Dr. Charles Zuker: The Biology of Taste Perception & Sugar Craving | Huberman Lab Podcast #81

Time: 0

[light rock music]

Time: 0.833

- Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,

Time: 2.28

where we discuss science

Time: 3.477

and science based tools for everyday life.

Time: 9.3

I'm Andrew Huberman,

Time: 10.35

and I'm a professor of neurobiology

Time: 11.94

and ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine.

Time: 15.03

Today my guest is Dr. Charles Zuker.

Time: 17.61

Dr. Zuker is a professor of biochemistry

Time: 19.77

and molecular biophysics and of neuroscience

Time: 22.38

at Columbia University School of Medicine.

Time: 25.2

Dr. Zuker is one of the world's

Time: 26.58

leading experts in perception.

Time: 28.92

That is how the nervous system converts physical stimuli

Time: 31.95

in the world into events within

Time: 34.29

the nervous system that we come

Time: 35.76

to understand as our sense of smell,

Time: 38.4

our sense of taste,

Time: 39.51

our sense of vision, our sense of touch,

Time: 41.64

and our sense of hearing.

Time: 43.11

Dr. Zuker's lab

Time: 44.04

is responsible for a tremendous amount

Time: 45.81

of pioneering and groundbreaking work

Time: 48

in the area of perception,

Time: 50.1

for a long time his laboratory worked on vision,

Time: 52.17

defining the very receptors that allow for the conversion

Time: 55.32

of light into signals that the rest

Time: 58.183

of the eye and the brain can understand.

Time: 59.67

In recent years,

Time: 61.171

his laboratory is focused mainly on the perception of taste.

Time: 63.57

And indeed his laboratory is responsible for discovering

Time: 66.39

many of the taste receptors leading

Time: 68.49

to our perception of things like sweetness,

Time: 71.07

sourness, bitterness, saltiness, and umami.

Time: 74.46

That is savoriness in food.

Time: 76.71

Dr. Zuker's laboratory is also responsible

Time: 78.69

for doing groundbreaking work on the sense of thirst.

Time: 81.66

That is how the nervous system determines whether

Time: 84.15

or not we should ingest more fluid

Time: 86.16

or reject fluids that are offered to us.

Time: 88.95

A key feature of the work from Dr. Zuker's laboratory

Time: 91.65

is that it bridges the brain and body.

Time: 94.17

As you'll soon learn from today's discussion.

Time: 96.33

His laboratory has discovered a unique set of sugar sensing

Time: 100.14

neurons that exist not just within the brain,

Time: 102.69

but a separate set of neurons.

Time: 103.89

That sense sweetness and sugar within the body.

Time: 106.92

And that much of the communication between the brain

Time: 109.17

and body leading to our seeking of sugar

Time: 111.84

is below our conscious detection.

Time: 113.88

Dr. Zuker has received a large number of prestigious awards

Time: 116.58

and appointments as a consequence

Time: 117.87

of his discoveries in neuroscience.

Time: 119.97

He is a member of the National Academy of Sciences,

Time: 122.25

the National Academy of Medicine,

Time: 123.63

and the American Association for the Advancement of Science.

Time: 126.33

He is also an investigator

Time: 127.65

with the Howard Hughes Medical Institute.

Time: 129.87

For those of you that are not familiar

Time: 131.34

with the so-called HHMI,

Time: 132.84

the Howard Hughes Medical Institute,

Time: 134.43

Howard Hughes Medical Institute

Time: 135.48

investigators are selected

Time: 137.07

on an extremely competitive basis.

Time: 139.41

And indeed they have to come back every five years

Time: 142.11

and prove themselves worthy

Time: 143.97

of being reappointed as Howard Hughes investigators,

Time: 147.39

Dr. Zuker has been a Howard Hughes investigator since 1989.

Time: 152.01

What all that means for you as a viewer

Time: 153.54

and or listener of today's podcast,

Time: 155.61

is that you are about to learn about the nervous system

Time: 158.34

and its ability to create perceptions in particular,

Time: 160.95

the perception of taste and sugar sensing

Time: 163.32

from the world's expert on perception and taste.

Time: 167.04

I'm certain that by the end of today's podcast,

Time: 169.59

you're not just going to come away with a deeper

Time: 171.15

understanding of our perceptions

Time: 172.71

and our perception of taste in particular.

