Dr. David Anderson: The Biology of Aggression, Mating, & Arousal | Huberman Lab Podcast #89

Time: 0.3

- Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,

Time: 2.25

where we discuss science

Time: 3.69

and science-based tools for everyday life.

Time: 9.27

I'm Andrew Huberman,

Time: 10.38

and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology

Time: 13.23

at Stanford School of Medicine.

Time: 15

Today, my guest is Dr. David Anderson,

Time: 17.43

Dr. Anderson is a professor of biology

Time: 19.38

at the California Institute of Technology,

Time: 21.96

often commonly referred to as Caltech University.

Time: 25.44

Dr. Anderson's research focuses on emotions

Time: 28.05

and states of mind and body,

Time: 30

and indeed he emphasizes how emotions,

Time: 31.83

like happiness, sadness, anger and so on,

Time: 34.56

are actually subcategories

Time: 36.27

of what are generally governed by states,

Time: 39.33

that is, things that are occurring

Time: 41.25

in the nervous system in our brain

Time: 43.02

and in the connections between brain and body

Time: 45.75

that dictate whether or not

Time: 46.71

we feel good about how we are feeling,

Time: 48.78

and that drive our behaviors,

Time: 50.64

that is, bias us to be in action or inaction

Time: 54.54

and strongly influence the way we interpret

Time: 56.82

our experience and our surroundings.

Time: 58.68

Today, Dr. Anderson teaches us, for instance,

Time: 61.14

why people become aggressive

Time: 62.67

and why that aggression can sometimes take the form of rage.

Time: 66.9

We also talk about sexual behavior,

Time: 68.88

and the boundaries and overlap

Time: 71.25

between aggression and sexual behavior.

Time: 73.62

And that discussion about aggression and sexual behavior

Time: 76.59

also starts to focus on particular aspects

Time: 79.11

of neural circuits and states of mind and body

Time: 81.36

that govern things like, for instance,

Time: 83.67

male-male aggression,

Time: 84.78

versus male-female aggression,

Time: 86.4

versus female-female aggression.

Time: 88.77

So today, you will learn a lot

Time: 90.06

about the biological mechanisms

Time: 91.47

that govern why we feel the way we feel.

Time: 93.9

Indeed, Dr. Anderson is an author

Time: 95.82

of a terrific new popular book,

Time: 97.86

entitled "The Nature of the Beast: How Emotions Guide Us".

Time: 101.01

I've read this book several times now,

Time: 103.093

I can tell you it contains so many gems

Time: 105.03

that are firmly grounded in the scientific research.

Time: 107.52

In fact, a lot of what's in the book

Time: 109.5

contrasts with many of the common myths

Time: 111.69

about emotions and biology.

Time: 113.91

So whether or not you're a therapist,

Time: 115.41

or you're a biologist,

Time: 116.52

or you're simply just somebody interested

Time: 118.5

in why we feel the way we feel

Time: 120.21

and why we act the way we act,

Time: 122.1

I cannot recommend the book highly enough.

Time: 123.9

Again, the title is

Time: 124.733

"The Nature of the Beast: How Emotions Guide Us".

Time: 127.2

Today's discussion also ventures into topics

Time: 129.48

such as mental health and mental illness,

Time: 132.12

and some of the exciting discoveries

Time: 133.95

that have been made by Dr. Anderson's laboratory

Time: 135.72

and other laboratories identifying specific peptides,

Time: 138.6

that is, small proteins

Time: 140.43

that can govern whether or not people

Time: 141.96

feel anxious or less anxious,

Time: 143.67

aggressive or less aggressive.

Time: 145.35

This is an important area of research

Time: 147.12

that has direct implications

Time: 148.47

for much of what we read about in the news,

Time: 150.57

both unfortunate and fortunate events,

Time: 153.06

and that will no doubt drive the future

Time: 155.58

of mental health treatments.

Time: 157.05

Dr. Anderson is considered one of the most pioneering

Time: 160.62

and important researchers in neurobiology of our time.

Time: 163.83

Indeed, he is a member of the National Academy of Sciences

Time: 165.96

and a Howard Hughes Medical Institute investigator.

Time: 169.11

I've mentioned the HHMI once or twice before

Time: 171.42

when we've had other HHMI guests on this podcast,

Time: 174.45

but for those of you that are not familiar,

Time: 175.86

the Howard Hughes Medical Institute

Time: 177.21

funds a small number of investigators

Time: 179.37

doing particularly high-risk, high-benefit work,

Time: 182.79

and it is an extremely competitive process

Time: 185.58

to identify those Howard Hughes investigators.

Time: 188.79

They are essentially appointed,

Time: 189.72

and then every five years,

Time: 191.55

they have to compete against one another

Time: 193.74

and against a new incoming flock

Time: 196.08

of would-be HHMI investigators

Time: 198.69

to get another five years of funding.

Time: 199.92

They are literally given a grade every five years

Time: 202.11

as to whether or not they can continue, not continue,

Time: 204.57

or whether or not they should worry

Time: 206.01

about being funded for an extended period of time.

Time: 208.53

Dr. Anderson has been an investigator

Time: 210.3

with the Howard Hughes Medical Institute since 1989.

Time: 213.87

I'm pleased to announce that the Huberman Lab Podcast

Time: 215.73

is now partnered with Momentous supplements.

Time: 218.07

We partnered with Momentous for several important reasons.

Time: 220.29

First of all, they ship internationally,

Time: 222

because we know that many of you are located

Time: 223.92

outside of the United States.

Time: 225.39

Second of all, and perhaps most important,

Time: 227.4

the quality of their supplements is second to none,

Time: 229.98

both in terms of purity

Time: 231.03

and precision of the amounts of the ingredients.

Time: 233.58

Third, we've really emphasized supplements

Time: 236.22

that are single-ingredient supplements,

Time: 238.29

and that are supplied in dosages

Time: 240.48

that allow you to build a supplementation protocol

Time: 243.45

that's optimized for cost,

Time: 245.28

that's optimized for effectiveness,

Time: 247.14

and that you can add things

Time: 248.64

and remove things from your protocol

Time: 250.29

in a way that's really systematic and scientific.

Time: 252.39

If you'd like to see the supplements

Time: 253.59

that we partner with Momentous on,

Time: 254.91

you can go to livemomentous.com/huberman.

Time: 258

There, you'll see those supplements,

Time: 259.14

and just keep in mind that we are constantly expanding

Time: 261.63

the library of supplements available through Momentous

Time: 264.3

on a regular basis.

Time: 265.26

Again, that's livemomentous.com/huberman.

Time: 267.84

Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize

Time: 269.55

that this podcast is separate from my teaching

Time: 271.41

and research roles at Stanford.

Time: 273

It is, however, part of my desire and effort

Time: 275.19

to bring zero cost to consumer information about science

Time: 277.71

and science-related tools to the general public.

Time: 280.23

In keeping with that theme,

Time: 281.34

I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.

Time: 284.13

Our first sponsor is Levels,

Time: 285.93

Levels is a program that lets you see

Time: 287.4

how different foods affect your health

Time: 289.02

by giving you real-time feedback on your diet

Time: 291.03

using a continuous glucose monitor.

Time: 293.46

One of the most important factors

Time: 294.75

governing our immediate and long-term health

Time: 296.46

and indeed our ability to think and focus clearly

Time: 299.61

is our blood glucose levels,

Time: 301.17

also referred to as blood sugar.

Time: 303.03

To maintain healthy energy and focus throughout the day,

Time: 305.16

you want to keep your blood glucose levels steady

Time: 307.11

and avoid peaks and crashes.

Time: 309.48

I first started using Levels about a year ago

Time: 311.76

as a way to better understand how specific foods,

Time: 314.04

food combinations, exercise and supplements

Time: 316.71

impact my blood glucose levels,

Time: 318.45

and it's been tremendously informative.

Time: 320.19

In fact, it's changed and shaped

Time: 321.69

the way that I organize my entire day.

Time: 323.88

Levels even provides a simple score

Time: 325.41

after you eat a meal

Time: 326.61

so you can see how different foods affect you

Time: 328.11

and develop a personalized nutrition program

Time: 330.12

that's ideal for you.

Time: 331.71

So if you're interested in learning more about Levels

Time: 333.54

and trying a continuous glucose monitor yourself,

Time: 335.49

you can go to levels.link/huberman,

Time: 338.07

that's levels.link/huberman.

Time: 341.4

Today's episode is also brought to us by Helix Sleep.

Time: 344.04

Helix Sleep makes mattresses and pillows

Time: 346.08

that are of the absolute highest quality.

Time: 347.97

They also have some really unique features,

Time: 350.01

because they are customized to your unique sleep needs.

Time: 353.49

I've talked over and over again on this podcast

Time: 355.38

and on other podcasts

Time: 356.67

about the fact that sleep

Time: 357.69

is the foundation of mental health,

Time: 359.7

physical health and performance,

Time: 361.26

there's just simply no other substitute

Time: 363.09

for a quality night's sleep on a regular basis.

Time: 365.61

I've been sleeping on a Helix mattress

Time: 366.99

for well over a year now

Time: 368.04

and it's the best sleep that I've ever had,

Time: 369.96

and that's in large part

Time: 371.34

because the mattress was designed for me.

Time: 374.07

What you need to know, however,

Time: 375.06

is what's the ideal mattress for you,

Time: 376.62

and you can do that by going to Helix's site,

Time: 378.99

you can take their brief quiz,

Time: 380.28

which will ask you, "Do you sleep on your side,

Time: 382.2

your back, your stomach?

Time: 383.033

Or maybe you don't know, or maybe all three.

Time: 384.6

Do you tend to run hot or cold in the night?

Time: 386.13

Maybe you know, maybe you don't."

Time: 388.08

At the end of that short quiz,

Time: 389.1

they will match you to the ideal mattress for you.

Time: 390.78

I matched to the Dusk, the D-U-S-K mattress,

Time: 393.12

but again, that's what I need,

Time: 394.62

that's not necessarily what you need

Time: 395.82

in order to get your best night's sleep.

Time: 397.68

But if you're interested in upgrading your mattress,

Time: 399.24

go to helixsleep.com/huberman,

Time: 401.46

take their two-minute sleep quiz

Time: 402.72

and they'll match you to a customized mattress for you

Time: 404.55

and you'll get up to $200 off any mattress order

Time: 407.82

and two free pillows,

Time: 408.99

they have terrific pillows.

Time: 409.92

And you get to try out that mattress

Time: 411.03

for 100 nights risk free.

Time: 412.23

They'll even pick it up for you if you don't love it,

Time: 414.18

but I'm certain you will.

Time: 415.41

Again, if you're interested in it,

Time: 416.243

you go to helixsleep.com/huberman

Time: 418.68

for up to $200 off your mattress order

Time: 420.84

and two free pillows.

Time: 422.16

Today's episode is also brought to us by LMNT.

Time: 424.761

LMNT is an electrolyte drink

Time: 426.27

that has everything you need

Time: 427.35

and nothing you don't, meaning no sugar,

Time: 429.99

but it has sodium, magnesium and potassium

Time: 432.6

in the proper ratios.

Time: 433.74

Sodium, magnesium and potassium

Time: 435.03

are critical for the functioning of all your cells,

Time: 437.58

but of your neurons, your nerve cells in particular.

Time: 440.28

Many people out there, believe it or not,

Time: 442.17

need to ingest more sodium and more potassium,

Time: 445.05

and magnesium in the proper ratios

Time: 447.24

to sodium and potassium.

Time: 449.07

For those of you that are prehypertensive or hypertensive,

Time: 451.23

you definitely don't want to crank up your sodium intake,

Time: 453.09

but many people out there,

Time: 453.99

especially people following low-carb diets

Time: 456.39

or eating exceptionally clean

Time: 458.49

very well may need more sodium.

Time: 460.92

And by drinking LMNT mixed with water,

Time: 462.81

which is what I do once or twice a day,

Time: 464.34

and certainly also during workouts and after workouts,

Time: 467.4

you can optimize mental functioning and physical performance

Time: 470.64

by increasing your electrolyte balance in the proper ratios.

Time: 474.69

If you'd like to try LMNT,

Time: 475.68

you can go to drinklmnt, that's L-M-N-T, .com/huberman,

Time: 479.91

and you'll get a free sample pack with your order.

Time: 481.92

Again, that's drinklmnt.com/huberman

Time: 485.31

to get a free sample pack with your order.

Time: 487.38

And now for my discussion with Dr. David Anderson.

Time: 490.77

David, great to be here

Time: 491.85

and great to finally sit down and chat with you.

Time: 494.13

- Great to be here too, thank you so much.

Time: 496.02

- Yeah, I have a ton of questions,

Time: 497.43

but I want to start with something fairly basic,

Time: 500.28

but that I'm aware is a pretty vast landscape,

Time: 503.64

and that's the difference between emotions and states,

Time: 507.66

if indeed there is a difference,

Time: 508.95

and how we should think about emotions.

Time: 511.89

What are they?

Time: 513.54

They have all these names,

Time: 514.5

happiness, sadness, depression, anger, rage,

Time: 518.76

how should we think about them

Time: 519.96

and why might states be

Time: 522.75

at least as useful a thing to think about,

Time: 525.45

if not more useful?

Time: 526.95

- That's great.

Time: 528.39

First, the short answer to your question

Time: 530.4

is that I see emotions as a type of internal state,

Time: 535.4

in the sense that arousal's also a type of internal state,

Time: 538.87

motivation's a type of internal state,

Time: 541.29

sleep is a type of internal state.

Time: 544.23

And the sort of simplest way

Time: 546.72

I think of internal states is that,

Time: 549.21

as you've shown in your own work,

Time: 550.5

they change the input-to-output transformation of the brain.

Time: 555.36

When you're asleep, you don't hear something

Time: 558.24

that you would hear if you were awake,

Time: 560.13

unless it's a really, really loud noise.

Time: 562.26

So from that broad perspective,

Time: 565.05

I see emotion as a class of state

Time: 568.35

that controls behavior.

Time: 570.21

The reason I think it's useful to think about it as a state

Time: 573.81

is it puts the focus on it as a neurobiological process

Time: 579.3

rather than as a psychological process.

Time: 582.6

And this gets around all of the definitional problems

Time: 586.47

that people have with the word emotion,

Time: 589.56

where many people equate emotion with feeling,

Time: 593.43

which is a subjective sense

Time: 595.62

that we can only study in humans,

Time: 599.04

because to find out what someone's feeling,

Time: 601.74

you have to ask them,

Time: 602.79

and people are the only animals that can talk,

Time: 605.61

that we can understand.

Time: 607.23

So that's how I think about emotion,

Time: 612.19

if you think of an iceberg,

Time: 613.38

it's the part of the iceberg

Time: 615.54

that's below the surface of the water,

Time: 618.06

the feeling part is the tip

Time: 620.22

that's sort of floating above

Time: 621.63

the surface of your consciousness.

Time: 623.7

Not that that isn't important, it is,

Time: 625.77

but you have to understand consciousness

Time: 628.26

if you want to understand feelings,

Time: 630.36

and we're not ready to study that in animals yet,

Time: 633.81

and so that's how I think about it.

Time: 636.42

- What are the different components of a state?

Time: 640.14

You mentioned arousal as a key component,

Time: 643.59

what are some of the other features of states

Time: 645.81

that represent this, as you so beautifully put in your book,

Time: 650.25

that represent below the tip

Time: 651.78

of the iceberg? - Right, right.

Time: 653.22

So you can break states up into different facets,

Time: 661.83

or people would call them dimensions,

Time: 663.84

and so there have been people

Time: 666.03

who have thought of emotions

Time: 668.25

as having just really two dimensions,

Time: 671.33

an arousal dimension, how intense is it?

Time: 676.68

And also a valence dimension,

Time: 680.28

which is, is it positive or negative, good or bad?

Time: 684.12

Ralph Adolphs and I have tried

Time: 685.92

to expand that a little bit

Time: 688.35

to think about components of emotion,

Time: 691.05

particularly those that distinguish emotion states

Time: 694.11

from motivational states,

Time: 696.12

because they are very closely related.

Time: 699.63

One of those important properties is persistence,

Time: 704.04

and this is something that distinguishes

Time: 706.53

state-driven behaviors from simple reflexes.

Time: 710.34

Reflexes tend to terminate when the stimulus turns off,

Time: 715.77

like the doctor hitting your knee with a hammer,

Time: 718.47

it initiates with the stimulus onset

Time: 720.9

and it terminates with the stimulus offset,

Time: 723.66

emotions tend to outlast, often,

Time: 726.18

the stimulus that evoke them.

Time: 728.34

If you're walking along a trail here in Southern California,

Time: 732.51

you hear a rattlesnake rattling,

Time: 735.15

you're going to jump in the air,

Time: 736.92

but your heart is going to continue to beat

Time: 739.26

and your palms sweat and your mouth is going to be dry

Time: 742.53

for a while after it's slithered off in the bush,

Time: 745.44

and you're going to be hypervigilant,

Time: 747.21

if you see something

Time: 748.41

that even remotely looks snake-like, a stick,

Time: 751.62

you're going to stop and jump.

Time: 753.51

So persistence is an important feature of emotion states,

Time: 759.51

not all states have persistence.

Time: 762.45

So for example, you think about hunger.

Time: 765.39

Once you've eaten, the state is gone,

Time: 768.45

you're not hungry anymore,

Time: 770.07

but if you are really angry

Time: 772.35

and you get into a fight with somebody,

Time: 774.33

even after the fight is over,

Time: 776.07

you may remain riled up for a long time

Time: 779.787

and it takes you a while to calm down,

Time: 782.28

and that may have to do with the arousal dimension

Time: 785.31

or some other part of it.

