave magnesium threonate to mice who had,
no, let me backtrack a bit.
They did a study in humans.
They hired a company to do a test that was a hands-off test.
It's one of these companies that gets hired
by the big pharma to do their test for them,
and they got patients who were diagnosed
as mild cognitive decline.
These are people who had cognitive disorder,
which was age inappropriate.
And the metric that they use for a determining
how far off they were is Spearman's G factor,
which is a generalized measure of intelligence
that most psychologists except,
and the biological age of the subjects was,
I think 51 and the cognitive age was 61 based
on the Spearman's G's test.
Oh, I should say the Spearman G factor starts
at a particular level in the population
at age 20 and declines about 1% a year.
So, sorry to say, we're not 20 year olds anymore,
but when you get a number from that,
you can put on the curve and see whether
it's about your age or not.
These people were about 10 years older according
to that metric and long story short after three months
this is a placebo controlled double blind study.
The people who were in the placebo arm improved two years,
which is common for human studies 'cause a placebo effect.
The people who got the compound
improved eight years on average,
and some improved more than eight years.
They didn't do any further diagnosis as to what caused
the molecule to decline but it was pretty,
it was extraordinarily impressive.
- So, it moved their cognition closer
to their biological age? - Biological age.
Biological age.
- Do you recall what the doses of magnesium threonate-
- It's in the paper and it's basically
what they have in the compound,
which is sold commercially.
So, the compound which is sold commercially
is handled by a nutraceutical wholesaler
who sells it to the retailers
and they make whatever formulation they want.
But it's a dosage which is,
my understanding is rarely tolerable.
I take half a dose.
The reason I take half a dose
is that I had my blood magnesium measured,
and it was low normal for my age.
I took half a dose, it became high normal,
and I felt comfortable staying in the normal range,
but a lot of people are taking
the full dose and at my age, I'm not looking to get smarter,
I'm looking to decline more slowly.
And it's hard for me to tell you whether
or not it's effective or not.
- Well, you remembered the millimolar of the magnesium
and the solution and on the high and low end,
so I would say it's not a well controlled study
and it's an N of one, but it seems to be working.
- When I've recommended it to my friends,
academics who are not by nature skeptical,
if not cynical, and I insist that they try it,
they usually don't report a major change
in their cognitive function,
although sometimes they do report,
"Well, I feel a little bit more alert than my,
my physical movements are better,"
but many of them report they sleep better.
- And that makes sense.
I think there's good evidence that threonate
can accelerate the transition
into sleep and maybe even access
to deeper modes of sleep for some people.
For many people actually,
a small percentage of people who take threonate
including one of our podcast staff here
have stomach issues with it.
They can't tolerate it.
So, I would say just anecdotally,
about 5% of people don't tolerate threonate well.
Stop taking it and then they're fine.
It caused them diarrhea or something of that sort,
but most people tolerate it well
and most people report that it vastly improves their sleep.
And again, that's anecdotally.
There are a few studies and there are more on the way,
but that's very interesting because I,
until you and I had the discussion about threonate,
I wasn't aware of the cognitive enhancing effects,
but the story makes sense from a mechanistic perspective.
And it brings it around to a bigger
and more important statement,
which is that I so appreciate your attention to mechanism.
I guess this stems from your early training
as a physicist and the desire to get numbers
and to really parse things at a fine level.
So, we've covered a lot today.
I know there's much more that we could cover.
I'm going to insist on a part two at some point,
but I really want to speak on behalf
of a huge number of people and just thank you,
not just for your time and energy and attention to detail
and accuracy and clarity around this topic today,
but also what I should have said at the beginning,
which is that you really are a pioneer
in this field of studying respiration
and the mechanisms underlying respiration
with modern tools for now for many decades,
and the field of neuroscience was one
that was perfectly content to address issues
like memory and vision and sensation perception, et cetera.
But the respiratory system was largely overlooked
for a long time and you've just been steadily clipping away
and clipping away and much cause of the events
of related to COVID and a number of other things,
and this huge interest in breathwork
and brain states and wellness,
the field of respiration
and interest in respiration has just exploded.
So, I really want to extend a sincere thanks.
It means a lot to me,
and I know to the audience of this podcast
as someone with your depth and rigor in this area
is both a scientist and a practitioner,
and that you would share this with us.
So, thank you.
- Well, I want to thank you.
This is actually a great opportunity for me.
I've been isolated in my silo for a long time,
and it's been a wonderful experience to communicate
to people outside the silo who have an interest in this.
And I think that there's a lot that remains to be done,
and I enjoy speaking to people who have interest in this.
I find the interest to be quite mind-boggling
and it's quite wonderful that people are willing to,
and listen to things that can be quite esoteric at times,
but it gets down to deep things about who we are
and how we are going to live our lives.
So, I appreciate the opportunity and I would be delighted
to come back at any time.
- Wonderful.
We will absolutely do it.
Thanks again, Jack.
- Bye now.
- Thank you for joining me for my conversation
with Dr. Jack Feldman.
I hope you found it as entertaining
and as informative as I did.
If you're learning from and/or enjoying this podcast,
please subscribe to us on YouTube.
That's a terrific zero cost way to support us.
In addition, please subscribe to the podcast
on Spotify and Apple,
and on Apple you can leave us a review
and you can leave us up to a five-star rating.
Please also check out the sponsors mentioned
at the beginning of the podcast.
That's the best way to support this podcast.
We also have a Patreon.
It's patreon.com/andrewhuberman.
And there you can support The Huberman Lab Podcast
at any level that you like.
In addition, if you're not already following us
on Instagram and Twitter,
I teach neuroscience on Instagram and Twitter.
Some of that information covers information covered
on the podcast,
some of that information is unique information,
and that includes science and science-based tools
that you can apply in everyday life.
During today's podcast,
and on many previous podcast episodes,
we talk about supplements.
While supplements aren't necessary for everybody,
many people derive tremendous benefit from them.
One of the key issues with supplements
if you're going to take them is that they be
of the utmost quality.
For that reason, The Huberman Lab Podcast
has partnered with Thorne, T-H-O-R-N-E.
Thorne Supplements are of the very highest quality,
both with respect to the quality
of the ingredients themselves,
and the precision of the amounts of the ingredients.
Why do I say that?
Well, many supplement companies out there list amounts
of particular substances on the bottle,
and when they've been tested,
they do not match up to what's actually in those products.
Thorne has the highest levels of stringency
for quality and the particular amounts
that are in each product.
They partnered with the Mayo Clinic
and all the major sports teams,
so there's tremendous trust in Thorne products.
That's why we partnered with them.
If you're interested in seeing the supplements that I take,
you can go to thorne.com/ the letter U /huberman.
You can see the supplements that I take from Thorne.
If you purchase any of those supplements there,
you can get 20% off and if you navigate further
into the Thorne site to see the huge array
of other products that they make,
if you go in through thorne.com/u/huberman,
you'll also get 20% off any
of the products that Thorne makes.
I also want to just mention one more time,
the program that I mentioned at the beginning
of the episode, which is Our Breath Collective,
the Our Breath Collective
has an advisory board that includes people
like Dr. Jack Feldman,
where you can learn detailed breathwork protocols.
If you're interested in doing or teaching breathwork,
I highly recommend checking it out.
You can find it at ourbreathcollective.com/huberman,
and that will give you $10 off your first month.
So, I want to thank you once again for joining me
for my conversation with Dr. Jack Feldman,
and last but certainly not least,
thank you for your interest in science.
[upbeat music]