Time: 174.9

But indeed you will come away with an understanding

Time: 177.33

of how we create internal representations

Time: 180.06

of the entire world around us and in doing so,

Time: 183.51

how we come to understand our life experience.

Time: 186.24

I'm pleased to announce that the Huberman Lab Podcast

Time: 188.1

is now partnered with Momentous Supplements.

Time: 190.41

We often talk about supplements on the Huberman Lab Podcast

Time: 192.9

and while supplements aren't necessary for everybody,

Time: 195.12

many people derive tremendous benefit from them.

Time: 197.25

For things like enhancing the quality and speed

Time: 200.04

with which you get into sleep

Time: 201.6

or for enhancing focus or for hormone support.

Time: 204.6

The reason we partnered

Time: 205.44

with Momentous Supplements is several fold.

Time: 207.21

First of all, their supplements are

Time: 208.13

of the absolute highest quality.

Time: 210

Second of all, they ship internationally,

Time: 212.25

which is important because many

Time: 213.69

of our podcast listeners reside outside the US.

Time: 216.36

Third, many of the supplements

Time: 218.04

that Momentous makes and most all

Time: 219.95

of the supplements that we partnered

Time: 221.37

with them directly on are single ingredient formulations.

Time: 224.7

This is important for a number of reasons.

Time: 226.2

First of all, if you're going to create

Time: 227.52

a supplement protocol that's customized for your needs,

Time: 230.31

you want to be able to figure out which supplement ingredients

Time: 232.68

are most essential and only use those and supplements

Time: 235.5

that combine lots of ingredients simply

Time: 236.94

won't allow you to do that.

Time: 238.5

So in trying to put together

Time: 239.82

a supplement protocol for yourself,

Time: 241.56

that's the most biologically effective

Time: 243.36

and cost effective single ingredient formulations

Time: 246.03

are going to be the most useful.

Time: 247.41

If you'd like to see the supplements that we partner

Time: 249.12

with Momentous on, you can go

Time: 250.68

to livemomentous.com/huberman,

Time: 253.846

and there you'll see many of the supplements

Time: 255.42

that we've talked repeatedly about

Time: 256.86

on the Huberman Lab Podcast episodes.

Time: 259.08

I should mention that the catalog of supplements

Time: 261.57

that are available at livemomentous.com/Huberman

Time: 264.18

is constantly being expanded.

Time: 265.98

So you can check back there,

Time: 267.24

livemomentous.com/huberman to see

Time: 269.91

what's currently available.

Time: 271.08

And from time to time,

Time: 272.28

you'll notice new supplements being added to the inventory.

Time: 274.86

Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize

Time: 276.54

that this podcast is separate

Time: 277.86

from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.

Time: 280.14

It is however, part of my desire

Time: 281.73

and effort to bring zero cost to consumer

Time: 283.53

information about science and science related tools

Time: 285.93

to the general public in keeping with that theme,

Time: 288.3

I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.

Time: 291.03

Our first sponsor is Thesis.

Time: 293.01

Thesis makes custom nootropics

Time: 295.26

and for frequent listeners of this podcast,

Time: 297.72

you may remember that I'm not a big fan of the word

Time: 299.79

nootropics because the word nootropics

Time: 302.13

means smart drugs or smart compound.

Time: 305.1

And the reason I don't like that phrase is that the brain

Time: 307.77

has many different circuits that it uses in order

Time: 310.11

to perform things like focus

Time: 312.36

or task switching or creativity.

Time: 315

So the idea that there's a single thing that we would call

Time: 317.64

a smart drug is simply not in concert with the biology.

Time: 322.02

Well, Thesis understands this,

Time: 323.55

and as a consequence has developed custom nootropic

Time: 326.37

formulations that are tailored to your unique needs.

Time: 329.52

So for instance,

Time: 330.63

Thesis will allow you to try different blends over

Time: 332.94

the course of a month and determine

Time: 334.53

which blends of specific ingredients work best

Time: 336.93

for you to focus or for you to gain motivation

Time: 340.05

and energy for workouts or

Time: 341.7

for cognitive work of some sort.

Time: 343.29

I've been using Thesis custom nootropics

Time: 344.91

for well over six months now.