Time: 787.5

And then generalization

Time: 790.32

is an important component of emotion states that make them,

Time: 797.04

if they have been triggered in one situation,

Time: 801.33

they can apply to another situation.

Time: 804.36

And my favorite example of that is,

Time: 806.49

you come home from work and your kid is screaming,

Time: 809.97

if you had a good day at work,

Time: 811.56

you might pick it up and sooth it,

Time: 813.687

and if you had a bad day at work,

Time: 815.91

you might react very differently to it and scream at it.

Time: 819.27

And so that's a generalization

Time: 821.43

of the state that was triggered at work,

Time: 824.13

by something your boss said to you,

Time: 826.05

to a completely different interaction.

Time: 828.87

And again, that's something that distinguishes

Time: 831.45

emotion states from motivation states,

Time: 834.42

motivation states are really specific,

Time: 837.21

find and eat food,

Time: 839.31

obtain and consume water,

Time: 842.01

and they're involved in homeostatic maintenance.

Time: 846.93

So states are very multifaceted,

Time: 851.88

and just asking questions

Time: 855.93

about how these components of states are encoded,

Time: 859.68

like what makes a state persist?

Time: 862.62

What gives a state a positive or a negative valance?

Time: 866.31

How do you crank up or crank down

Time: 868.56

the intensity of the state?

Time: 870.24

It just opens up a whole bunch of questions

Time: 872.85

that you can ask in the brain

Time: 875.13

with the kinds of tools we have now.

Time: 878.46

- You mentioned arousal a few times,

Time: 880.11

and you mentioned valence,

Time: 882.66

realizing that there are these other aspects of states,

Time: 885.66

I'd like to just talk about

Time: 886.89

arousal a little bit more, and valence,

Time: 888.45

because at a very basic level,

Time: 890.25

it seems to me that arousal,

Time: 892.41

we can be very alert and pissed off,

Time: 896.73

stressed, worried, we can have insomnia,

Time: 901.14

we can also be very alert and be quite happy.

Time: 903.48

So the valence flips, people can be sexually aroused,

Time: 908.34

people can be aroused in all sorts of ways.

Time: 911.19

Is there any simple or simple-ish neurochemical signature

Time: 916.44

that can flip valence?

Time: 917.97

So for instance, is there any way that we can safely say

Time: 920.79

that arousal with some additional dopamine release

Time: 925.68

is going to be of positive valence,

Time: 928.2

and arousal with very low dopamine

Time: 931.59

is going to be of negative valence?

Time: 934.8

- I would be reluctant to say that it's a chemical flip,

Time: 939.27

I would say it's more likely to be

Time: 941.52

a circuit flip. - Mm-hmm.

Time: 943.2

- Different circuits being engaged.

Time: 944.787

And it might be that a given neurochemical, even dopamine,

Time: 948.96

is involved in both positively valanced arousal

Time: 952.65

and negatively valanced arousal,

Time: 955.02

that's why people think about these as different axes.

Time: 959.28

So I think the interesting question that you touch on is,

Time: 964.56

is arousal something that is just completely generic

Time: 967.95

in the brain,

Time: 969

or are there actually different kinds of arousal

Time: 972.96

that are specific to different behaviors?

Time: 975.33

And you raise the question,

Time: 976.41

sexual arousal feels different

Time: 978.69

from aggressive arousal, for example,

Time: 981.51

and we actually published a paper on this,

Time: 984.48

back in 2009, in fruit flies,

Time: 987.66

where we found some evidence

Time: 989.91

for two types of arousal states.

Time: 992.82

One of which is sleep-wake arousal,

Time: 995.64

you're more aroused when you wake up

Time: 997.38

than when you're asleep,

Time: 998.61

and flies show that,

Time: 999.9

and the other is a startle response,

Time: 1003.68

an arousal response to a mechanical stimulus,

Time: 1006.74

and not just mechanical stimulus.

Time: 1008.78

If you puff air on flies,

Time: 1010.73

kind of like trying to swat the wasp away

Time: 1013.49

from your burger at a picnic table,

Time: 1015.62

they come back more and more and more vigorously.

Time: 1018.8

And we were able to dissect this

Time: 1021.2

and show that although both of those forms of arousal

Time: 1025.19

required dopamine, they were exerted

Time: 1029.3

through completely separable neural circuits in the fly.

Time: 1033.47

And so that really put, number one,

Time: 1036.08

the emphasis on it's the circuit

Time: 1038.21

that determines the type of arousal,

Time: 1040.64

but also that arousal isn't unitary,

Time: 1044.09

that there are behavior-specific forms of arousal.

Time: 1047.39

And I think the jury is still out

Time: 1049.79

as to whether there is such a thing

Time: 1052.37

as completely generalized arousal or not.

Time: 1055.727

And I think some people would argue there is,

Time: 1058.43

but I think more attention needs to be paid to this question

Time: 1062.06

of domain-specific or behavior-specific forms of arousal.

Time: 1066.59

- Yeah, it's a super-interesting idea,

Time: 1068.18

'cause I always thought of arousal as along a continuum,

Time: 1070.22

like you can either be in a panic attack

Time: 1072.02

at the one end of the extreme,

Time: 1073.13

or you can be in a coma,

Time: 1074.27

and then somewhere in the middle, you're alert and calm,

Time: 1076.7

but then this issue of valence really, as you say,

Time: 1080.57

presents this opportunity

Time: 1081.65

that really there might be multiple circuits for arousal.

Time: 1084.57

- Yeah. - Or multiple mechanisms

Time: 1087.08

that would include neurochemicals, as well as

Time: 1089.33

different neural pathways. - Yeah.

Time: 1091.22

- So like to talk a bit about a state,

Time: 1095.09

if it is indeed a state, which is aggression,

Time: 1098.03

your labs worked extensively on this.

Time: 1100.49

And if you would, could you highlight

Time: 1102.89

some of the key findings there,

Time: 1105.02

which brain areas that are involved?

Time: 1107.54

The beautiful work of Dayu Lin and others in your lab

Time: 1112.04

that point to the idea

Time: 1113.6

that indeed there are kind of switches in the brain,

Time: 1117.14

but that thinking of switches for aggression

Time: 1120.26

might be too simple,

Time: 1122.54

how should we think about aggression?

Time: 1124.37

And I'll just sort of skew the question

Time: 1127.19

a bit more by saying,

Time: 1128.78

we see lots of different kinds of aggression,

Time: 1131.66

this terrible school shooting down in Texas recently,

Time: 1136.31

clearly an act that included aggression,

Time: 1138.29

and yet, you could imagine

Time: 1140.15

that's a very different type of aggression

Time: 1141.35

than an all-out rage or a controlled aggression,

Time: 1145.49

there's a lot of variation there.

Time: 1146.66

So what are your thoughts on aggression,

Time: 1149.03

how it's generated, the neural circuit mechanisms

Time: 1150.98

and some of the variation in what we call aggression?

Time: 1153.8

- Yeah, this is a great question,

Time: 1155.3

and it's a large area.

Time: 1157.67

I would say that, first of all,

Time: 1161.3

the word aggression, in my mind,

Time: 1164.75

refers more to a description of behavior

Time: 1168.32

than it does to an internal state.

Time: 1172.34

Aggression could reflect an internal state

Time: 1175.28

that we would call anger in humans,

Time: 1178.25

or could reflect fear,

Time: 1180.56

or it could reflect hunger if it's predatory aggression.

Time: 1185.057

And so this gets at the issue that you raised

Time: 1187.67

of the different types of aggression that exist.

Time: 1191.39

The work that Dayu did when she was in my lab

Time: 1195.68

that really broke open the field

Time: 1198.71

to the application of modern genetic tools

Time: 1201.74

for studying circuits in mice

Time: 1203.99

is that she found a way to evoke aggression in mice

Time: 1209.9

using optogenetics to activate specific neurons

Time: 1215.45

in a region of the hypothalamus,

Time: 1218.33

the ventromedial hypothalamus, VMH,

Time: 1221.3

which people had been studying

Time: 1223.34

and looking at for decades,

Time: 1227.42

following, first, the work of,

Time: 1231.11

in cats, the famous Nobel Prize-winning work

Time: 1234.74

of Walter Hess,

Time: 1236.45

and then followed by work

Time: 1238.37

done by Menno Kruk, in the Netherlands, in rats,

Time: 1242.57

where they would stick electrical wires into the brain

Time: 1246.92

and send electric currents into the brain,

Time: 1249.59

and they could trigger a placid cat

Time: 1252.8

to suddenly bare its teeth, hiss

Time: 1256.04

and almost strike out at the experimenter,

Time: 1259.37

and they could trigger rats to fight with each other.

Time: 1263.45

And even in Hess's original experiments,

Time: 1267.17

he describes two types of aggression

Time: 1269.96

that he evokes from cats

Time: 1271.91

depending on where in the hypothalamus

Time: 1274.61

he puts his electrode,

Time: 1276.83

one of which he calls defensive rage,

Time: 1280.01

that's the ears laid back, teeth bared and hissing.

Time: 1284.33

And the other one is predatory aggression,

Time: 1287.54

where the cat has its ears forward,

Time: 1290.63

and it's batting with its paw at a mouse-like object,

Time: 1294.38

like it wants to catch it and eat it.

Time: 1296.78

So he already had, at that stage,

Time: 1299.3

some information about segregation in the brain

Time: 1302.78

of different forms of aggression.

Time: 1304.76

So fast forward to 2008, 2009,

Time: 1312.98

when Dayu came to the lab

Time: 1314.75

and we had started working on aggression in fruit flies,

Time: 1318.14

and I wanted to bring it into mice

Time: 1320.81

so that we could apply genetic tools.

Time: 1323.21

And we started by having Dayu,

Time: 1325.46

who was an electrophysiologist,

Time: 1328.13

just repeat the electrical stimulation

Time: 1331.76

of the ventromedial hypothalamus in the mouse,

Time: 1335.21

just like people had done in rats,

Time: 1337.76

in cats, in hamsters, even in monkeys.

Time: 1340.58

And she could not get that experiment to work

Time: 1343.52

over 40 different trials,

Time: 1346.37

it just didn't work.

Time: 1347.51

What she got instead was fear behaviors,

Time: 1351.08

she got freezing, cornering and crouching.

Time: 1355.76

And finally, in desperation,

Time: 1359.09

and we got a lot of input from Menno Kruk on this,

Time: 1361.76

he really was mystified,

Time: 1362.997

"Why doesn't it work in mice?"

Time: 1364.82

We realized why there had been no paper

Time: 1367.67

on brain-stimulated aggression in mice in 50 years,

Time: 1372.44

'cause the experiment doesn't work.

Time: 1374.87

And the one bit of credit I can claim there

Time: 1379.19

is I convinced Dayu to try optogenetics,

Time: 1383.39

because it just had sort of come into use deep in the brain,

Time: 1389.75

from Karl Deisseroth and others' work.

Time: 1393.11

And I thought maybe because it could be

Time: 1395.45

directed more specifically to a region of the brain

Time: 1399.35

and types of cells than electrical stimulation,

Time: 1404.66

it might work.

Time: 1405.5

And Dayu said, "Never, never going to work.

Time: 1407.84

If it doesn't work with electricity,

Time: 1409.58

why should it work with optogenetics?"

Time: 1412.67

And the fact is that it did work,

Time: 1415.43

and we were able to trigger aggression in this region

Time: 1420.14

using optogenetic stimulation of ventromedial hypothalamus.

Time: 1424.01

And in retrospect, I think the reason

Time: 1426.59

that we were seeing all these fear behaviors

Time: 1430.28

is because right at the upper part,

Time: 1433.73

if you think of ventromedial hypothalamus

Time: 1436.7

like a pear sitting on the ground,

Time: 1439.25

the fat part of the pear near the ground

Time: 1441.95

is where the aggression neurons are,

Time: 1443.84

but the upper part of the pear has fear neurons.

Time: 1447.47

And it could be because it's so small in a mouse,

Time: 1451.1

when you inject electrical current anywhere in the pear,

Time: 1455.12

it flows up through the entire pear

Time: 1457.61

and it activates the fear circuits,

Time: 1459.74

and those totally dominate aggression.

Time: 1462.77

And so that's why we were never able to see

Time: 1465.5

any fighting with electrical stimulation,

Time: 1468.02

whereas when you use optogenetics,

Time: 1469.7

you confine the stimulation just to the region

Time: 1474.08

where you've implanted the channelrhodopsin gene

Time: 1478.46

into those neurons.

Time: 1481.13

And so fast forward from that,

Time: 1483.56

from a lot of work from Dayu now on her own at NYU,

Time: 1487.79

and with her postdoc, Annegret Falkner,

Time: 1491.84

as well as work of other people,

Time: 1493.55

there's evidence that the type of fighting

Time: 1498.32

that we elicit when we stimulate VMH

Time: 1501.77

is offensive aggression

Time: 1504.41

that is actually rewarding to male mice.

Time: 1507.8

- They like it. - They like it,

Time: 1509.3

male mice will learn to poke their nose

Time: 1513.2

or press a bar to get the opportunity

Time: 1516.35

to beat up a subordinate male mouse.

Time: 1519.98

And in more recent experiments,

Time: 1522.23

if you activate those neurons

Time: 1524.6

and the mouse has a chance to be

Time: 1526.46

in one of two compartments in a box,

Time: 1529.07

they will gravitate towards the compartment

Time: 1531.23

where those neurons are activated,

Time: 1533.42

it has a positive valance.

Time: 1535.94

And when I went into this field

Time: 1537.74

and I was thinking, "Well, what goes on in my brain

Time: 1541.22

and my body when I'm furious?"

Time: 1543.53

it certainly doesn't feel like a rewarding experience,

Time: 1547.46

it's not something that I would want to repeat

Time: 1550.13

because it feels good when I'm in that state,

Time: 1552.62

it doesn't feel good at all when I'm in that state.

Time: 1555.56

And it is still, I think, a mystery

Time: 1559.25

as to where that type of aggression,

Time: 1561.26

which is more defensive aggression,

Time: 1563.81

the kind of aggression you feel if you're being attacked

Time: 1567.05

or if you've been cheated by somebody,

Time: 1569.99

where that is encoded in the brain and how that works,

Time: 1574.13

still, I think, is a very important mystery

Time: 1577.43

that we haven't solved.

Time: 1578.93

And predatory aggression there has been some progress on,

Time: 1582.23

so mice show predatory aggression,

Time: 1584.51

they use that to catch crickets that they eat,

Time: 1587.21

and that involves different circuits

Time: 1589.73

than the ventromedial hypothalamic circuits.

Time: 1592.73

So it's become clear that, if you want to call it

Time: 1596.66

the state of aggressiveness, is multifaceted,

Time: 1601.82

it depends on the type of aggression

Time: 1604.55

and it involves different sorts of circuits.

Time: 1608.72

There's a paper suggesting

Time: 1610.34

that there might be a final common pathway

Time: 1614.72

for all aggression in a region,

Time: 1617.72

which is one of my favorites,

Time: 1619.19

it's called the substantia innominata,

Time: 1621.77

the substance with no name, I like.

Time: 1624.47

- Anatomists are so creative. - Yes.

Time: 1626.525

[Andrew laughing] Or the nucleus ambiguous,

Time: 1628.7

or the zona incerta, these are places

Time: 1631.31

that no one can think of what they are.

Time: 1633.11

Anyhow, that might be a final common pathway

Time: 1636.08

for predatory aggression,

Time: 1637.88

and offensive and defensive aggression,

Time: 1640.4

but it can be really hard to tell

Time: 1642.59

just from looking at a mouse fight

Time: 1645.47

whether it's engaged in offensive or defensive aggression.

Time: 1649.64

We've tried to take that apart

Time: 1651.47

using machine learning analysis of behavior,

Time: 1654.59

but in rats, for example,

Time: 1656.3

it's much clearer when the animal

Time: 1658.7

is engaged in offensive versus defensive aggression.

Time: 1661.82

They direct their bites

Time: 1663.53

at different parts of the opponent's body.

Time: 1666.02

- [Andrew] In particular.

Time: 1667.43

- Offensive aggression is flank directed,

Time: 1671.21

defensive aggression goes for the neck,

Time: 1673.82

goes for the throat. - Mm,

Time: 1675.17

I've seen some nature specials

Time: 1676.46

where in a very barbaric way,

Time: 1679.129

[laughing] at least to me, it seems,

Time: 1680.84

like hyenas will try and go after the reproductive axis,

Time: 1685.16

they'll go after testicles and penis

Time: 1687.38

and they basically want to, it seems they want to limit

Time: 1690.95

future breeding potential. - Yes, or create pain.

Time: 1693.697

[David laughing] - Right, or create pain,

Time: 1694.85

or both. - Yeah.

Time: 1695.72

- Yeah, I mean, in terms of offensive aggression

Time: 1698.81

and your reflection that it doesn't feel good,

Time: 1703.344

I mean, I can say, I know some people

Time: 1705.41

who really enjoy fighting.

Time: 1707.15

- [David] Hmm.

Time: 1708.08

- I have a relative who's a lawyer,

Time: 1709.49

he loves to argue and fight.

Time: 1711.32

- [David] Huh.

Time: 1712.16

- I don't think of him as physically aggressive,

Time: 1714.71

in fact, he's not,

Time: 1715.58

but loves to fight and loves to prosecute

Time: 1718.58

and go after people. - Hmm.

Time: 1719.84

- And he's pretty effective at it.

Time: 1721.38

- Right. - I have a friend,

Time: 1722.27

former military special operations,

Time: 1724.49

and very calm guy, had a great career

Time: 1728.48

in military special operations,

Time: 1729.8

and he'll quite plainly say, "I love to fight."