Time: 346.71

And they completely transform the way that I do cognitive

Time: 349.26

work and indeed the way that I do physical fitness,

Time: 352.68

if you want to try your

Time: 353.64

own personalized nootropic starter kit,

Time: 355.26

you can go online to takethesis.com/huberman.

Time: 358.56

There you'll just do a brief

Time: 359.73

three minute quiz and Thesis will

Time: 361.44

send you four different formulas to try in your first month.

Time: 364.26

That's takethesis.com/huberman

Time: 366.39

and use the code Huberman at checkout

Time: 367.781

to get 10% off your first box.

Time: 370.17

Today's episode is also brought to us by ROKA.

Time: 372.303

ROKA makes eyeglasses and sunglasses

Time: 374.148

that are the absolute highest quality.

Time: 376.44

The company was founded by two

Time: 377.46

all American swimmers from Stanford

Time: 379.02

and everything about ROKA eyeglasses and sunglasses

Time: 381.54

were designed with performance

Time: 383.16

in mind and with the biology of the visual system in mind,

Time: 386.25

I spent my lifetime working on the visual system,

Time: 387.99

and I can tell you that your visual system has to contend

Time: 390.18

with a number of very important challenges in order

Time: 392.58

to be able to see the world around you clearly,

Time: 394.65

things like adjusting for background illumination so that

Time: 396.96

when you go from a sunny area to a shady area,

Time: 398.88

you can see what's in front

Time: 399.84

of you still with crystal clarity.

Time: 401.76

ROKA takes all of that into account

Time: 403.47

when designing their eyeglasses and sunglasses,

Time: 405.9

their eyeglasses and sunglasses

Time: 407.1

also have some unique qualities for instance,

Time: 409.44

because they were initially designed

Time: 411

for performance with things like running and cycling.

Time: 413.73

If you get sweaty, they won't fall off your face.

Time: 415.77

Also they're extremely lightweight.

Time: 417.42

In fact, most of the time,

Time: 418.253

I don't even remember that I'm wearing them.

Time: 420.09

I wear ROKA eyeglasses at night.

Time: 422.16

So I wear readers and I wear sunglasses often

Time: 424.5

during the day, if it's very bright or if I'm driving,

Time: 426.96

if you'd like to try ROKA eyeglasses and sunglasses,

Time: 429.12

you can go to ROKA that's roka.com and enter

Time: 432.03

the code Huberman to save 20% off your first order.

Time: 435.18

Today's episode is also brought to us by Helix Sleep.

Time: 437.67

Helix Sleep makes mattresses and pillows

Time: 439.41

that are uniquely tailored to your sleep needs.

Time: 442.08

I've talked repeatedly about the fact that sleep

Time: 444.39

is the foundation of mental health,

Time: 445.77

physical health, and performance.

Time: 447.42

So getting adequate deep sleep is absolutely essential.

Time: 450.63

Now, one of the key things to getting

Time: 451.77

a great night's sleep is to make sure

Time: 452.94

that the surface that you're sleeping on

Time: 454.65

is the right one for you.

Time: 456.03

Helix understands this,

Time: 457.29

and they've created a sleep quiz.

Time: 459.39

That is you go to their website and you fill out

Time: 461.88

a brief quiz that asks questions

Time: 463.56

like you sleep on your side, your back, your stomach.

Time: 465.63

Do you tend to run hotter cold during the night?

Time: 467.16

Maybe you don't know the answers.

Time: 468.15

In which case you simply say you don't know,

Time: 470.4

by taking that quiz,

Time: 471.36

they will match you to a mattress

Time: 473.31

that is ideal for your sleep needs.

Time: 475.62

I took that quiz and I matched to the so-called Dusk,

Time: 477.9

D-U-S-K mattress.

Time: 479.19

And I've been sleeping on that mattress for some time now,

Time: 481.26

and it's the best sleep that I've ever had.

Time: 483

So if you're interested in upgrading your mattress to one,

Time: 484.92

that's uniquely tailored to your sleep needs,

Time: 487.02

you can go to helixsleep.com/huberman,

Time: 488.956

take their two minute sleep quiz,

Time: 490.68

and they'll match you to a customized mattress.

Time: 492.39

And you'll get up to $200 off any mattress order

Time: 495.3

and two free pillows.