Time: 1732.71

- Mm-hmm. - "It's one my great joys."

Time: 1734.39

He really enjoyed his work.

Time: 1735.71

- [David] Yep.

Time: 1736.97

- And also respected the other side

Time: 1738.86

because they offered the opportunity to test that

Time: 1741.83

and to experience that joy.

Time: 1743.06

So in a kind of bizarre way to somebody like me,

Time: 1744.98

who I'll certainly defend my stance if I need to.

Time: 1748.32

- Yeah. - But I certainly

Time: 1750.02

don't consider myself somebody

Time: 1751.1

who offensively goes after people just to go after them,

Time: 1753.8

there's no, quote-unquote, dopamine hit here.

Time: 1757.19

- Right. - Acknowledging that dopamine

Time: 1758.21

does many things, of course.

Time: 1759.41

- Yeah. - I have couple of questions

Time: 1761.3

about the way you describe the circuitry,

Time: 1764

I should say, the way the circuitry is arranged.

Time: 1765.8

- [David] Mm-hmm.

Time: 1766.88

- And of course, we don't know,

Time: 1768.02

because we weren't consulted at the design phase.

Time: 1771.436

[David laughing] But why do you think

Time: 1773.93

there would be such a close positioning of neurons

Time: 1776.87

that can elicit such divergent states and behaviors?

Time: 1781.25

I mean, you're talking about this pear-shaped structure,

Time: 1783.53

where the neurons that generate fear

Time: 1786.05

are cheek to jowl with the neurons

Time: 1788.3

that generate offensive aggression of all things.

Time: 1791.63

It's like putting the neurons that control swallowing

Time: 1795.89

next to the neurons that control vomiting,

Time: 1798.23

[laughing] it just seems to me that,

Time: 1800.21

on the one hand, this is the way

Time: 1801.44

that neural circuits are often arranged,

Time: 1803.03

and yet, to me, it's always been perplexing

Time: 1804.59

as to why this would be the case.

Time: 1806.27

- Yeah, I think that is a very profound question,

Time: 1810.17

and I've wondered about that a lot.

Time: 1815.21

If you think from an evolutionary perspective,

Time: 1820.28

it might have been the case

Time: 1822.44

that defensive behaviors and fear

Time: 1826.34

arose before offensive aggression,

Time: 1831.53

because animals, first and foremost,

Time: 1833.81

have to defend themselves from predation by other animals.

Time: 1838.16

And maybe it's only when they're comfortable

Time: 1841.25

with having warded off predation

Time: 1843.59

and made themselves safe,

Time: 1845.27

that they can start to think about,

Time: 1847.797

"Who's going to be the alpha male in my group here?"

Time: 1851.84

And so it could be that if you think that brain regions

Time: 1856.37

and cell populations evolve by duplication

Time: 1860.21

and modification of preexisting cell populations,

Time: 1865.4

that might be the way that those regions

Time: 1868.67

wound up next to each other.

Time: 1871.58

And developmentally, they start out

Time: 1874.31

from a common pool of precursors

Time: 1876.74

that expresses the same gene,

Time: 1878.54

the fear neurons and the aggression neurons,

Time: 1881.27

and then with development,

Time: 1882.56

it gets shut off in the aggression neurons

Time: 1884.99

and maintained in the fear neurons.

Time: 1887.63

Now, that view says, "Oh, it's an accident

Time: 1890.99

of evolution and development,"

Time: 1892.64

but I think there must be a functional part as well.

Time: 1896.12

So one thing we know about offensive aggression

Time: 1898.88

is that strong fear shuts it down,

Time: 1902.48

whereas defensive aggression, at least in rats,

Time: 1906.14

is actually enhanced by fear.

Time: 1908.75

It's one of the big differences

Time: 1910.46

between defensive aggression and offensive aggression.

Time: 1914.27

And if you think about it,

Time: 1915.92

if offensive aggression is rewarding and pleasurable,

Time: 1919.25

if you start to get really scared,

Time: 1921.65

that tends to take the fun out of it,

Time: 1923.9

and maybe these two regions are close to each other

Time: 1927.11

to facilitate inhibition of aggression by the fear neurons.

Time: 1933.35

We know for a fact that if we deliberately stimulate

Time: 1936.56

those fear neurons at the top of the pear,

Time: 1939.38

when two animals are involved in a fight,

Time: 1941.48

it just stops the fight dead in its tracks

Time: 1944.3

and they go off into the corner and freeze.

Time: 1947.03

So at least hierarchically, it seems like fear

Time: 1950.81

is the dominant behavior over offensive aggression,

Time: 1954.35

and how that inhibition would work is not clear,

Time: 1957.32

'cause all these neurons are pretty much excitatory,

Time: 1959.96

they're almost all glutamatergic.

Time: 1961.91

And so one of the interesting questions for the future is,

Time: 1965.84

how exactly does fear dominate over

Time: 1969.26

and shut down offensive aggression in the brain?

Time: 1972.68

How does that work, is it all circuitry,

Time: 1974.75

are there chemicals involved?

Time: 1976.52

What's the mechanism and when is it called into play?

Time: 1980.3

But I think that's the way

Time: 1982.28

I tend to think about why these neurons

Time: 1984.29

are all mixed up together.

Time: 1986.36

And it's not just fight and freezing,

Time: 1989.57

or fight and flight,

Time: 1990.5

there are also metabolic neurons that are mixed together

Time: 1994.43

in VMH as well. - Mm-hmm.

Time: 1996.08

Controlling body-wide metabolism?

Time: 1998.36

- Yeah.

Time: 1999.278

- Very interesting. - There are neurons there

Time: 2000.25

that respond to glucose,

Time: 2001.75

when glucose goes up in your bloodstream, they're activated,

Time: 2005.77

and VMH has a whole history in the field of obesity,

Time: 2010.15

because if you destroy it in a rat, you get a fat rat.

Time: 2015.22

So the way most of the world thinks about VMH

Time: 2018.15

is they think about, "Oh, that's the thing

Time: 2020.26

that keeps you from getting fat."

Time: 2022.12

It's the anti-obesity area,

Time: 2024.16

but in the area of social behavior,

Time: 2027.19

we see it as a center for control of aggression

Time: 2030.7

and fear behaviors.

Time: 2031.81

And again, why these neurons and these functions,

Time: 2036.4

I like to call them the four Fs,

Time: 2038.41

feeding, freezing, fighting and mating.

Time: 2040.926

- Mm-hmm. - That they all seem to be

Time: 2041.95

closely intermingled with each other,

Time: 2044.05

maybe because crosstalk between them

Time: 2046.48

is very important to help the animal's brain

Time: 2049.54

decide what behavior to prioritize

Time: 2052.84

and what behavior to shut down at any given moment.

Time: 2057.19

- One of the things that we will do

Time: 2058.42

is link to the incredible videos of these mice

Time: 2062.38

that have selective stimulation of neurons in the VMH,

Time: 2066.443

Dayu's and the other studies that you've done.

Time: 2069.67

Whenever I teach, I show those videos at some point,

Time: 2073.51

with the caveats and warnings that are required

Time: 2076.57

when one is about to see a video of a mouse

Time: 2080.83

trying to mate with another mouse,

Time: 2082.06

or mating with another mouse,

Time: 2083.41

and they seem both to be quite happy

Time: 2085.15

about the mating experience,

Time: 2086.53

at least as far as we know, as observers of mice.

Time: 2090.19

And then upon stimulation of those VMH neurons,

Time: 2095.47

one of the mice essentially tries to kill the other mouse.

Time: 2098.89

And then when that stimulation is stopped,

Time: 2102.76

they basically go back to hanging out.

Time: 2104.65

They don't go right back to mating.

Time: 2106.243

- Right. - There's some reconciliation

Time: 2107.29

clearly that needs to [laughing] happen first,

Time: 2108.79

we assume. - Yes.

Time: 2109.84

- But it's just so striking,

Time: 2110.797

and I think equally striking is the video

Time: 2112.72

where the mouse is alone in there with the glove,

Time: 2116.77

the VMH neurons are stimulated

Time: 2118.33

and the mouse goes into a rage,

Time: 2120.16

it looks like it wants to kill

Time: 2121.09

the glove, basically. - Yep.

Time: 2122.98

- So striking, I encourage people to go watch those,

Time: 2125.53

because it really puts a tremendous amount of color

Time: 2129.7

on what we're describing,

Time: 2130.84

and it's just the idea that there are switches in the brain,

Time: 2134.92

to me, really became clear upon seeing that.

Time: 2137.44

One of the, excuse me, one of the concepts

Time: 2140.26

that you've raised in your lectures before

Time: 2143.14

and that I think was Hess's idea

Time: 2145.3

is this idea of a sort of hydraulic pressure.

Time: 2147.43

- Mm-hmm. - Or maybe it was Konrad,

Time: 2149.77

I can't speak now.

Time: 2150.75

[David laughing] Excuse me, Konrad Lorenz,

Time: 2153.1

pardon. - Mm-hmm.

Time: 2153.933

- Who talked about a kind of hydraulic pressure

Time: 2155.68

towards behavior.

Time: 2157.21

I'm fascinated by this idea of hydraulic pressure,

Time: 2160.15

because I don't consider myself a hot-tempered person,

Time: 2163.18

but I am familiar with the fact that when I lose my temper,

Time: 2166.39

it takes quite a while for me to simmer down.

Time: 2169.69

I can't think about anything else,

Time: 2171.25

I don't want to think about anything else.

Time: 2172.66

In fact, trying to think about anything else

Time: 2175.36

becomes aversive to me,

Time: 2177.37

which, to me, underscores this notion of prioritization

Time: 2181.93

of the different states. - Mm-hmm.

Time: 2183.58

- And potentially conflicting states.

Time: 2186.76

What do you think funnels into this idea

Time: 2188.56

of hydraulic pressure toward a state?

Time: 2191.17

And why is it, perhaps, that sometimes we can be very angry,

Time: 2196.12

and if we succeed in winning an argument,

Time: 2199.75

all of a sudden, it will subside?

Time: 2201.91

Because clearly that means

Time: 2202.84

that there are external influences,

Time: 2204.31

it's a complex space here that we're creating,

Time: 2206.17

I realize I'm creating

Time: 2207.003

a bit of a cloud. - Yeah.

Time: 2207.836

- And I'm doing it on purpose, because, to me,

Time: 2209.86

the idea of a hydraulic pressure towards a state,

Time: 2211.78

like sleep, there's a sleep pressure.

Time: 2213.61

- Yeah. - There is a pressure

Time: 2214.75

towards aggression, that all makes sense,

Time: 2216.43

but what's involved?

Time: 2218.02

Is it too multifactorial

Time: 2219.67

to actually separate out the variables,

Time: 2222.64

but what's really driving hydraulic pressure

Time: 2226.51

toward a given state?

Time: 2228.01

- Yeah, so really important question,

Time: 2231.94

I think one way that is helpful, at least for me,

Time: 2235.39

to break this question apart and think about it

Time: 2238.39

is to distinguish homeostatic behaviors,

Time: 2242.92

that is, need-based behaviors,

Time: 2245.2

where the pressure is built up because of a need,

Time: 2249.4

like, "I'm hungry, I need to eat.

Time: 2252.16

I'm thirsty, I need to drink.

Time: 2255.1

I'm hot, I need to get to a cold place,"

Time: 2258.1

it's basically the thermostat model of your brain.

Time: 2261.64

You have a set point,

Time: 2262.93

and then if the temperature gets too hot,

Time: 2264.76

you turn on the AC,

Time: 2266.08

and if the temperature gets too cold,

Time: 2267.91

you turn on the heater

Time: 2268.87

and you put yourself back to the set point.

Time: 2271.09

I don't think that's how aggression works.

Time: 2273.73

That is, it's not that we all go around,

Time: 2276.76

at least subjectively,

Time: 2277.9

I don't go around with an accumulating need to fight,

Time: 2282.64

which I then look for an excuse to release it.

Time: 2287.17

Now, maybe there are people that do that,

Time: 2288.667

and they go out and look for bar fights to get into to.

Time: 2292.314

- Or Twitter.

Time: 2293.36

- Yeah, [laughing] or Twitter,

Time: 2294.528

yeah. - Twitter seems to,

Time: 2295.361

I'm sort of half joking, because Twitter seems to draw

Time: 2298.27

a reasonably sized crowd of people

Time: 2300.73

that are there for combat of some sort,

Time: 2304.42

even though the total intellectual power

Time: 2306.4

of any of their comments is about that of a cap gun.

Time: 2308.721

[David laughing] They seem to really like

Time: 2309.76

to fire off that cap gun. - Right, right.

Time: 2312.52

- But I agree. - Yeah.

Time: 2313.72

- Before we continue with today's discussion,

Time: 2315.46

I'd like to just briefly acknowledge

Time: 2317.35

our sponsor, Athletic Greens, now called AG1.

Time: 2321.25

Athletic Greens, aka AG1,

Time: 2323.5

is an all-in-one, vitamin-mineral probiotic drink

Time: 2325.99

that also has adaptogens and digestive enzymes.

Time: 2329.02

I've been taking Athletic Greens since way back in 2012,

Time: 2331.75

so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast.

Time: 2334.15

The reason I started taking Athletic Greens

Time: 2335.8

and the reason I still drink Athletic Greens twice a day

Time: 2338.74

is that it supplies total foundational coverage

Time: 2341.65

of my vitamin-mineral needs

Time: 2343.03

and it supplies important nutrients

Time: 2345.55

that I need to support my gut microbiome.

Time: 2347.53

The gut microbiome, as many of you know,

Time: 2349.27

supports the immune system,

Time: 2350.38

it also supports the so-called gut-brain access,

Time: 2352.39

which is vital for mood, for energy levels,

Time: 2354.76

for regulating focus

Time: 2356.02

and many other features of our mental health

Time: 2357.88

and physical health that impact our daily performance

Time: 2360.19

and high performance in any endeavors

Time: 2361.81

we might be involved in.

Time: 2363.22

If you'd like to try Athletic Greens,

Time: 2364.6

you can go to athleticgreens.com/huberman

Time: 2367.27

and claim a special offer.

Time: 2368.86

They're giving away five free travel packs

Time: 2370.54

plus a year's supply of vitamin D3 K2 with every order.

Time: 2374.44

And of course, vitamin D3 K2

Time: 2376.12

are vital for all sorts of things,

Time: 2377.8

like hormone health and metabolic health,

Time: 2379.57

and K2 for cardiovascular health and calcium regulation.

Time: 2382.36

Again, you can go to athleticgreens.com/huberman

Time: 2385.54

to claim that special offer.

Time: 2386.86

- So you can think of this accumulated hydraulic pressure

Time: 2392.14

either being based on something that you were deprived of

Time: 2395.56

creating an accumulating need,

Time: 2397.93

or something that you want to do

Time: 2400.54

building up a drive or a pressure to do that,

Time: 2404.92

and the natural way to think about that, at least for me,

Time: 2408.97

is as gradual increases in neural activity

Time: 2413.5

in a particular region of the brain.

Time: 2415.93

And so for example, in the area of the hypothalamus

Time: 2419.89

that controls feeding,

Time: 2421.39

Scott Sternson and others have shown

Time: 2423.88

that the hungrier you get,

Time: 2426.22

the higher the level of activity

Time: 2428.56

in that region in the brain,

Time: 2430.3

and then when you eat, boom,

Time: 2431.92

the activity goes right back down again.

Time: 2434.56

And that state is actually negatively valenced,

Time: 2438.58

so it's like the animal, quote-unquote,

Time: 2441.79

feels increasingly uncomfortable,

Time: 2444.55

just like we feel increasingly uncomfortable

Time: 2447.19

the hungrier we are,

Time: 2448.6

and then when we eat, it tamps it down,

Time: 2451

but there is this increased activity.

Time: 2453.34

And I think in the case of aggression,

Time: 2455.47

our data and others show

Time: 2457.09

that the more strongly you drive

Time: 2460.39

this region of the brain optogenetically,

Time: 2463.63

the more of just a hair trigger you need

Time: 2467.26

to set the animal off to get it to fight.

Time: 2469.963

Now, the interesting thing

Time: 2471.73

is that if there is nothing for the animal to attack,

Time: 2475.78

it doesn't really do much

Time: 2477.55

when you're stimulating this region.

Time: 2479.65

It sort of wanders around the cage a little bit more,

Time: 2483.16

but it will not actually show overt attack

Time: 2487.69

unless you put something in front of it.

Time: 2489.64

And the same thing is true for the areas

Time: 2491.53

we've described that control mating behavior.

Time: 2494.47

This is what Lindsay is working on,

Time: 2496.39

you can stimulate those areas 'til you're blue in the face,

Time: 2499.99

and the mouse just sort of wanders around,

Time: 2502.18

but if you put another mouse in,

Time: 2504.49

wham, he will try to mount that mouse,

Time: 2506.8

if you put a kumquat in the cage.

Time: 2508.992

[Andrew laughing] He'll try to mount

Time: 2509.95

the kumquat,

Time: 2511.21

and so it becomes a sort of any port in the storm.

Time: 2514.84

So there is this idea that the drive

Time: 2518.53

is building up pressure

Time: 2520.78

that somehow needs to be released

Time: 2523.57

where that pressure is actually being exerted,

Time: 2527.65

if you accept that it's increased activity

Time: 2530.26

in some circuit or circuits someplace,

Time: 2532.9

what is it pushing up against that needs something else

Time: 2538.75

to sort of unplug it in the Lorenz hydraulic model?

Time: 2542.86

That is, you don't see the behavior

Time: 2545.05

until you release a valve on this bucket

Time: 2547.9

and let the accumulated pressure flow out.

Time: 2551.41

And that's one of the things we're trying to study

Time: 2553.93

in the context of the mating behavior as well,

Time: 2557.41

how does the information

Time: 2559.24

that there's an object in front of you

Time: 2561.52

come together with this drive state

Time: 2564.04

that is generated by stimulating

Time: 2566.32

these neurons in the hypothalamus to say,

Time: 2568.577

"Okay, pull the trigger and go,

Time: 2570.82

it's time to mate, it's time to attack?"

Time: 2573.34

And we're just starting to get some insights into that now.

Time: 2576.82

- Fascinating, and I should mention to people,

Time: 2579.7

Dr. Anderson mentioned Lindsay,

Time: 2580.99

Lindsay is a former graduate student of mine

Time: 2582.61

that's now a postdoc in David's lab.

Time: 2584.44

And I haven't caught up with her recently

Time: 2586.33

to hear about these experiments,

Time: 2587.407

but they sound fascinating.

Time: 2589.03

I would love to spend some time on this issue

Time: 2591.64

of why is it that a mouse won't attack nothing,

Time: 2595.96

but it'll attack even a glove,

Time: 2598.45

and why it will only try and mate

Time: 2602.02

if there's another mouse to mate with?

Time: 2604.57

It's actually, I think, fortunately for you,

Time: 2608.29

you're not spending a lot of time

Time: 2609.43

on Twitter and Instagram or YouTube,

Time: 2612.43

but there's this whole online community that exists now,

Time: 2615.4

as far as I know, it's almost exclusively young males

Time: 2619.84

who are obsessed with this idea,

Time: 2622.81

I'll just say it, it has a name, it's called NoFap,

Time: 2624.69

of no masturbation as a way to maintain their motivation

Time: 2628.06

to go out and actually seek mates.

Time: 2629.71

Because of the ready availability of online pornography.

Time: 2633.733

- Huh. - There's probably

Time: 2635.41

a much larger population of young males

Time: 2637.3

that are never actually going out and seeking mates

Time: 2639.16

because they're getting porn addicted, et cetera.

Time: 2641.71

There's actually a serious issue

Time: 2643.03

that came up in our episode

Time: 2644.59

with Anna Lembke, who wrote the book

Time: 2645.797

"Dopamine Nation". - Hmm.

Time: 2647.38

- Because of the availability of pornography,

Time: 2649.39

there's a whole social context

Time: 2651.64

that's being created around this, and genuine addiction.

Time: 2654.73

So humans are not like the mice,

Time: 2656.98

or mice are not like the humans,

Time: 2658.48

humans seem to resolve the issue on their own.

Time: 2661.21

- Yeah. - In ways that might actually

Time: 2662.5

impede seeking and finding of sexual partners

Time: 2665.41

and/or long-term mates.

Time: 2666.55

- Right. - So serious issue there,

Time: 2668.47

I raise it as a serious issue.

Time: 2669.373

- Yep. - That I hear a lot about,

Time: 2670.497

'cause I get asked hundreds

Time: 2672.34

if not thousands of questions about this,

Time: 2673.757

"Is there any physiological basis

Time: 2675.37

for what they call NoFap?"

Time: 2676.87

And I never actually reply 'cause there's no data.

Time: 2679.57

- [David] Yep.

Time: 2680.53

- But what you're raising here

Time: 2681.43

is a very interesting mechanistic scenario

Time: 2686.05

that can, and as you mentioned, is being explored.

Time: 2689.5

So what do we know about the internal state of a mouse

Time: 2694.63

whose VMH is being stimulated

Time: 2697

or a mouse whose other brain region

Time: 2700.66

that can stimulate the desire to mate,

Time: 2703.33

what do we know about the internal state of that mouse

Time: 2705.61

if it's just alone in the cage wandering around?

Time: 2707.62

iI it wandering around really wanting to mate

Time: 2709.54

and really wanting to fight?

Time: 2710.65

We, of course, don't know,

Time: 2713.11

but is its heart rate up,

Time: 2715.36

is its blood pressure up?

Time: 2717.25

Is it wishing that there was pornography?

Time: 2719.532

[David and Andrew laughing]

Time: 2721.63

Something's going on, presumably,

Time: 2723.97

that's different than prior to that stimulation,

Time: 2728.98

and is it arousal?

Time: 2731.23

And what do you think it is

Time: 2732.55

about the visual factory perception of a conspecific

Time: 2737.143

that ungates this tremendous repertoire of behaviors?

Time: 2740.38

- Right, that is a central question.

Time: 2743.83

I can say, at least with respect to the fear neurons

Time: 2747.73

that sit on top of the aggression neurons,

Time: 2749.74

we know that when those neurons

Time: 2751.51

are activated optogenetically,

Time: 2754.36

in the same way we would activate the aggression neurons,

Time: 2757.66

that there's clearly an arousal process that's occurring,

Time: 2761.29

you can see the pupils dilate in the animal.

Time: 2764.32

There is an increase

Time: 2765.79

in stress hormone release into the bloodstream,

Time: 2769.09

we've shown that heart rate goes up.

Time: 2772.51

So in addition to the drive to actually freeze,

Time: 2777.28

which is what those animals do,

Time: 2779.65

there is autonomic arousal

Time: 2782.35

and neuroendocrine activation of stress responses.

Time: 2786.19

And some of that is probably shared

Time: 2789.04

by the aggression neurons and the mating neurons,

Time: 2792.19

although we haven't investigated it in as much detail,

Time: 2795.43

but I wouldn't be surprised

Time: 2797.14

because they project to many of the same regions

Time: 2800.92

that the fear neurons project to,

Time: 2804.34

which is a interesting issue

Time: 2806.47

in the context to discuss later maybe,

Time: 2808.81

in the context of why we're comfortable

Time: 2811.15

with mental illnesses that are based

Time: 2813.28

on maladaptations of fear, but not mental illnesses

Time: 2816.61

that are based on maladaptations of aggression

Time: 2819.76

if they have pretty similar circuits in the brain.

Time: 2822.49

But that's how I would imagine

Time: 2825.61

there is an arousal dimension, as you say,

Time: 2828.82

there are stress hormones that are activated, these regions,

Time: 2832.15

VMH projects to about 30 different regions in the brain,

Time: 2836.77

and it gets input from about 30 different regions.

Time: 2840.04

So I kind of see it as both an antenna

Time: 2843.43

and a broadcasting center,

Time: 2845.17

it's like a satellite dish that takes in information

Time: 2848.77

from different sensory modalities,

Time: 2851.29

smell, maybe vision, mechanosensation,

Time: 2856.03

and then it sort of synthesizes and integrates that

Time: 2859.87

into a fairly low-dimensional,

Time: 2863.05

as the computational people call it,

Time: 2865.66

representation of this pressure to attack,

Time: 2868.81

and it broadcasts that all over the brain

Time: 2871.78

to trigger all these systems

Time: 2873.7

that have to be brought into play

Time: 2875.77

if the animal is going to engage in aggression.

Time: 2878.53

Because aggression is a very risky thing

Time: 2881.35

for an animal to engage in,

Time: 2883.03

it could wind up losing

Time: 2884.68

and it could wind up getting killed,

Time: 2886.75

and so its brain constantly has to make

Time: 2890.29

a cost-benefit analysis of whether to continue on that path

Time: 2894.43

or to back off as well.

Time: 2896.41

And I think that part of this broadcasting function

Time: 2899.59

of this region is engaging all these other brain domains

Time: 2904.54

that play a role in this kind of cost-benefit analysis.

Time: 2909.28

- I want to talk more about mating behavior,

Time: 2911.32

but as a segue to that,

Time: 2913.63

as we're talking about aggression and mating behavior,

Time: 2917.02

I think, "Hormones."

Time: 2918.4

And whenever there's an opportunity on this podcast

Time: 2921.01

to shatter a common myth, I grab it.

Time: 2924.67

One of the common myths that's out there,

Time: 2926.968

and I think that persists,

Time: 2928.18

is that testosterone makes animals and humans aggressive,

Time: 2932.74

and estrogen makes animals placid and kind or emotional.

Time: 2936.82

And as we both know,

Time: 2938.2

nothing could be further from the truth,

Time: 2939.94

although there's some truth to the idea

Time: 2941.26

that these hormones are all involved.

Time: 2943.84

Robert Sapolsky supplied some information to me

Time: 2948.31

when he came on this podcast,

Time: 2949.72

that if you give people exogenous testosterone,

Time: 2952.6

it tends to make them more of the way they were before.

Time: 2955.51

If they were a jerk before, they'll become more of a jerk,

Time: 2957.61

if they were very altruistic,

Time: 2958.93

they'll become more altruistic.

Time: 2960.13

And then eventually I pointed out,

Time: 2961.547

"You'll aromatize that testosterone into estrogen

Time: 2963.82

and you'll start getting opposite effects,"

Time: 2965.08

so it's a murky space, it's not straightforward.

Time: 2967.99

But if I'm not mistaken,

Time: 2970.75

testosterone plays a role in generating aggression,

Time: 2973.45

however, the specific hormones

Time: 2977.53

that are involved in generating aggression via VMH

Time: 2981.91

are things other than testosterone.

Time: 2984.28

Can you tell us a little bit more about that?

Time: 2985.57

'Cause there's some interesting surprises in there.

Time: 2987.7

- Yeah, that's a really important question.

Time: 2990.19

So when we finally identified the neurons in VMH

Time: 2995.38

that control aggression with a molecular marker,

Time: 2998.8

we found out that that marker was the estrogen receptor.

Time: 3002.64

So that might strike you as a little strange,

Time: 3005.07

why should aggression-promoting neurons in male mice

Time: 3010.47

be labeled with the estrogen receptor?

Time: 3013.5

Other labs have shown that the estrogen receptor

Time: 3017.28

in adult male mice is necessary for aggression.

Time: 3020.94

If you knock out the gene in VMH,

Time: 3023.52

they don't fight.

Time: 3024.9

And it's been shown, and a lot of this is work

Time: 3027.36

from your colleague, Nirao Shah, at Stanford,

Time: 3029.88

who is one of my former PhD students,

Time: 3032.7

that if you castrate a mouse

Time: 3035.58

and it loses the ability to fight,

Time: 3039.18

not only can you rescue fighting

Time: 3041.79

with a testosterone implant,

Time: 3043.92

but you can rescue it with an estrogen implant.

Time: 3046.74

So you can bypass completely the requirement

Time: 3050.01

for testosterone to restore aggressiveness to the mice.

Time: 3054.09

And as you say,

Time: 3054.923

it's because many of the effects of testosterone,

Time: 3059.01

although not all, many of them are mediated

Time: 3061.59

by its conversion to estrogen,

Time: 3064.59

by a process called aromatization,

Time: 3067.38

it's carried out by an enzyme called aromatase.

Time: 3070.92

In fact, most of your listeners

Time: 3073.56

may have heard of aromatase

Time: 3075.63

'cause aromatase inhibitors are widely used in female humans

Time: 3080.52

as adjuvant chemotherapy for breast cancer.

Time: 3083.55

They are a way of reducing the production of estrogen

Time: 3088.8

by preventing testosterone

Time: 3090.96

from being converted into estrogen.

Time: 3093.12

And in fact, there are a lot of animal experiments

Time: 3095.34

showing if you give males aromatase inhibitors,

Time: 3098.88

they stop fighting,

Time: 3100.23

as well as also stop being sexually active.

Time: 3104.88

And so that's one of the counterintuitive ideas,

Time: 3108.45

and Nirao has shown that progesterone

Time: 3111.45

also seems to play a role in aggression,

Time: 3114.15

because these aggression neurons

Time: 3115.68

also express the progesterone receptor.

Time: 3118.62

So here are two hormones that are classically thought of

Time: 3122.13

as female reproductive hormones,

Time: 3124.65

this is what goes up and goes down during the estrous cycle,

Time: 3128.1

estrogen and progesterone,

Time: 3130.05

and yet, they're playing a very important role

Time: 3133.29

in controlling aggression in male mice,

Time: 3137.16

and presumably in male humans as well.

Time: 3139.95

- Fascinating, so estrogen is doing many more things

Time: 3144.72

than I think most people believe.

Time: 3145.71

- Yep. - And testosterone is doing

Time: 3148.2

maybe different and fewer things in some cases,

Time: 3150.87

and more in others.

Time: 3153.54

I've known some aggressive females

Time: 3155.91

over the time I've been alive,

Time: 3158.43

what's involved in female aggression

Time: 3160.23

that's unique from the pathways

Time: 3162.42

that generate male aggression?

Time: 3164.43

- Great question,

Time: 3165.69

so we and other labs have studied this in both mice

Time: 3170.58

and also in fruit flies.

Time: 3172.47

So one thing in mice that distinguishes aggression

Time: 3176.85

in females from males

Time: 3178.08

is that male mice are pretty much ready to fight

Time: 3180.6

at the drop of a hat,

Time: 3182.19

female mice only fight when they are nurturing

Time: 3188.04

and nursing their pups

Time: 3190.11

after they've delivered a litter.

Time: 3191.76

And there is a window there

Time: 3193.62

where they become hyper-aggressive,

Time: 3196.56

and then after their pups are weened,

Time: 3199.11

that aggressiveness goes away.

Time: 3200.67

So this is pretty remarkable

Time: 3203.16

that you take a virgin female mouse

Time: 3205.92

and expose it to a male,

Time: 3207.57

and her response is to become sexually receptive

Time: 3210.6

and to mate with him.

Time: 3212.01

And now you let her have her pups,

Time: 3214.53

and you put the same male

Time: 3216.33

or another male mouse in the cage with her,

Time: 3218.49

and instead of trying to mate with him, she attacks him.

Time: 3221.58

So there is some presumably hormonal

Time: 3224.88

and also neuronal switch that's occurring in the brain

Time: 3229.05

that switches the response of the female

Time: 3232.08

from sex to aggression

Time: 3234.99

when she goes from virginity to maternity.

Time: 3237.6

And we recently showed in a paper,

Time: 3239.7

this is work from one of my students, Mengyu Liu,

Time: 3243.09

that within VMH in females,

Time: 3245.55

there are two clearly divisible subsets

Time: 3249.93

of estrogen receptor neurons,

Time: 3252.6

and she showed that one of those subsets controls fighting

Time: 3257.22

and the other one controls mating.

Time: 3259.35

And in fact, if you stimulate

Time: 3261.09

the fighting-specific subset in a virgin,

Time: 3264.9

you can get the virgin to attack,

Time: 3267.12

which is something that we were never able to do before,

Time: 3271.2

and if you stimulate the mating one, you enhance mating.

Time: 3274.53

The reason we could never get these results

Time: 3277.02

when we stimulated the whole population

Time: 3279.93

of estrogen receptor neurons

Time: 3281.52

is that these effects are opposite and they cancel out.

Time: 3285.27

And so it turns out that if you measure the activity

Time: 3289.08

of the fighting and the mating neurons

Time: 3291.33

going from a virgin to a maternal female,

Time: 3295.2

the aggression neurons are very low in their activity

Time: 3299.79

in the virgin,

Time: 3301.14

but once the female has pups,

Time: 3304.11

the activation ability of those neurons goes way up

Time: 3308.76

and the mating neurons stay the same.

Time: 3311.04

So if you think of the balance between them like a seesaw,

Time: 3314.67

in the virgin, there is more activity

Time: 3317.64

in the mating neurons than in the fighting neurons,

Time: 3320.4

whereas in the nursing mother,

Time: 3323.01

there's more activity or more activation

Time: 3326.7

the other way around,

Time: 3327.63

the fighting neurons in the mating.

Time: 3329.075

- Mm-hmm. - Did I say,

Time: 3329.908

"Fighting and mating," the first?

Time: 3331.2

Mating neurons dominate fighting in the virgin,

Time: 3333.9

fighting neurons dominate mating in the mother.

Time: 3337.47

So that's a really cool observation,

Time: 3341.67

and it's not something that happens in males,

Time: 3344.16

and we don't know what causes that or controls that.

Time: 3348.42

Interestingly, this gets into the whole issue

Time: 3352.62

of neurons that are present in females, but not in males.

Time: 3357.15

So the field has known for a long time

Time: 3360.54

that male and female fruit flies have sex-specific neurons.

Time: 3364.68

And most of the neurons that we've identified

Time: 3367.35

in fruit flies that control fighting in males

Time: 3370.38

are male specific,

Time: 3371.73

they're not found in the female brain,

Time: 3374.37

but recently, we discovered a set

Time: 3377.19

of female-specific fighting neurons in the female brain,

Time: 3381.57

together with a couple of other laboratories.

Time: 3384.57

Now, they do share one common population of neurons

Time: 3388.86

in both male and female flies,

Time: 3391.2

that in females,

Time: 3392.61

activates the female-specific fighting neurons,

Time: 3395.34

and in males, activates the male-specific fighting neurons,

Time: 3398.88

so it's kind of a hierarchy with this common neuron on top.

Time: 3402.69

And in mice, we discovered

Time: 3405.36

that there are male-specific neurons in VMH,

Time: 3409.71

and those neurons are activated during male aggression.

Time: 3414.09

Now, the neurons that are active in females

Time: 3417

when females fight in VMH are not sex specific,

Time: 3420.99

so they are also found in males.

Time: 3423.9

So this is already showing you some complexity,

Time: 3426.39

the male mouse VMH

Time: 3428.73

has both male-specific aggression neurons

Time: 3431.64

and generic aggression neurons.

Time: 3433.86

And then the female VMH,

Time: 3435.66

the mating cells are only found in females,

Time: 3438.69

they are female specific

Time: 3440.28

and not found in the male brain.