Time: 496.68

They have a 10 year warranty and you get to try out

Time: 498.63

that mattress for a hundred nights, risk free.

Time: 500.76

They'll even pick it up for you if you don't love it,

Time: 502.59

but I do believe

Time: 503.423

that you will love it because you'll be

Time: 504.57

sleeping far better than you have before.

Time: 506.52

Again, if you're interested,

Time: 507.72

you can go to helixsleep.com/huberman to get up to $200 off

Time: 511.8

and two free pillows.

Time: 513

And now for my discussion with Dr. Charles Zuker,

Time: 516.18

Charles, thank you so much for joining me today.

Time: 518.607

- My pleasure.

Time: 520.11

- I want to ask you about many things related

Time: 522.45

to taste and gustatory perception,

Time: 525.72

but maybe to start off and because you've worked

Time: 528.27

on a number of different topics in neuroscience,

Time: 530.28

not just taste,

Time: 532.56

how do you think about perception,

Time: 534.81

or rather, I should say,

Time: 535.95

how should the world and people think about perception,

Time: 540.3

how it's different from sensation and what leads

Time: 544.32

to our experience of life in terms of vision,

Time: 547.56

hearing, taste, et cetera?

Time: 549.48

- So the brain is an extra ordinary organ

Time: 557.708

that weights maybe 2% of your body mass.

Time: 561.567

Yet it consumes anywhere between 25

Time: 565.387

to 30% of all of your energy and oxygen.

Time: 572.822

And it gets transformed into a mind

Time: 577.17

and this mind changes the human condition.

Time: 580.765

It make, it changes, it transforms fear into courage,

Time: 589.02

conformity into creativity, sadness into happiness.

Time: 594.81

How the hell does that happen?

Time: 599.04

Now, the challenge that the brain faces

Time: 602.64

is that the world is made of real things.

Time: 605.94

You know, this here is a glass,

Time: 609

and this is a cord and this is a microphone,

Time: 612.12

but the brain is only made of neurons

Time: 614.88

that only understand electrical signals.

Time: 618.63

So how do you transform that reality into nothing

Time: 624.45

that electrical signals that now need

Time: 627.45

to represent the world?

Time: 632.286

And that process

Time: 634.32

is what we can operationally define as perception,

Time: 639.42

in the senses, let's say olfactory,

Time: 643.38

other taste, vision we can very straightforwardly

Time: 649.293

separate detection from perception,

Time: 652.41

detection is what happens when you take a sugar molecule,

Time: 655.53

you put it in your tongue and then a set of specific cells

Time: 660.36

now sense that sugar molecule that's detection,

Time: 664.95

you haven't perceived anything yet.

Time: 666.99

That is just your cells in your tongue,

Time: 670.271

interacting with this chemical.

Time: 671.88

But now that cell gets activated and sends a signal

Time: 674.81

to the brain and now detection

Time: 677.25

gets transformed into perception.

Time: 681.6

And he's trying to understand how that happens.

Time: 685.08

That's been the maniacal drive,

Time: 691.89

of my entire career in neuroscience.

Time: 694.861

How does the brain ultimately transform detection

Time: 698.7

into perception so that it can guide actions and behaviors?

Time: 704.31

Does that make sense?

Time: 705.51

- Absolutely.

Time: 706.56

And is a very clear and beautiful description,

Time: 710.31

a sort of high level question related to that.

Time: 712.5

And then I think we can get

Time: 713.88

into some of the intermediate steps.

Time: 717.081

I think many people would like to know whether

Time: 720.206

or not my perception of the color

Time: 721.68

of your shirt is the same as your perception

Time: 724.62

of the color of your shirt.

Time: 726.472

- Excellent question.

Time: 727.675

Am I okay to interrupt you

Time: 729.472

as I'm guessing what you're going?

Time: 731.988

- Interruption is welcome on this podcast.

Time: 734.04

The audience will always penalize me

Time: 736.26

for interrupting you,

Time: 738.078

and will never penalize you for interrupting me.

Time: 739.47

- Good, I like the one way penalize.

Time: 744.302

Now, given what I told you before

Time: 748.96

that the brain is trying to represent

Time: 750.84

the world based in nothing

Time: 753.425

but the transformation of these signals into electrical

Time: 759.42

languages that now neurons had to encode and decode.