Time: 3442.29

And so we're trying to find out

Time: 3443.85

what these sex-specific populations of neurons are doing,

Time: 3447.27

but that indicates that that is some of the mechanism

Time: 3450.72

by which different sexes show different behaviors.

Time: 3454.14

- I'm fixated on this transition

Time: 3456.9

from the virgin female mouse

Time: 3458.37

to the maternal female mouse,

Time: 3459.837

and I have a couple of questions about whether or not,

Time: 3462.48

for instance, the transition is governed

Time: 3465.21

by the presence of pups.

Time: 3466.41

So for instance, if you take a virgin female,

Time: 3468.03

she'll mate with a male,

Time: 3470.1

once she's had pups,

Time: 3471.3

she'll try and fight that male, or presumably another

Time: 3474.81

intruder female, right? - Yes,

Time: 3476.25

equally towards females and male intruders.

Time: 3479.01

- Does that require the presence of her pups?

Time: 3480.96

Meaning if you were to take those pups

Time: 3482.37

and give them to another mother,

Time: 3484.05

does she revert to the more virgin-like behavior?

Time: 3488.084

Is it triggered by lactation

Time: 3489.15

or could it actually be triggered

Time: 3490.95

by the mating behavior itself?

Time: 3492.63

'Cause it's possible for the virgin to become a non-virgin,

Time: 3496.05

but not actually have a litter of pups.

Time: 3498.21

- Right, those are all great questions,

Time: 3500.58

and we don't know the answer to most of them.

Time: 3503.46

What I can say is that a nursing mother

Time: 3507.36

doesn't have to have her pups with her in the cage

Time: 3510.6

in order to attack an intruder male

Time: 3513.63

or an intruder female,

Time: 3516.42

she is just in a state of brain

Time: 3520.14

that makes her aggressive to any intruder.

Time: 3523.38

And those aggression neurons in that female's brain

Time: 3527.16

are activated by both male and female intruders equally,

Time: 3531.81

whereas in male mice, the aggression neurons

Time: 3534.87

are only ever activated by males, not by females,

Time: 3539.1

because males are never supposed to attack females,

Time: 3542.34

they're only supposed to mate with them.

Time: 3544.56

So that's another difference

Time: 3546.15

in how those neurons are tuned

Time: 3548.58

to signals from different conspecifics.

Time: 3551.55

Does it require lactation?

Time: 3553.83

I don't know the answer to that,

Time: 3555.39

I think there are some experiments

Time: 3557.28

where people have tried to, classical experiments,

Time: 3560.4

people have tried to reproduce the changes in hormones

Time: 3564.51

that occur during pregnancy in female rats

Time: 3568.02

to see if it can make them aggressive.

Time: 3570.15

And some of those manipulations do, to some extent,

Time: 3573.81

but there's a whole biology there

Time: 3576.09

that remains to be explored

Time: 3578.25

about how much of this is hormones,

Time: 3580.65

how much of this is circuitry and electricity,

Time: 3583.83

and how much of it is other factors

Time: 3586.56

that we haven't identified yet?

Time: 3588.63

- I don't want to anthropomorphize,

Time: 3590.04

but, well, I'll just ask the question.

Time: 3592.86

So the other day, I was watching ferrets mate, right?

Time: 3597.574

They're mustelids, and they're mating behavior,

Time: 3600.57

I guess I didn't say why I was watching this,

Time: 3602.25

doesn't matter. [David laughing]

Time: 3604.65

It simply doesn't matter,

Time: 3605.88

but if one observes the mating behaviors

Time: 3608.43

of different animals,

Time: 3609.54

we know that there's a tremendous range

Time: 3611.28

of mating behaviors in humans.

Time: 3613.35

There can be no aggressive component,

Time: 3615.45

there could be an aggressive component

Time: 3616.34

in humans that have all sorts of kinks

Time: 3618.45

and fetishes and behaviors,

Time: 3619.77

and most of which probably has never been documented

Time: 3621.72

'cause most of this happens in private.

Time: 3623.67

And here, I always say on this podcast,

Time: 3624.96

any time we're talking about sexual behavior in humans,

Time: 3627.48

we're always making the presumption

Time: 3628.74

that it's consensual, age appropriate,

Time: 3631.29

context appropriate and species appropriate.

Time: 3633.42

Well, today, we're talking about a lot of different species.

Time: 3635.52

With that said, just to set context,

Time: 3638.94

I was watching this video of ferrets mating,

Time: 3641.88

and it's quite violent actually.

Time: 3644.49

There's a lot of neck biting,

Time: 3645.93

there's a lot of squealing.

Time: 3647.22

If I were going to project and anthropomorphize,

Time: 3649.38

I'd say it's not really clear they both want to be there,

Time: 3653.73

one would make that assumption.

Time: 3654.84

And of course, we don't know, we have no idea,

Time: 3657.12

this could be the ritual.

Time: 3660.33

It seems, to me, that there is some crossover

Time: 3662.76

of aggression and mating behavior circuitry

Time: 3666.48

during the act of the mating,

Time: 3668.55

and do you think that reflects

Time: 3671.04

this sort of stew of competing neurons

Time: 3675.78

that are prioritizing in real time?

Time: 3679.11

Because, of course, as states,

Time: 3681.57

they have persistence, as you point out,

Time: 3683.07

and you can imagine that states overlapping,

Time: 3685.32

four different states,

Time: 3686.25

the motivational drive to mate,

Time: 3687.75

the motivational drive to get away from this experience,

Time: 3690.33

the motivational drive to eat at some point,

Time: 3694.68

to defecate at some point,

Time: 3695.88

all of these things are competing,

Time: 3697.41

and what we're really seeing is a bias in probabilities.

Time: 3701.28

But when you look at mating behavior of various animals,

Time: 3704.1

you see an aggressive component sometimes, but not always.

Time: 3707.76

Is it species specific, is it context specific?

Time: 3711.27

And more generally, do you think that there

Time: 3714.15

is crosstalk between these different neuronal populations

Time: 3717.12

and the animal itself might be kind of confused

Time: 3718.92

about what's going on?

Time: 3719.94

- Right, great questions.

Time: 3722.25

I can't really speak to the issue

Time: 3724.11

of whether this is species specific,

Time: 3726.45

'cause I'm not a naturalist or a zoologist.

Time: 3729.18

I've seen, like you have,

Time: 3730.8

in the wild, for example, lions when they mate,

Time: 3733.98

I've seen them in Africa,

Time: 3735.12

there's often a biting component of that as well.

Time: 3738.45

One of the things that surprised us

Time: 3740.58

when we identified neurons in VMHvl

Time: 3744.39

that control aggression in males

Time: 3747.21

is that within that population,

Time: 3749.88

there is a subset of neurons

Time: 3752.16

that is activated by females

Time: 3754.8

during male-female mating encounters.

Time: 3757.32

Now, you don't generally think of mouse sex as rough sex,

Time: 3763.35

but there is a lot of what superficially

Time: 3768

looks like violent behavior sometimes,

Time: 3770.49

especially if the female rejects the male and runs away.

Time: 3774.9

And there's some evidence

Time: 3777.39

that those female-selective neurons in VMH

Time: 3781.98

are part of the mating behavior.

Time: 3786.45

If you shut 'em down,

Time: 3788.31

the animals don't mate as effectively

Time: 3791.25

as they otherwise would.

Time: 3793.23

What happens when you stimulate them we don't yet know

Time: 3796.5

because we don't have a way to specifically do that

Time: 3799.56

without activating the male aggression neurons.

Time: 3802.8

But I think they must be there for a reason

Time: 3806.4

because VMH is not traditionally the brain region

Time: 3810.39

to which male sexual behavior has been assigned.

Time: 3814.08

That's another area called the medial preoptic area,

Time: 3817.98

and there we have shown that there are neurons

Time: 3821.07

that definitely stimulate mating behavior.

Time: 3824.07

In fact, if we activate those mating neurons in a male

Time: 3827.61

while it's in the middle of attacking another male,

Time: 3830.73

it will stop fighting, start singing to that male

Time: 3834.45

and start to try to mount that male

Time: 3836.97

until we shut those neurons off.

Time: 3838.86

So those are the make-love-not-war neurons,

Time: 3842.22

and VMH are the make-war-not-love neurons,

Time: 3845.4

and there are dense interconnections

Time: 3848.13

between these two nuclei,

Time: 3850.47

which are very close to each other in the brain.

Time: 3853.86

And we've shown that some of those connections

Time: 3856.32

are mutually inhibitory,

Time: 3858.66

to prevent the animal from attacking a mate

Time: 3862.86

that it's supposed to be mating with,

Time: 3864.72

or to prevent it from mating with an animal

Time: 3868.47

it's supposed to be attacking.

Time: 3869.94

But it's also possible

Time: 3871.59

that there are some cooperative interactions

Time: 3874.65

between those structures,

Time: 3876.21

as well as antagonistic interactions,

Time: 3879.93

and the balance of whether it's the cooperative

Time: 3882.87

or antagonistic interactions

Time: 3885.09

that are firing at any given moment

Time: 3887.49

in a mating encounter, as you suggest,

Time: 3890.34

may determine whether a moment of coital bliss

Time: 3897.06

among two lions may suddenly turn into a snap

Time: 3901.77

or a growl and a baring of fangs.

Time: 3904.44

We don't know that,

Time: 3905.55

but certainly the substrate, the wiring is there

Time: 3909.42

for that to happen.

Time: 3910.95

- I'm sure people's minds are running wild with all this.

Time: 3914.07

I'll just use this as an opportunity

Time: 3915.9

to raise something I've wondered about

Time: 3918.18

for far too long, [laughing]

Time: 3919.83

which is, I have a friend who's a psychiatrist

Time: 3923.22

who works on the treatment of fetishes.

Time: 3925.56

This is not a psychiatrist that I was treated by,

Time: 3927.66

I'll just point that out. [David laughing]

Time: 3929.25

But they mentioned something

Time: 3930.93

very interesting to me long ago,

Time: 3932.76

which is that when you look at true fetishes,

Time: 3936.15

and what meets the criteria for fetish,

Time: 3938.19

that there does seem to be some,

Time: 3941.04

what one would think would be competing circuitry

Time: 3943.77

that suddenly becomes aligned.

Time: 3946.95

For instance, avoidance of feces, dead bodies, feet,

Time: 3952.8

things that are very infectious,

Time: 3955.11

typically those states of disgust

Time: 3957.93

are antagonistic to the states of desire,

Time: 3960.51

as one would hope is present during sexual behavior.

Time: 3965.73

Fetishes often involve exactly those things

Time: 3969.66

that are aversive, feet, dead bodies,

Time: 3975.06

disgusting things to most people,

Time: 3977.52

and true fetishes, in the pathologic sense,

Time: 3981.24

exist when people have, basically, a requirement

Time: 3984.84

for thinking about or even the presence

Time: 3986.49

of those ordinarily disgusting things

Time: 3989.52

in order to become sexually aroused.

Time: 3991.17

- Hmm. - As if the circuitry

Time: 3992.67

has crossed over, and the statement that wrung in my mind

Time: 3995.43

was people don't develop fetishes to mailboxes,

Time: 3999.51

or to the color red, or to random objects and things,

Time: 4004.25

they develop fetishes to things that are highly infectious

Time: 4007.25

and counter-reproductive appetitive states.

Time: 4010.213

- Hmm. - So I find that interesting,

Time: 4011.9

I don't know if you have any reflections on that

Time: 4013.85

as to why that might be.

Time: 4015.65

I'm tempted to ask whether or not

Time: 4016.82

you've ever observed fetish-like behavior in mice,

Time: 4019.43

but I find it fascinating

Time: 4021.41

that you have this area of the brain

Time: 4022.91

that's so highly concerned with the hypothalamus,

Time: 4024.58

in which you have these dense populations intermixed,

Time: 4026.99

and that the addition of a forebrain, especially in humans,

Time: 4031.07

that can think and make decisions

Time: 4032.84

could in some ways facilitate

Time: 4035.27

the expression of these primitive behaviors,

Time: 4036.95

but could also complicate

Time: 4038.51

the expression of primitive behaviors.

Time: 4040.55

- Right, I would agree.

Time: 4042.68

I think one way of looking at fetishes

Time: 4046.07

from a neurobiological standpoint

Time: 4048.65

is that they represent a kind of appetitive conditioning

Time: 4054.2

where something that is natively aversive or disgusting,

Time: 4059.39

by being repeatedly paired with a rewarding experience,

Time: 4064.82

changes its valence, its sign

Time: 4069.11

so that now it somehow produces the anticipation of reward

Time: 4075.29

the next time a person sees it.

Time: 4077.84

Now, I don't know that literature in animals,

Time: 4081.11

so I don't know if you could condition

Time: 4083.84

a mouse to eat feces, for example,

Time: 4086.45

although there are animals that are naturally coprophagic,

Time: 4090.14

and maybe mice do that occasionally, I'm not sure.

Time: 4094.64

But that is one way to think about it,

Time: 4098.03

and that could certainly involve in humans,

Time: 4100.82

the more recently evolved arts of the brain,

Time: 4103.31

the cortex that is sort of orchestrating

Time: 4107.33

both what behaviors are happening

Time: 4109.85

and whether reward states are turning on

Time: 4113.3

in association with those behaviors that are happening.

Time: 4117.02

And that's the part that I think is difficult

Time: 4121.19

and challenging to study in a mouse,

Time: 4124.31

but certainly bears thinking about,

Time: 4128.24

because it's a really interesting,

Time: 4130.79

again, sort of counterintuitive aspect.

Time: 4133.73

Again, like rough sex,

Time: 4134.96

people that want to have fighting,

Time: 4137.27

or violence, or aggressiveness

Time: 4138.92

in order to be sexually aroused, and fetishes.

Time: 4142.517

And in fact, when we made that discovery initially,

Time: 4145.94

it raised the question in my mind

Time: 4148.01

whether some people that are serial rapists, for example,

Time: 4154.79

and engage in sexual violence

Time: 4156.77

might, in some level, have their wires crossed in some way,

Time: 4161.15

that these states that are supposed to be

Time: 4163.64

pretty much separated and mutually antagonistic are not,

Time: 4167.51

and are actually more rewarding and reinforcing.

Time: 4171.47

I think it's going to be a long time

Time: 4173.84

before we have figured it out,

Time: 4175.97

but when you think about it,

Time: 4177.77

there is no treatment

Time: 4180.11

that we have for a violent sexual offender

Time: 4184.07

that eliminates the violence,

Time: 4186.56

but not the sexual desire and sexual urge,

Time: 4190.58

whether it's physical castration or chemical castration,

Time: 4194.78

it eliminates both.

Time: 4197.24

- Definitely an area that I think,

Time: 4199.16

well, human neuroscience in general

Time: 4200.81

needs a lot of tools, right?

Time: 4203.42

In terms of how to probe and manipulate neural circuitry.

Time: 4206.78

I'd love to turn to this area that you mentioned,

Time: 4209.69

the medial preoptic area.

Time: 4212

I'm fascinated by it, because just as within the VMH,

Time: 4214.817

you have these neurons for mating

Time: 4216.47

and fighting, or aggression,

Time: 4219.11

my understanding is medial preoptic area

Time: 4221.63

contains neurons for mating,

Time: 4224.06

but also for temperature regulation.

Time: 4226.31

And perhaps I'm making too much of a leap here,

Time: 4228.44

but I've always wondered about this phrase, "In heat,"

Time: 4232.04

as certainly the menstrual and or estrous cycle in females

Time: 4236.99

is related to changes in body temperature.

Time: 4238.85

In fact, measuring body temperature

Time: 4240.11

is one way that women can fairly reliably

Time: 4243.26

predict ovulation, et cetera.

Time: 4245.66

Although this is not a show about contraception,

Time: 4248.36

please rely on multiple methods [laughing] as necessary,

Time: 4250.52

don't use this discussion as your guide for contraception

Time: 4254.27

based on temperature.

Time: 4255.32

But if you stimulate certain neurons

Time: 4258.17

in the medial preoptic area,

Time: 4259.31

you can trigger dramatic changes in body temperature

Time: 4263.45

and/or mating behavior.

Time: 4266.12

What's the relationship, if any,

Time: 4267.47

between temperature and mating,

Time: 4269.18

or do we simply not know?

Time: 4271.61

- I don't know what the relationship

Time: 4274.34

is between temperature and mating neurons

Time: 4278.75

in the preoptic area.

Time: 4280.94

I suspect that they are different populations of neurons

Time: 4285.32

because it's become pretty clear that the preoptic area

Time: 4289.79

has many different subsets of neurons

Time: 4292.55

that are specifically active during different behaviors,

Time: 4296.15

even different phases of mating behavior.

Time: 4298.76

So there are mounting neurons,

Time: 4300.53

there are intromission thrusting neurons,

Time: 4303.11

and ejaculation neurons and sniffing neurons.

Time: 4306.02

- Wait, wait, so I think I've heard this before,

Time: 4308.63

but I just want to make sure that people get this

Time: 4309.887

and I want to make sure I get this.

Time: 4311.39

So you're telling me within medial preoptic area,

Time: 4315.53

there are specific neurons that if you stimulate them,

Time: 4317.78

will make males thrust as if they're mating?

Time: 4320.75

- No, so this is not based on stimulation experiments.

Time: 4325.88

- Mm. - It's based on

Time: 4326.713

imaging experiments right now. - I see, I see.

Time: 4328.91

- That we see when we look in the preoptic area

Time: 4331.76

at what neurons are active

Time: 4334.1

during different phases of aggression,

Time: 4337.22

we see that there are different neurons

Time: 4339.26

that are active during sniffing, mounting,

Time: 4342.47

thrusting and ejaculation,

Time: 4344.87

and they become repeatedly activated

Time: 4347.99

each time the animal goes through that cycle.

Time: 4350.3

- During mating, yeah. - During the mating cycle.

Time: 4352.88

There are also some neurons there

Time: 4354.38

that are active during aggression, which are distinct,

Time: 4357.35

and we don't know whether those neurons

Time: 4359.66

are there to promote aggression

Time: 4362.21

or to inhibit mating when animals are fighting.

Time: 4366.23

We have some evidence that suggest it may be the latter,

Time: 4369.5

but we don't know for sure yet.

Time: 4371.39

The thermosensitive neurons are really interesting,

Time: 4374.24

because you mentioned the phrase, "In heat,"

Time: 4377.45

and then in the context of aggression,

Time: 4379.79

you talk about hotblooded people or hotheads,

Time: 4382.91

there's just recently a paper

Time: 4384.41

showing there are thermoregulatory neurons in VMH as well.

Time: 4388.88

So all of these homeostatic systems

Time: 4391.76

for metabolic control and temperature control

Time: 4395.15

are intermingled in these nuclei,

Time: 4398.6

these zones that control these basic survival behaviors,

Time: 4402.86

like mating and aggression and predator defense.

Time: 4406.85

And I would imagine that the thermal regulation

Time: 4411.23

is tightly connected to energy expenditure,

Time: 4415.55

and that, again, these neurons are mixed together

Time: 4419.15

to facilitate integration of all these signals

Time: 4423.02

by the brain in some way that we don't understand

Time: 4426.59

to maintain the proper balance

Time: 4428.48

between energy conservation and energy consumption

Time: 4433.58

during this particular behavior or that behavior.

Time: 4436.76

I mean, I've always been fascinated by the question,

Time: 4438.92

why is it that violence goes up in the summertime

Time: 4443

when the temperatures are high?

Time: 4444.83

Does it really have something to do

Time: 4447.2

with the idea that increased temperature increases violence?

Time: 4452.06

It seems hard to believe because we're homeothermic

Time: 4455.33

and we pretty much stay around 98.6 Fahrenheit.

Time: 4460.58

It could be other social reasons why that happens,

Time: 4463.61

people are outside, out on the street,

Time: 4465.56

bumping into each other,

Time: 4467.03

but I think there could well be something

Time: 4470.27

that ties thermoregulation to aggressiveness,

Time: 4473.99

as well as to mating behavior.

Time: 4478.16

- Fascinating, yeah.

Time: 4479.81

I ask in the hopes that maybe in the years to come

Time: 4483.35

your lab will parse some of the temperature relationships.

Time: 4486.38

And I realize it could be also regulated

Time: 4489.32

by hormones in general,

Time: 4490.43

so it's tapping into two systems

Time: 4491.84

for completely different reasons,

Time: 4493.07

but anyway, an area that intrigues me,

Time: 4496.97

because of this notion of hotheadedness.

Time: 4498.037

- Right. - Or cool, calm and collected.

Time: 4500.3

And also the fact that,

Time: 4502.517

and I probably should've asked about this earlier,

Time: 4504.35

that arousal itself is tethered

Time: 4507.2

to the whole mating and reproductive process.

Time: 4509.6

I mean, without a sort of seesawing back

Time: 4512.03

between the sympathetic and parasympathetic

Time: 4515.15

arousal, relaxed states,

Time: 4517.31

there is no mating that will take place.

Time: 4520.7

So it's fascinating the way

Time: 4521.99

these different competing forces and seesaws operate.

Time: 4525.74

Several times during the discussion so far,

Time: 4527.9

we've hit on this idea

Time: 4530.6

that the same behavior can reflect different states,

Time: 4534.74

and different states can converge

Time: 4536.45

on multiple behaviors as well.

Time: 4539.33

You had a paper not long ago about mounting behavior,

Time: 4543.29

which I found fascinating.

Time: 4545.51

Maybe you could tell us about that result,

Time: 4547.7

because, to me, it really speaks

Time: 4549.17

to the fact that mounting behavior

Time: 4551.75

can, in one context, be sexual,

Time: 4553.85

and in another context, actually be related to,

Time: 4556.43

we presume, dominance.

Time: 4557.93

And I think that my friends who practice jujitsu,

Time: 4563.78

when I talk about that result,

Time: 4564.83

they say, "Of course, mounting the other person

Time: 4567.05

and dominating them,

Time: 4568.82

there's nothing sexual about it,"

Time: 4570.2

it's about overtaking them physically,

Time: 4572.39

literally being on their neck side,

Time: 4574.31

as opposed to lying on their own back.

Time: 4577.55

- [David] Hmm.

Time: 4578.383

- Just fascinating,

Time: 4579.29

very primitive. - Hmm.

Time: 4580.123

- And yet, I think speaks to this idea

Time: 4583.07

that mounting behavior might be

Time: 4584.69

one of the most fundamental ways

Time: 4586.43

in which animals and perhaps even humans

Time: 4588.8

express dominance and/or sexual interactions.

Time: 4594.02

- Yep, and that's a fascinating question,

Time: 4597.5

and it was harder to figure out

Time: 4599.84

than you might've thought.

Time: 4601.22

So there's been this debate for a long time in the field,

Time: 4604.64

when you see two male mice mounting each other,

Time: 4608.69

is this homosexual behavior,

Time: 4610.85

is this a case of mistaken sexual identification,

Time: 4614.27

or is this dominance behavior?

Time: 4616.4

And if you train an AI algorithm

Time: 4619.52

to try to distinguish male-male mounting

Time: 4623.78

from male-female mounting,

Time: 4625.34

it does not do a very good job,

Time: 4627.14

because motorically, those behaviors look so similar.

Time: 4631.43

And so how did we wind up figuring out

Time: 4636.32

that most male-male mounting is dominance mounting?

Time: 4640.91

There are two important clues,

Time: 4644.03

one is the context,

Time: 4646.82

and so male-male mounting

Time: 4649.28

tends to be more prominent among mice

Time: 4652.37

when they haven't had a lot of fighting experience.

Time: 4655.58

And then as they become more experienced in fighting,

Time: 4660.41

they will show relatively less mounting

Time: 4663.71

towards the other male and more attack,

Time: 4666.08

and they'll transition quickly from mounting to attack,

Time: 4670.13

and so the mounting is always seen

Time: 4673.19

in this context of an overall aggressive interaction.

Time: 4677.99

And then the second thing,

Time: 4679.4

which, believe it or not, was suggested

Time: 4681.38

by a computational, theoretical person in my lab,

Time: 4685.46

Ann Kennedy, who now has her own lab at Northwestern.

Time: 4689.15

She said, "Well, males are known to sing

Time: 4692.48

when they mount females, ultrasonic vocalizations,

Time: 4695.6

why don't you see what kinds of songs they're singing

Time: 4698.96

when they're mounting males?

Time: 4700.28

Maybe it's a different kind of song."

Time: 4701.78

Well, what we found out is,

Time: 4702.95

they don't sing at all when they're mounting a male,

Time: 4706.82

so you can easily distinguish

Time: 4709.55

whether mounting behavior by a male mouse

Time: 4713.6

is reproductive or agonistic, aggressive,

Time: 4717.71

according to whether it's accompanied

Time: 4720.14

by ultrasonic vocalizations or not.

Time: 4723.56

And it turns out that different brain regions

Time: 4727.04

are maximally active

Time: 4729.05

during these different types of mounting.

Time: 4731.03

So VMH, the aggression locus

Time: 4735.08

is actually active during dominance mounting,

Time: 4738.35

and you can stimulate mounting, dominance mounting,

Time: 4742.31

if you weakly activate VMH,

Time: 4745.4

whereas MPOA is most strongly activated

Time: 4748.82

during sexual mounting,

Time: 4750.77

and that's always accompanied

Time: 4752.78

by the ultrasonic vocalization.

Time: 4754.67

So this shows how difficult and dangerous it can be

Time: 4758.48

to try to infer an animal's state, or intent, or emotion,

Time: 4763.37

from the behavior that it's exhibiting

Time: 4765.86

because the same behavior can mean very different things

Time: 4769.1

depending on the context of the interaction with the animal.

Time: 4771.81

- And I would say, even more so

Time: 4773.84

with when that animal is a human or is multiple humans.

Time: 4777.62

- That's right, and there are many examples,

Time: 4780.41

animals show chasing to obtain food,

Time: 4784.4

a prey animal that they're going to kill and eat,

Time: 4787.07

and they show chasing to obtain a mate

Time: 4789.83

that they're going to have sex with.

Time: 4791.57

And so the intent of the chasing is completely different,

Time: 4795.53

and we don't know in all these cases

Time: 4797.36

whether there are separate circuits

Time: 4799.49

or common circuits that are being activated.

Time: 4802.67

- I'm obsessed with dogs and dog breeds

Time: 4804.77

and et cetera, et cetera,

Time: 4806.09

and one thing I can tell you

Time: 4808.7

is that female dogs will mount and thrust.

Time: 4812.72

We had a female pit bull mix, a very sweet dog,

Time: 4817.01

but in observing her,

Time: 4819.807

it convinced me that one can never assume

Time: 4822.83

that male dogs are more aggressive than female dogs.

Time: 4826.4

It turns out, in talking to people

Time: 4827.447

who are quite skilled at dog genetics and dog breeding,

Time: 4831.26

that there's a dominance hierarchy within a litter

Time: 4833.87

and it crosses over male-female delineations.

Time: 4839.42

So you can get a female in the litter that's very dominant

Time: 4841.79

and a male that's very subordinate,

Time: 4843.17

and no one really knows what relates to.

Time: 4845.84

This is also why little dogs

Time: 4846.98

sometimes will get right up in the face

Time: 4848.87

of a big Doberman Pinscher. - Mm.

Time: 4850.58

- And just start barking,

Time: 4851.413

which is an idiotic thing for it to do,

Time: 4853.04

but they can be dominant over a much larger dog.

Time: 4856.16

- Hmm. - Very strange,

Time: 4857.66

to me anyway.

Time: 4859.46

Female-female mounting, do you observe it in mice?

Time: 4863.09

Are there known circuits,

Time: 4864.62

and what evokes female-female mounting,

Time: 4867.23

or female-to-male mounting if it occurs?

Time: 4870.41

- Good, yes, there are clear examples of females

Time: 4876.41

displaying male-type mounting behavior

Time: 4879.02

towards other females.

Time: 4880.52

We see this most commonly in the lab

Time: 4883.91

where we are housing females with their sisters,

Time: 4887.39

say three or four in a cage,

Time: 4889.55

we take one out and we have her mate with a male,

Time: 4893.45

where the male's doing the mounting,

Time: 4895.22

now we take that female

Time: 4897.02

and we put her back in the cage with her litter mates

Time: 4899.93

and she starts mounting them.

Time: 4902.423

Now, what the function of that is,

Time: 4905.93

if it has any function,

Time: 4907.7

or what it means, what's driving it, we don't know,

Time: 4911.12

but we do know that if we stimulate

Time: 4915.601

the neurons that control mounting in males

Time: 4918.23

in the medial preoptic area,

Time: 4919.76

if we stimulate that same population in females,

Time: 4923.72

it evokes male-type mounting

Time: 4927.14

towards either a male or a female target.

Time: 4929.66

In fact, we have a movie

Time: 4931.4

where we have a female

Time: 4933.92

that has just been mounted by a male,

Time: 4936.53

so the male's on top and she's underneath,

Time: 4939.74

and we stimulate that region of MPOA in the female.

Time: 4944.12

And she crawls out from underneath the male

Time: 4947.39

who has just mounted her,

Time: 4949.07

circles around behind him

Time: 4951.5

and climbs up on top of him

Time: 4953.39

and starts to try to mount him and thrust at him.

Time: 4956.51

- That has a name online, it's called a switch.

Time: 4959.127

[Andrew laughing] - Is that right? [laughing]

Time: 4960.65

- [Andrew] Don't ask me how I know that.

Time: 4961.76

- Okay.

Time: 4962.84

- But it's a pretty, yeah, it's a term that you hear.

Time: 4968.09

You also hear the term topping from the bottom,

Time: 4970.67

which it sounds like that is a literal topping

Time: 4972.44

from the bottom. - I see.

Time: 4973.273

- That's a more of a psychological phrase,

Time: 4974.93

from what I hear.

Time: 4975.77

I have friends that are educating me in this language,

Time: 4979.91

mostly because I find

Time: 4981.29

this kind of neurobiological discussion fascinating.

Time: 4984.727

And at some point, right?

Time: 4987.11

I attempt, in my mind, to superimpose observations

Time: 4989.93

from the online communities.

Time: 4991.32

- Yeah. - That I'm told about

Time: 4992.9

and asked about to this,

Time: 4994.46

but I should point out, it's always dangerous,

Time: 4997.01

and in fact, inappropriate

Time: 4998.24

to make a one-to-one link. - Yes.

Time: 5000.73

- Humans, they maintain all the same neural circuitry

Time: 5003.61

and pathways that we're talking about today in mice,

Time: 5006.01

but that forebrain does allow for context, et cetera.

Time: 5010.21

- Yep. - Yeah.

Time: 5011.043

- So what the function is of female mounting,

Time: 5016.42

I don't know, it could be a type of dominance display.

Time: 5019.54

It's hard to measure that

Time: 5021.04

because people haven't worked

Time: 5022.39

on female-dominance hierarchies

Time: 5025.06

to the same extent that they've worked

Time: 5026.71

on male-dominance hierarchies,

Time: 5029.05

but it indicates that the circuits for male-type mounting

Time: 5034.06

are there in females,

Time: 5035.8

as early work from Catherine Dulac suggested some years ago.

Time: 5040.24

- Fascinating, fascinating.

Time: 5041.68

I love that paper because, as you pointed out for chase,

Time: 5046.93

for mounting behavior, we see it

Time: 5048.61

and we think one thing specifically,

Time: 5051.4

and after hearing this result,

Time: 5052.78

actually, I'm not a big fan of fight sports.

Time: 5054.91

I watch them occasionally 'cause friends are into them,

Time: 5056.83

but I've seen boxing matches, MMA matches,

Time: 5061.3

where at the end of a round,

Time: 5063.1

if someone felt that they dominated,

Time: 5065.08

they will do the unsportsmanlike thing

Time: 5067.78

of thrusting on the back of the other person

Time: 5070.24

before they get off. - Really?

Time: 5071.073

- Almost like, "I dominated you, and I'm,"

Time: 5072.97

so mimicking sexual-like behavior,

Time: 5075.04

but there's no reason to think that it's sexual,

Time: 5077.32

but they're sending a message. - Yeah.

Time: 5078.46

- Of dominance is what it implies.

Time: 5081.58

I'd love to talk about something

Time: 5084.37

slightly off from this circuitry,

Time: 5086.2

but I think that's related to the circuitry,

Time: 5088.21

at least in some way,

Time: 5089.26

which is this structure that I've always been fascinated by

Time: 5092.5

and I can't figure out what the hell it's for,

Time: 5095.082

'cause it seems to be involved in everything,

Time: 5096.07

which is the PAG, the periaqueductal gray,

Time: 5100.27

which is a little bit further back in the brain,

Time: 5101.77

for people that don't know.

Time: 5103.33

It's been studied in the context of pain,

Time: 5105.01

it's been studied in the context

Time: 5106.57

of the so-called lordosis response,

Time: 5108.49

the receptivity or arching of the back of the female

Time: 5110.92

to receive intromission and mating from the male.

Time: 5114.52

How should we think about PAG?

Time: 5117.37

Clearly, it can't be involved in everything,

Time: 5119.56

I'm guessing it's at least as complex

Time: 5121.6

as some of these other regions

Time: 5122.59

that we've been talking about,

Time: 5123.55

different types of neurons controlling different things,

Time: 5125.38

but how does PAG play into this?

Time: 5127.81

In particular, I want to know,

Time: 5129.94

is there some mechanism of pain modulation and control

Time: 5133.48

during fighting and/or mating?

Time: 5137.35

And the reason I ask is that,

Time: 5139.57

while I'm not a combat sports person,

Time: 5142.21

years ago, I did a little bit of martial arts,

Time: 5144.52

and it always was impressive to me

Time: 5146.77

how little it hurt to get punched during a fight

Time: 5149.29

and how much it hurt afterwards, [laughing] right?

Time: 5151.9

So there clearly is some endogenous pain control.

Time: 5154.9

- Yep. - That then wears off,

Time: 5156.37

and then you feel beat up.

Time: 5157.9

- [David] Yep.

Time: 5158.733

- Or at least, in my case, I felt beat up.

Time: 5160.72

What's PAG doing vis-a-vis pain,

Time: 5164.17

and what's pain doing vis-a-vis these other behaviors?

Time: 5166.27

- Good, good.

Time: 5167.44

So I think of PAG

Time: 5170.11

like a old-fashioned telephone switchboard,

Time: 5173.95

where there are calls coming in,

Time: 5177.43

and then the cables have to be punched into the right hole

Time: 5180.85

to get the information,

Time: 5182.38

to be routed to the right recipient on the other end of it,

Time: 5186.49

because pretty much every type of innate behavior

Time: 5190.6

you can think of has had the PAG implicated.

Time: 5194.047

And there's a whole literature

Time: 5196.39

showing the involvement of the PAG in fear,

Time: 5199.36

different regions of the PAG,

Time: 5201.64

the dorsal PAG is involved

Time: 5203.38

in panic-like behavior, running away,

Time: 5206.35

the ventral PAG is involved in freezing behavior.

Time: 5210.91

Both the MPOA and VMH send projections to the PAG,

Time: 5216.46

to different regions of the PAG.

Time: 5218.86

So in cross-section, I hate to say this,

Time: 5222.79

but in cross-section, the PAG kind of looks like

Time: 5225.58

the water in a toilet

Time: 5226.93

when you're standing over an open toilet bowl.