Time: 763.8

It follows that your brain is different than my brain.

Time: 768

And therefore it follows that

Time: 769.89

the way that you are perceiving the world must be different

Time: 773.43

than mine, even when receiving the same sensory cues.

Time: 778.17

And I'll tell you about an experiment

Time: 779.523

is a simple experiment yet brilliant.

Time: 782.73

That demonstrates why we perceive the world,

Time: 786.18

how we perceive the world different.

Time: 790.461

So in the world of vision, as you know,

Time: 794.85

well now we have three classes of photo receptor neurons

Time: 799.35

that sense three basic colors, red, blue, and green.

Time: 807.42

Blue, green, and red

Time: 809.346

if we go from short to long wavelength

Time: 812.273

and these three are sufficient to accommodate

Time: 814.68

the full visible spectra,

Time: 817.53

I'm going to take three light projectors,

Time: 821.432

and I'm going to project with one into a wide screen,

Time: 824.55

a red light, and the other one green light,

Time: 827.97

I'm going to overlap the two beams.

Time: 830.01

And on the screen, there's be yellow.

Time: 833.22

This is the superposition

Time: 834.84

when you have two beams of red and green,

Time: 838.98

and then I'm going to take a third projector

Time: 841.71

and I'm going to put a filter

Time: 843.21

that projects right next to that mixed beam,

Time: 846.96

a spectral pure yellow.

Time: 851.28

And I'm going to ask you to come to the red and green

Time: 856.451

projectors and play with intensity knobs

Time: 860.28

so that you can match that yellow

Time: 862.62

that you're projecting to the spectrally pure next to it.

Time: 867.21

Is this making sense?

Time: 868.35

- Perfect sense.

Time: 869.891

- And I'm going to write down the numbers

Time: 871.32

in those two volume

Time: 873.27

intensity knobs and then I'm going to ask

Time: 878.481

the next person to do the same.

Time: 879.84

And then I'm going to ask every person around

Time: 882.48

this area of Battery Park in New York to do the same,

Time: 886.89

and guess what, we're going to end up

Time: 889.86

with thousands of different number combinations.

Time: 893.19

- Amazing.

Time: 894.3

- So for all of us,

Time: 895.59

is yellow enough that we can use a common language,

Time: 901.08

but for every one of us,

Time: 902.82

that yellow is going to be ever so slightly differently.

Time: 908.25

And so I think that simple psychological experiment

Time: 911.46

beautifully illustrates

Time: 912.69

how we truly perceive the world differently.

Time: 916.92

- I love that example.

Time: 918.06

And yet, in that example,

Time: 919.95

we know the basic elements from which color is created.

Time: 924.03

If we migrate into a slightly different sense,

Time: 927.18

let me pick a hard one, like-

Time: 930.06

- Sound. - Sound, olfaction.

Time: 932.593

- Very hard then to do an experiment

Time: 936.001

that will allow us to get that degree

Time: 939.45

of granularity and beautiful causality,

Time: 944.16

where we can show that A produces and leads to B.

Time: 949.62

If I give you the smell of a rose,

Time: 951.27

you can describe it to me.

Time: 954.3

If I smell the same rose, I can describe it also,

Time: 958.08

but I have no way whether

Time: 959.97

the two of us are experiencing the same,

Time: 963.09

but it's close enough that we can both pretty much say

Time: 968.46

that it has the following enough features

Time: 972.653

or other determinants,

Time: 974.79

but no question that your experience is different than mine.

Time: 981.806

- The fact that it's good enough for us to both survive

Time: 985.38

that your perception of yellow

Time: 986.61

and my perception of yellow at least up

Time: 988.56

until now is good enough for us both to survive.

Time: 990.96

- You got it.

Time: 992.1

- Raises a thought

Time: 993.3

about a statement made by a colleague

Time: 996.09

of ours, Marcus Meister at Caltech.

Time: 999.48

It's never been on this podcast,

Time: 1000.8

but in a review that I read by Marcus, at one point,

Time: 1005.51

he said that the basic function of perception

Time: 1008.36

is to divide our behavioral responses

Time: 1011

into the outcomes downstream

Time: 1015.41

of three basic emotional responses.

Time: 1018.95

Yum.

Time: 1020.06

I like it.