Time: 5229.75

- Mm-hmm. - And if you imagine

Time: 5231.46

a clock face projected onto that,

Time: 5235

it's like the PAG has sectors,

Time: 5238.66

from one to 12, maybe even more of them,

Time: 5241.24

and in each of those sectors,

Time: 5242.89

you find different neurons

Time: 5244.33

from the hypothalamus are projecting.

Time: 5246.85

So could turn out that there is a topographic arrangement

Time: 5251.17

along the dorsal-ventral axis of the PAG

Time: 5254.17

and the medial-lateral axis of the PAG

Time: 5256.99

that determines the type of behavior

Time: 5259.84

that will be emitted when neurons

Time: 5262.24

in that region are stimulated.

Time: 5263.95

And I think sort of all of the evidence

Time: 5266.02

is pointing in that direction,

Time: 5267.79

but by no means, has it been mapped out.

Time: 5270.52

Now, the thing that you mentioned about it not hurting

Time: 5274

when you got beat up during martial arts,

Time: 5276.88

there is a well-known phenomenon

Time: 5279.22

called fear-induced analgesia,

Time: 5283.78

where when an animal is in a high state of fear,

Time: 5288.7

like if it's trying to defend itself,

Time: 5291.19

there is a suppression of pain responses,

Time: 5296.17

and I'm not sure completely about the mechanisms

Time: 5300.64

and how well that's understood,

Time: 5302.68

but for example, the adrenal gland has a peptide in it

Time: 5308.74

that is released from the adrenal medulla,

Time: 5312.16

which controls the fight-or-flight responses,

Time: 5315.01

and that peptide has analgesic activities.

Time: 5318.91

Now, whether. - May I ask what that

Time: 5319.743

peptide is? - It's called

Time: 5320.576

bovine adrenal medullary peptide of 22 amino acid residues.

Time: 5325.45

And I only know about it

Time: 5327.16

because it activates a receptor

Time: 5329.47

that we discovered many years ago

Time: 5331.54

that's involved in pain,

Time: 5333.22

and we thought it promoted pain,

Time: 5334.78

but it turns out that this actually inhibits pain,

Time: 5337.66

it's like an endogenous analgesic.

Time: 5340.42

Whether this is happening, this type of analgesia

Time: 5345.37

is happening when an animal

Time: 5347.38

is engaged in offensive aggression

Time: 5350.17

or in mating behavior, I don't know,

Time: 5354.4

but it certainly is possible.

Time: 5356.38

And I don't know whether these analgesic mechanisms

Time: 5360.46

are happening in the PAG,

Time: 5362.44

they could also be happening a little further down

Time: 5365.5

in the spinal cord.

Time: 5366.55

The PAG is really continuous with the spinal cord,

Time: 5369.97

if you just follow it down towards the tail of an animal,

Time: 5373.87

you will wind up in the spinal cord.

Time: 5378.291

And so it could be that there are influences acting

Time: 5381.19

at many levels on pain in the PAG

Time: 5383.98

and in the spinal cord as well.

Time: 5386.32

And it may well be known, I just don't know it,

Time: 5389.05

I want to distinguish clearly between things

Time: 5391.75

that are not known, that I know are unknown,

Time: 5395.05

which is in a fairly small area where I have expertise,

Time: 5398.74

from things that may be known,

Time: 5400.27

but I'm ignorant of them,

Time: 5401.77

because I just don't have a broad enough

Time: 5403.51

knowledge base to know that.

Time: 5404.51

- Sure, we appreciate those delineations.

Time: 5409.24

Thank you, PAG, I think this description of it

Time: 5411.67

as an old-fashioned telephone switchboard,

Time: 5414.04

and now every time I look into the toilet, I'll think about

Time: 5416.92

the periaqueductal gray. - [laughing] That's right.

Time: 5418.15

- [Andrew] And every time I see an image

Time: 5419.2

of periaqueductal gray,

Time: 5420.22

I'll think about a toilet. - That's right. [laughing]

Time: 5421.053

- That is an excellent description,

Time: 5423.64

because, in fact, I drew a circle

Time: 5425.17

with a little thing at the bottom.

Time: 5426.25

And well, I'll put a post or link to a picture of PAG

Time: 5430.24

and you'll understand why David and I are chuckling here,

Time: 5433.21

because, indeed, it looks like a toilet,

Time: 5435.37

when staring into a toilet.

Time: 5438.28

Tell us about tachykinin,

Time: 5440.32

I've talked about this a couple times

Time: 5441.64

on different podcast episodes

Time: 5442.9

because of its relationship to social isolation,

Time: 5447.37

and in part, because the podcast was launched

Time: 5451.09

during a time when there was more social isolation.

Time: 5454.63

My understanding is that tachykinin,

Time: 5456.79

and you'll tell us what it is in a moment,

Time: 5459.31

is present in flies and mice and in humans,

Time: 5461.8

and may do similar things in those species.

Time: 5465.82

- That's right, so tachykinin refers to a family

Time: 5471.01

of related neuropeptides.

Time: 5472.87

So these are brain chemicals,

Time: 5476.11

they're different from dopamine and serotonin

Time: 5479.35

in that they're not small, organic molecules,

Time: 5483.04

they're actually short pieces of protein

Time: 5485.83

that are directly encoded by genes

Time: 5488.74

that are active in specific neurons

Time: 5491.14

and not in others.

Time: 5492.28

And when those neurons are active,

Time: 5494.05

those neuropeptides are released

Time: 5496.27

together with classical transmitters,

Time: 5498.85

like glutamate, whatever.

Time: 5500.53

Tachykinins have been famously implicated in pain,

Time: 5505.48

particularly Tachykinin-I,

Time: 5507.91

which is called Substance P,

Time: 5510.34

one of the original pain modulating,

Time: 5513.31

this is something that promotes inflammatory pain.

Time: 5517.45

But there are other tachykinin genes,

Time: 5519.97

in mice, there are two,

Time: 5521.62

in humans, I think there are three,

Time: 5523.93

and in Drosophila, there's one.

Time: 5526.36

And the way we got into tachykinins

Time: 5529.21

is from studying aggression in flies.

Time: 5532.51

We thought, since neuropeptides

Time: 5535.69

have this remarkable parallel evolutionary conservation

Time: 5540.22

of structure and function,

Time: 5542.02

like Neuropeptide Y controls feeding

Time: 5545.41

in worms, in flies and mice and in people.

Time: 5548.95

Oxytocin-like peptides control reproduction

Time: 5552.16

in worms and mice and in people.

Time: 5555.55

We thought we might find peptides that control aggression

Time: 5559.24

in flies and in people,

Time: 5560.71

and so we did a screen, unbiased screen of peptides,

Time: 5564.67

and found, indeed, that one of the tachykinins,

Time: 5568.737

Drosophila tachykinin, those neurons when you activate them

Time: 5572.98

strongly promote aggression,

Time: 5574.75

and it depends on the release of tachykinin.

Time: 5577.51

Now, the interesting thing is that,

Time: 5579.37

in flies, just like in people

Time: 5581.98

and practically any other social animal

Time: 5585.43

that shows aggression,

Time: 5586.9

social isolation increases aggressiveness.

Time: 5590.38

So putting a violent prisoner in solitary confinement

Time: 5594.88

is absolutely the worst, most counterproductive thing

Time: 5597.73

you could do to them.

Time: 5599.02

And indeed, we found in flies

Time: 5601.36

that social isolation increases the level

Time: 5604.66

of tachykinin in the brain,

Time: 5606.85

and if we shut that gene down,

Time: 5609.13

it prevents the isolation from increasing aggression.

Time: 5612.61

So since my lab also works on mice,

Time: 5615.64

it was natural to see whether tachykinins

Time: 5618.94

might be upregulated in social isolation

Time: 5622.24

and whether they play a role in aggression.

Time: 5624.58

And this is work done

Time: 5625.63

by a former postdoc, Moriel Zelikowsky,

Time: 5628.03

now at University of Salt Lake City in Utah,

Time: 5630.85

and she found, remarkably,

Time: 5632.95

that when mice are socially isolated for two weeks,

Time: 5636.82

there is this massive upregulation

Time: 5640.18

of Tachykinin-II in their brain.

Time: 5643.39

In fact, if you tag the peptide

Time: 5646.27

with a green fluorescent protein

Time: 5648.85

from a jellyfish, genetically,

Time: 5650.74

the brain looks green when the mice are socially isolated

Time: 5654.91

'cause there's so much of this stuff released.

Time: 5657.7

And she went on to show that that increase in tachykinin

Time: 5664.06

is responsible for the effect of social isolation

Time: 5667.78

to increase aggressiveness

Time: 5669.79

and to increase fear

Time: 5671.59

and to increase anxiety.

Time: 5673.21

And in fact, there are drugs

Time: 5675.22

that block the receptor for tachykinin

Time: 5678.13

which were tested in humans and abandoned

Time: 5681.13

because they had no efficacy

Time: 5683.05

in the tests that they were analyzed for.

Time: 5685.54

If you give those drugs to a socially isolated mouse,

Time: 5689.71

it blocks all of the effects of social isolation.

Time: 5693.37

It blocks the aggression,

Time: 5695.29

it blocks the increased fear and the increased anxiety,

Time: 5698.89

and Moriel described it, "The mice just look chill."

Time: 5702.49

It's not a sedative, which is really important,

Time: 5705.16

it's not that the mice are going to sleep.

Time: 5708.19

Most remarkably is, once you socially isolate a mouse

Time: 5713.17

and it becomes aggressive,

Time: 5714.58

you can never put it back in its cage

Time: 5717.76

with its brothers from its litter

Time: 5719.62

because it will kill them all overnight,

Time: 5722.08

but if you give it this drug,

Time: 5724.36

which is called osanetant, that blocks Tachykinin-II,

Time: 5729.64

that mouse can be returned to the cage with its brothers

Time: 5733.27

and will not attack them,

Time: 5734.77

and seems to be happy about that for the rest of the time.

Time: 5738.67

So this is an incredibly powerful effect of this drug,

Time: 5742.51

and I've been really interested

Time: 5744.76

in trying to get pharmaceutical companies to test this drug,

Time: 5749.26

which has a really good safety profile in humans,

Time: 5752.95

in testing it in people

Time: 5755.38

who are subjected to social isolation stress

Time: 5758.44

or bereavement stress.

Time: 5760.57

And this is one of the areas

Time: 5762.94

where I learned an eye-opening lesson,

Time: 5766.9

as a basic scientist who naively thought

Time: 5769.66

that if you make a discovery

Time: 5771.43

and it has translational applications to humans,

Time: 5775.18

that pharmaceutical companies

Time: 5776.86

are going to be falling all over themselves to try it.

Time: 5779.98

And they are not interested,

Time: 5782.11

because once burned, twice shy,

Time: 5785.56

these drugs were tested for efficacy in schizophrenia.

Time: 5790.21

I have no idea why,

Time: 5792.22

there's very little preclinical data to suggest that.

Time: 5795.31

Not surprisingly, they failed.

Time: 5799.517

When a drug fails in clinical trials in Phase 3,

Time: 5802.48

it costs $100 million to the company

Time: 5807.61

that carried out that clinical trial.

Time: 5809.32

So there's a huge slag heap of discarded pharmaceuticals,

Time: 5814.24

many of them inhibitors of neuropeptide action,

Time: 5818.29

that could be useful in other indications,

Time: 5822.22

such as the one we discovered,

Time: 5824.08

but there's a huge economic disincentive

Time: 5827.5

for pharmaceutical companies to test them again,

Time: 5831.85

because the conclusion that they drew

Time: 5834.31

from all these failed tests,

Time: 5836.14

particularly in the 2010s and before that,

Time: 5840.22

is that the reason they failed

Time: 5842.74

is because animal experiments with drugs

Time: 5846.55

don't predict how humans will respond to the drugs,

Time: 5851.2

and therefore, we shouldn't try to extrapolate

Time: 5855.1

from any other data that we get from animal experiments,

Time: 5858.76

mouse or rat experiments to humans,

Time: 5860.83

because they'll lead us down the wrong track,

Time: 5863.53

and I think that that is probably wrong.

Time: 5865.96

In some cases, it may be right,

Time: 5868.24

but in other cases, there's good reason to think,

Time: 5871.18

because these brain regions and molecules

Time: 5874.03

are so evolutionarily conserved

Time: 5876.73

that they ought to be playing a similar role in humans.

Time: 5880.51

In fact, there is a paper showing that

Time: 5884.68

in humans that have borderline personality disorder,

Time: 5889.81

there's a strong correlation

Time: 5891.49

between their self-reported level of aggressiveness

Time: 5895.51

and serum levels of a tachykinin,

Time: 5898.63

in this case, Tachykinin-I,

Time: 5900.67

as detected by radioimmunoassay.

Time: 5902.86

This is work of Emil Coccaro,

Time: 5905.08

who's a clinical psychiatrist at the University of Chicago.

Time: 5908.62

So there is a smoking gun in the case of humans as well.

Time: 5913.21

And I was actually trying to interest

Time: 5916.96

a pharmaceutical company

Time: 5918.79

that was testing these drugs,

Time: 5921.7

actually, for treatment of hot flashes

Time: 5924.91

in females, in humans,

Time: 5926.86

where there is actually good animal data

Time: 5929.86

to think that it might be useful,

Time: 5931.93

but I realized that this clinical trial

Time: 5935.11

was going on during the COVID pandemic.

Time: 5938.68

And I approached him and said,

Time: 5939.887

"Look, nature may have actually done for you

Time: 5942.82

the experiment that I want you to do,

Time: 5945.01

'cause some of the people

Time: 5946.06

who are getting drug or placebo

Time: 5948.1

are going to have been socially isolated

Time: 5950.59

and some of them will have not.

Time: 5952.09

Why don't you get them to fill out questionnaires

Time: 5954.76

and see whether the ones

Time: 5956.02

who were given the drug and socially isolated

Time: 5958.87

felt less stressed and less anxious

Time: 5961.51

than the ones who were not socially isolated?"

Time: 5964

And they would not touch it,

Time: 5966.13

because they're in the middle of a clinical trial

Time: 5969.28

for a different indication for this drug,

Time: 5971.77

and they have to report any observation

Time: 5975.19

that they make about that drug

Time: 5977.32

in their patient population.

Time: 5978.97

So if they were to ask these questions

Time: 5981.43

and get an unfavorable answer,

Time: 5983.927

"Oh my God, I felt even worse

Time: 5985.72

when I took this drug and I was isolated,"

Time: 5987.97

they would be obliged to report that to the FDA

Time: 5991.18

and that could torpedo the chances

Time: 5993.16

for the drug being approved

Time: 5994.81

in the thing that it was in clinical trials for.

Time: 5996.88

So it's better not to ask and not to know

Time: 6002.37

than it is to try to find out more information

Time: 6005.16

that could lead to another clinical indication.

Time: 6007.77

So I remain convinced that this family of drugs

Time: 6012.66

could have very powerful uses

Time: 6015.21

in treating some forms of stress-induced anxiety

Time: 6019.53

or aggressiveness in humans,

Time: 6021.24

but it's just very difficult, for economic reasons,

Time: 6024.42

to find a way to get somebody to test that.

Time: 6026.97

- Yeah, a true shame that these companies won't do this,

Time: 6030.48

and especially given the fact that many of these drugs exist

Time: 6034.17

and their safety profiles are established,

Time: 6036.9

'cause that's always

Time: 6037.89

a serious consideration. - Yep.

Time: 6039.57

- When embarking on a clinical trial.

Time: 6042.63

Perhaps in hearing this discussion,

Time: 6044.46

someone out there will understand

Time: 6046.32

the key importance of this and will reach out to us,

Time: 6049.62

we'll provide ways to do that,

Time: 6052.29

to get such a study going in humans.

Time: 6055.17

Because I think if enough laboratories

Time: 6057.51

ran small-scale clinical trials,

Time: 6059.82

pharma certainly would perk up their ears, right?

Time: 6062.04

I mean, they're so strategic.

Time: 6064.05

- Yep. - Sometimes to their own.

Time: 6065.64

- I mean, I would like to say also,

Time: 6067.56

I'd like to see this tested on pets.

Time: 6070.32

I mean, there's a huge number of pets right now

Time: 6073.05

that are suffering separation anxiety

Time: 6075.63

because humans bought them to keep them company

Time: 6078.54

during the COVID pandemic,

Time: 6080.016

and now they're home alone. - And now they're home alone,

Time: 6081.729

yeah. - Okay?

Time: 6082.562

And if this thing works in mice,

Time: 6084.54

there's certainly a higher chance

Time: 6086.37

it's going to work in dogs or in cats

Time: 6088.98

than it is going to work in humans.

Time: 6090.75

And if it did, that would be even more encouragement

Time: 6093.72

to continue along those lines.

Time: 6095.67

People sometimes forget that although we work on animals

Time: 6099.18

and we ultimately want to understand humans,

Time: 6101.49

we care about how our results

Time: 6103.68

apply to the welfare of animals as well,

Time: 6106.74

and particularly domestic pets,

Time: 6108.75

which is a multi-billion-dollar industry

Time: 6111.96

in this country.

Time: 6112.89

So if there is ways that they can be made to feel better

Time: 6116.31

when they're separated from their owners,

Time: 6119.52

that would certainly be a good thing.

Time: 6121.29

- Absolutely, we will put out the call,

Time: 6123.81

we are putting out the call,

Time: 6125.04

and I know for sure there will be a response.

Time: 6130.5

Just underscoring what we've been talking about even more,

Time: 6133.41

every time we hear about a school shooting,

Time: 6136.71

like in Texas recently,

Time: 6138.03

or I happened to be in New York

Time: 6139.65

during the time when there was a subway shooting.

Time: 6143.01

For whatever reason, I listened to the book about Columbine,

Time: 6148.14

that went into a very detailed way

Time: 6150

about the origin of those boys that committed that,

Time: 6153.27

and every single time,

Time: 6157.08

the person who commits those acts

Time: 6159.18

is socially isolated,

Time: 6160.38

as far as I know. - Yeah, yeah.

Time: 6161.46

- There might be some exceptions there.

Time: 6162.99

And sometimes this crosses over

Time: 6164.31

with other mental health issues,

Time: 6165.45

but sometimes no, no apparent mental health issues.

Time: 6168.317

So social isolation clearly drives

Time: 6171.24

powerful neurochemical and neuro biological changes,

Time: 6174.33

I really hope that Tachykinin-I and II,

Time: 6177.03

those are the main ones

Time: 6177.96

in humans? - Yeah, yeah.

Time: 6178.83

- Will be explored in more detail.

Time: 6181.41

Also, I didn't know that Tachykinin-I is Substance P.

Time: 6183.9

- Yes. - And Substance P

Time: 6184.818

is Tachykinin-I. - Yes.

Time: 6185.651

Tachykinin-I is the gene name,

Time: 6188.07

and Tachykinin-II, in humans, is called Neurokinin B,

Time: 6192.24

that's the name of the protein.

Time: 6194.16

I just refer to it by the gene name

Time: 6196.11

'cause it makes it easier

Time: 6197.28

and I don't have to keep remembering

Time: 6198.78

two names for each thing.

Time: 6200.31

- And if I'm not mistaken,

Time: 6203.16

you put yourself in the company of geneticists

Time: 6205.56

because your original training was in genetics,

Time: 6208.17

immunology and areas

Time: 6210

related to that. - It was in cell biology,

Time: 6211.53

and I didn't actually have formal training in genetics

Time: 6214.68

as a graduate student,

Time: 6215.88

but I think I'm a geneticist at heart,

Time: 6218.13

that's just the way I like to think about things.

Time: 6221.25

And when I started working on flies, that sort of,

Time: 6224.82

I came out of the closet as a geneticist,

Time: 6226.92

as it were. [laughing]

Time: 6228.18

- Wonderful, as long as we're talking about humans,

Time: 6230.85

I'd love to get your thoughts

Time: 6231.75

about human studies of emotion.

Time: 6233.16

I know you wrote this book with Ralph Adolphs,

Time: 6235.14

you have this new book, which we'll provide a link to,

Time: 6237.33

which I've read front to back twice, it's phenomenal.

Time: 6241.085

- Thank you. - I've mentioned it before

Time: 6241.918

on the podcast, it's really,

Time: 6243.27

there are books that are worth reading,

Time: 6245.1

and then there are books that are important,

Time: 6246.6

and I think this book is truly important

Time: 6248.19

for the general population to read and understand,

Time: 6250.77

and neuroscientists should read and understand the contents,

Time: 6253.2

because we, as a culture,

Time: 6257.01

are way off in terms of how we think about emotions

Time: 6259.47

and states and behaviors.

Time: 6261.3

So we'll put a link to that,

Time: 6262.44

it's really worth the time and energy to read it,

Time: 6265.83

and it's written beautifully,

Time: 6267

I should say. - Thank you.

Time: 6267.833

- Very accessible even for non-scientists.

Time: 6270.24

There's a heat map diagram in that book that I think about,

Time: 6275.55

this is a heat map diagram of subjective reports

Time: 6278.61

that people gave of where they experience an emotion,

Time: 6283.71

or a feeling, a somatic feeling,

Time: 6285.66

in their body, or in their head, or both,

Time: 6288.48

when they are angry, sad, calm, lonely,

Time: 6292.35

et cetera, et cetera.

Time: 6293.79

And I wouldn't want people to think that those heat maps

Time: 6296.46

were generated by any physiological measurement,

Time: 6300.87

because they were not.

Time: 6301.83

And yet, I don't think we can have a discussion

Time: 6304.89

about emotions and states

Time: 6306.84

and the sorts of behaviors that we're talking about today

Time: 6309.15

without thinking about the body also.

Time: 6311.01

- [David] Yep.

Time: 6311.843

- And I'm not coming to this

Time: 6312.84

as a Northern California, mind-body.

Time: 6314.99

- Yeah. - I've been to Esalen once.

Time: 6316.344

[David laughing] I didn't go in the baths,

Time: 6317.4

I went there, I gave a talk and I left.

Time: 6318.96

It is very beautiful.

Time: 6320.49

If anyone wants to know what it looks like,

Time: 6321.6

I think that final scene of "Mad Men" is shot at Esalen,

Time: 6325.65

it's a very beautiful place.

Time: 6326.61

And yet, mind-body, to me, is a neurobiological construct.

Time: 6331.8

- Yes. - Because the nervous system

Time: 6332.7

extends through out of the cranial vault

Time: 6335.1

and into the spinal cord. - Yeah.

Time: 6336

- And body and back and forth, okay.

Time: 6338.49

How should we think about the body, in terms of states?

Time: 6343.47

And at some point, I'd love for you to comment

Time: 6345.78

on that heat map experiment,

Time: 6347.58

because it does seem that there's some regularity

Time: 6350.28

as to where people experience emotions.

Time: 6352.86

When people are in a rage, for instance,

Time: 6354.57

they seem to feel it both in their gut and in their head,

Time: 6358.08

it seems, on average.

Time: 6360.57

And people love to extrapolate to gut intuition

Time: 6365.28

or that the chakras or anger is in the stomach,

Time: 6368.73

and this goes to Eastern medicine, et cetera.

Time: 6371.76

How should we think about mind-body

Time: 6373.92

in the context of states,

Time: 6375

and think about it as scientists,

Time: 6376.77

maybe even as neuroscientists or geneticists?

Time: 6380.04

- Good, so for the answer to the first question

Time: 6383.67

about the heat maps

Time: 6385.11

and people associating certain parts of their body

Time: 6388.44

with certain emotional feelings,

Time: 6390.84

this goes back to something

Time: 6393.15

called the somatic marker hypothesis,

Time: 6395.64

that was proposed by Antonio Damasio,

Time: 6398.28

who is a neurologist at USC,

Time: 6401.73

the idea that our subjective feeling

Time: 6405.45

of a particular emotion is,

Time: 6408.09

in part, associated with a sensation

Time: 6413.34

of something happening in a particular part of our body,

Time: 6417.45

the gut, the heart,

Time: 6420

I don't see the liver

Time: 6421.23

invoked very much in emotional characterization,

Time: 6426.66

but. - But gall

Time: 6427.493

and the gallbladder. - Yes.

Time: 6429.33

- Somebody having a lot of gall.

Time: 6430.369

- That's right. - I don't know why I make

Time: 6431.202

a fist when I say that.

Time: 6432.035

- Right. - But I'm guessing

Time: 6432.868

the gall bladder is shaped like a fist.

Time: 6434.233

[Andrew laughing] - That's right,

Time: 6435.6

and if there is a physiology underlying these heat maps,

Time: 6440.25

it could reflect increased blood flow

Time: 6442.8

to these different structures.

Time: 6444.3

And that, in turn, reflects what you were talking about,

Time: 6448.05

that is, emotion definitely involves

Time: 6450.84

communication between the brain and the body,

Time: 6453.72

and it's bidirectional communication,

Time: 6456.84

and it's mediated by the peripheral nervous system,

Time: 6461.25

the sympathetic and the parasympathetic nervous system,

Time: 6464.49

which control heart rate, for example,

Time: 6467.43

blood vessel, blood pressure.

Time: 6470.19

And those neurons receive input from the hypothalamus

Time: 6474.69

and other brain regions,

Time: 6476.88

central brain regions that control their activity.

Time: 6480.33

And when the brain is put in a particular state,

Time: 6484.98

it activates sympathetic and parasympathetic neurons,

Time: 6488.97

which have effects on the heart and on blood pressure,

Time: 6493.74

and these, in turn, feed back onto the brain

Time: 6498.12

through the sensory system.

Time: 6500.01

And a large part of this bidirectional communication

Time: 6503.82

is also mediated through the vagus nerve,

Time: 6507.18

which many of your listeners and viewers

Time: 6510.81

may have heard about

Time: 6511.68

because it's become a topic of intense activity now.

Time: 6515.67

People have known for a long time,

Time: 6518.16

so the vagus nerve is a bundle of nerve fibers

Time: 6523.53

that comes out basically of your skull,

Time: 6527.25

out of the central nervous system,

Time: 6529.44

and then sends fibers into your heart, your gut,

Time: 6535.56

all sorts of visceral organs.

Time: 6539.13

And that information is both,

Time: 6541.44

you used the words earlier in our discussion,

Time: 6544.71

afferent and efferent.

Time: 6546.93

So the vagal fibers sense things

Time: 6551.7

that are happening in the body,

Time: 6553.83

so the reason you feel your stomach

Time: 6556.89

tied up in knots if you're tense

Time: 6559.53

is that those vagal fibers

Time: 6561.6

are sensing the contraction of the gut muscles,

Time: 6565.86

and they're also afferents,

Time: 6567.72

which means that information coming out of the brain

Time: 6571.11

can influence those peripheral organs as well.

Time: 6574.74

And there's work from a number of labs,

Time: 6577.08

just in the last six months or so,

Time: 6580.35

where people are starting to decode the components

Time: 6584.82

of the different fibers in the vagus nerve.

Time: 6588.24

And it's amazing how much specificity is,

Time: 6591.21

there are specific vagal nerves that go to the lung,

Time: 6595.32

that control breathing responses,

Time: 6597.57

that go to the gut, that go to other organs.

Time: 6601.59

It's almost like a set of color-coded lines,

Time: 6606.21

labeled lines for those things.

Time: 6608.07

And now how those vagal afferents

Time: 6611.97

play a role in the playing out of emotion states

Time: 6616.56

is a fascinating question

Time: 6618.27

that people are just beginning to scrape the surface of.

Time: 6621.66

But I think what's exciting now

Time: 6623.76

is that people are going to be developing tools

Time: 6625.95

that will allow us to turn on or turn off

Time: 6628.89

specific subsets of fibers within the vagus nerve

Time: 6633.3

and ask how that affects particular emotional behaviors.

Time: 6637.02

So you're absolutely right,

Time: 6638.64

this brain-body connection is critical,

Time: 6641.31

not just for the gut, but for the heart,

Time: 6643.98

for the lungs, for all kinds of other parts of your body,

Time: 6648.03

and Darwin recognized that as well.

Time: 6650.52

And I think it's a central feature of emotion state,

Time: 6654.66

and I think, what underlies

Time: 6656.34

our subjective feelings of an emotion.

Time: 6659.82

- Incredible, well, David, I have to say,

Time: 6662.52

as a true fan of the work

Time: 6664.29

that your lab has been doing over so many decades,

Time: 6667.05

and first of all, I was delighted

Time: 6669.12

when you stopped working on stem cells.

Time: 6670.712

[David laughing] Not because you weren't doing

Time: 6671.55

incredible work there,

Time: 6672.54

but because I saw a talk

Time: 6674.55

where you showed a movie of an octopus

Time: 6678.42

spitting out, or not spitting,

Time: 6679.86

but squirting out a bunch of ink and escaping,

Time: 6682.32

and you said you were going to work

Time: 6683.25

on things of the sort that we're talking about today,

Time: 6685.56

fear, aggression, mating behaviors, social behaviors.

Time: 6689.07

It's been incredible to see the work that your lab has done,

Time: 6691.41

and I know I speak on behalf

Time: 6695.632

of a tremendous number of people

Time: 6697.08

when I say thank you for taking time

Time: 6699.24

out of your important schedule

Time: 6700.74

to share with us what you've learned.

Time: 6702.69

My last question is a simple one,

Time: 6705.48

which is, will you come back

Time: 6707.16

and talk to us again in the future

Time: 6708.87

about the additional work that's sure to come?

Time: 6710.82

- I would be happy to do that,

Time: 6712.29

and I really have appreciated your questions,

Time: 6715.71

they've all been right on the money,

Time: 6717.45

you've hit all of the critical, important issues

Time: 6720.81

in this field.

Time: 6721.86

And you've uncovered what is known,

Time: 6725.61

the little bit is known,

Time: 6726.81

and how much is not known,

Time: 6728.82

and I think it's important to emphasize the unknown things,

Time: 6733.11

because that's what the next generation of neuroscientists

Time: 6736.74

has to solve.

Time: 6738.09

And so I hope this will help

Time: 6739.32

to attract young people into this field,

Time: 6742.47

because it's so important,

Time: 6744.27

particularly for our understanding of mental illness

Time: 6747.6

and mental health and psychiatry,

Time: 6751.65

we've got to figure out how emotion systems

Time: 6755.04

are controlled in a causal way

Time: 6757.92

if we ever want to improve

Time: 6759.87

on the psychiatric treatments that we have now,

Time: 6762.54

and that's going to require

Time: 6763.89

the next generation of people coming into the field.

Time: 6766.71

- Absolutely, I second that.

Time: 6768.81

Well, thank you, it's been a delight.

Time: 6770.34

- Thank you, great, really appreciate it.

Time: 6773.46

- Thank you for joining me today

Time: 6774.6

for my discussion with Dr. David Anderson.

Time: 6776.64

Please also be sure to check out his new book,

Time: 6778.807

"The Nature of the Beast: How Emotions Guide Us".

Time: 6781.83

It's a truly masterful exploration

Time: 6783.84

of the biology and psychology

Time: 6785.61

behind what we call emotions

Time: 6787.14

and states of mind and body.

Time: 6789.03

If you're learning from and/or enjoying this podcast,

Time: 6791.43

please subscribe to our YouTube channel,

Time: 6793.41

that's a simple, zero-cost way to support us.

Time: 6795.84

Please also subscribe to the podcast on Spotify and Apple,

Time: 6799.08

and on both Spotify and Apple,

Time: 6800.97

you have the opportunity to leave us

Time: 6802.41

up to a five-star review.

Time: 6804.24

If you have questions, or comments,

Time: 6805.59

or suggestions about topics you'd like us to cover,

Time: 6807.78

or guests you'd like us to interview

Time: 6809.01

on the Huberman Lab Podcast,

Time: 6810.66

please put those in the comment section on YouTube.

Time: 6813.36

We do read all those comments

Time: 6814.77

and we do take them to heart.

Time: 6816.27

Please also check out the sponsors

Time: 6817.59

mentioned at the beginning of today's podcast,

Time: 6819.78

and check out Momentous supplements,

Time: 6821.64

our new partners in the supplement space,

Time: 6823.29

and check out Athletic Greens,

Time: 6825.33

that's the best way to support this podcast.

Time: 6827.61

If you're not already following us on social media,

Time: 6829.86

please do so.

Time: 6831.18

We are hubermanlab on Twitter,

Time: 6832.92

and we are also hubermanlab on Instagram.

Time: 6834.96

And both places, I cover science and science-related tools,

Time: 6838.29

some of which overlap with the content

Time: 6839.88

of the Huberman Lab Podcast,

Time: 6841.05

but much of which is unique from the content

Time: 6843.27

covered on the Huberman Lab Podcast.

Time: 6844.8

Again, that's hubermanlab on Instagram

Time: 6846.177

and hubermanlab on Twitter.

Time: 6848.16

Please also check out our Neural Network monthly newsletter.

Time: 6851.04

This is a newsletter that has summaries of podcast episodes,

Time: 6854.55

it also includes a lot of actionable protocols.

Time: 6856.83

It's very easy to sign up for the newsletter,

Time: 6858.87

you go to hubermanlab.com,

Time: 6860.43

click on the menu, go to Newsletter.

Time: 6862.14

You supply your email,

Time: 6863.13

but we do not share your email with anybody,

Time: 6865.11

we have a very clear and rigorous privacy policy,

Time: 6868.02

which is we do not share your email with anybody.

Time: 6870.24

And the newsletter comes out once a month,

Time: 6872.73

and it is completely zero cost.

Time: 6874.29

Again, just go to hubermanlab.com

Time: 6876.297

and go to the Neural Network Newsletter.

Time: 6878.76

I'd also like to point out

Time: 6879.78

that the Huberman Lab Podcast has a clips channel.

Time: 6882.24

So these are brief clips,

Time: 6884.58

anywhere from three to 10 minutes,

Time: 6886.62

that encompass single concepts and actionable protocols,

Time: 6890.19

related to sleep, to focus,

Time: 6892.11

interviews with various guests,

Time: 6893.37

we talk about things like caffeine,

Time: 6894.78

when to drink caffeine relative to sleep,

Time: 6897.21

alcohol, when and how and if anyone should ingest it

Time: 6899.85

relative to sleep,

Time: 6901.59

dopamine, serotonin, mental health, physical health,

Time: 6904.2

and on and on,

Time: 6905.1

all the things that relate to the topics

Time: 6907.05

most of interest to you.

Time: 6908.4

You can find that easily by going to YouTube,

Time: 6911.07

look for, "Huberman Lab Clips," in the search area,

Time: 6913.547

and it will take you there, subscribe.

Time: 6915.54

And we are constantly updating those with new clips.

Time: 6917.88

This is especially useful, I believe,

Time: 6919.44

for people that have missed some of the earlier episodes

Time: 6921.45

or you're still working through

Time: 6922.35

the back catalog of Huberman Lab podcasts,

Time: 6924.15

which admittedly can be rather long.

Time: 6926.28

And last but certainly not least,

Time: 6928.56

thank you for your interest in science.

Copyright © 2024. All rights reserved.