Time: 1021.41

Yuck.

Time: 1022.43

I hate it.

Time: 1023.45

Or meh, whatever.

Time: 1026.48

What do you think about,

Time: 1027.41

I'm not looking to establish a debate

Time: 1029.33

between you and Marcus, without Marcus here.

Time: 1031.07

- I understand that.

Time: 1032.798

- But what I like about that is that it seems like

Time: 1036.23

we know the brain is a very economical organ in some sense,

Time: 1040.25

despite its high metabolic demands and this variation

Time: 1045.05

in perception from one individual to the next, at once,

Time: 1048.38

seems like a problem because

Time: 1050.21

we're all literally seeing different things.

Time: 1052.82

And yet we function,

Time: 1054.86

we function well enough for most of us to avoid death

Time: 1058.7

and cliffs and eating poisons and so forth

Time: 1062.6

and to enjoy some aspects of life one hopes.

Time: 1065.63

So is there a general statement that we can about the brain,

Time: 1069.77

not just as a organ to generate perception,

Time: 1072.68

not just as an organ to keep us alive,

Time: 1074.96

but also an organ that is trying to batch

Time: 1078.77

our behaviors into general categories of outcome?

Time: 1081.62

- I think so, but, and again,

Time: 1083.75

I think the world of Marcus too,

Time: 1086.9

and I think he's right that broadly speaking,

Time: 1092.78

you could categorize a lot of behaviors falling

Time: 1096.321

into those two categories

Time: 1097.154

and that's 100% likely

Time: 1100.01

to be the case for animals in the wild where

Time: 1105.32

the choices are not necessarily binary,

Time: 1111.136

but they're very unique and distinct.

Time: 1114.635

Do I want to eat this?

Time: 1117.38

Do I want to kill that?

Time: 1120.05

Do I want to go there?

Time: 1121.22

Or do I want to go here?

Time: 1124.91

We humans deviated from that world long ago.

Time: 1131.54

And learned to experience life

Time: 1137.81

where we do things that we should not be doing.

Time: 1140.96

- Some of us more than others.

Time: 1142.34

- Exactly.

Time: 1144.07

You know, in my own world of taste,

Time: 1149.587

the likelihood that an animal

Time: 1151.61

in the wild will enjoy eating something bitter

Time: 1158.622

it's inconceivable, yet we love tonic water.

Time: 1165.71

We enjoy, we like living on the edge.

Time: 1169.52

We love enjoying experiences

Time: 1174.74

that makes us human.

Time: 1180.83

And that goes beyond that simple set of categories,

Time: 1187.307

which is yummy, yukky, ah, who cares.

Time: 1191.93

And so I think it's not a bad palette,

Time: 1194.21

but I think it's overly reductionist

Time: 1199.91

for certainly what we humans do.

Time: 1204.35

- I agree.

Time: 1205.956

And since we're here in New York,

Time: 1206.789

I can say that the many options,

Time: 1210.322

the extensive variety of food,

Time: 1212.45

flora and fauna in New York explains

Time: 1215.9

a lot of the more nuanced behaviors that we observe.

Time: 1219.92

Let's talk about taste because although

Time: 1223.55

you've done extensive work in the field of vision,

Time: 1225.83

and it's a topic that I love you could spend

Time: 1229.31

all day on taste is fascinating.

Time: 1232.46

First of all, I'd like to know

Time: 1233.6

why you migrated from studying vision to studying taste.

Time: 1238.19

And perhaps in that description,

Time: 1239.54

you could highlight to us why we should think about

Time: 1242.3

and how we should think about this sense of taste.

Time: 1246.35

- My goal has been to understand,

Time: 1248.27

as I highlighted before how the brain does its magic,

Time: 1252.2

you know what part you might wonder, ideally,

Time: 1255.2

I like to help contribute to understand all of it.

Time: 1260.24

You know how do you encode and decode emotions?

Time: 1264.62

How do you encode and decode memories and actions?

Time: 1268.85

How do you make decisions?

Time: 1270.89

How do you transform detection into perception?

Time: 1274.64

And the list goes on and on.

Time: 1277.954

But one of the key things in science, as you know,

Time: 1284.03

is ensuring that you always ask the right question

Time: 1287.33

so that you have a possibility of answering it.

Time: 1293

Because if the question cannot be tractable or reduced

Time: 1296.24

to an experimental path that helps you resolve it,

Time: 1300.47

then we end up doing some really fun science,

Time: 1303.62

but not necessarily answering

Time: 1305.57

the important problem that we want to study.

Time: 1309.77

Make sense?

Time: 1311.048

- From a first person perspective, yes.

Time: 1314.36

The hardest question,

Time: 1316.527

the most important question is what are you going

Time: 1319.13

to try and answer.

Time: 1319.963

- You got it.

Time: 1321.47

And so for example

Time: 1322.43

I will have to understand the neural basis of empathy.

Time: 1329.21

- [Andrew] There's a big market for that.

Time: 1330.89

- 100%, but I wouldn't even know.

Time: 1333.734

I mean, at the molecular level, that's what we do.

Time: 1336.58

How do the circuits in your brain create that sense?

Time: 1340.61

I have no clue how to do it.

Time: 1343.43

I can come up with ways to think about it,

Time: 1345.23

but I like to understand what

Time: 1347.96

in your brain makes someone a great philanthropist.

Time: 1353.12

What is the neural basis of love?

Time: 1357.11

I wouldn't even know where to begin.

Time: 1360.38

So if I want to begin to study

Time: 1362.09

these questions about brain function,

Time: 1364.97

that can cover so many aspects of the brain,

Time: 1368.81

I need to choose a problem that affords me that window.

Time: 1373.94

But in a way that I can ask questions

Time: 1375.74

that get me answers and among the senses

Time: 1380.21

that have the capacity

Time: 1384.016

of transforming detection into perception

Time: 1387.216

of being stored as memories or creating emotions,

Time: 1391.04

of giving you different actions and perceptions

Time: 1395.84

as a function of the internal state.

Time: 1398.36

You know, when you're hungry,

Time: 1399.23

things taste very differently that when you're sated,

Time: 1402.71

how, why?

Time: 1405.113

When you taste something,

Time: 1406.1

you now remember this amazing meal

Time: 1408.32

you had with your first date, how does that happen?

Time: 1413.18

So if I want to begin to explore all of these things

Time: 1416.72

that the brain does,

Time: 1419.12

I felt I have to choose a sensory system that affords

Time: 1425.135

some degree of simplicity in the way

Time: 1430.595

that the input output relationships are put together.

Time: 1434.99

And in a way that still can be used to ask every one

Time: 1438.05

of these problems that the brain

Time: 1440.03

has to ultimately compute, encode and decode.

Time: 1444.564

And what's remarkable

Time: 1447.23

about the taste system at the time

Time: 1448.763

that I began working on this

Time: 1452.54

is that nothing was known

Time: 1454.968

about the molecular basis of taste.

Time: 1458.69

You know, we knew that we could taste

Time: 1460.61

what has been usually defined as

Time: 1463.46

the five basic taste qualities, sweet, sour,

Time: 1467.99

bitter, salty, and umami,

Time: 1471.74

umami is a Japanese word

Time: 1473.3

that means yummy, delicious.

Time: 1477.037

And that's nearly every animal species,

Time: 1479.84

the taste of amino acids.

Time: 1482.18

And in humans, it's mostly associated with the taste of MSG,

Time: 1487.19

monosodium glutamate, one amino acid in particular.

Time: 1490.85

- What are just by way of example,

Time: 1492.65

some foods that are rich in the umami evoking stimulation?

Time: 1497.6

- Seaweed, tomatoes, cheese, eh, and it's a great,

Time: 1503.51

great flavor enhancer.

Time: 1506.48

It enriches our sensory experience.

Time: 1510.05

And so the beautiful thing of the system

Time: 1511.723

is that the lines of input are limited to five.

Time: 1517.97

You know, sweet, sour, bitter, salt and umami,

Time: 1520.157

and each of them has a predetermined meaning,

Time: 1524.63

you are born liking sugar and disliking bitter.

Time: 1529.49

You have no choice.

Time: 1531.53

These are hard wire systems,

Time: 1535.01

but of course you can learn to dislike sugar

Time: 1539.21

and to like bitters, but in the while,

Time: 1543.83

let's take humans out of the question,

Time: 1546.71

these are a hundred percent predetermined.

Time: 1550.55

You are born with that specific valance value for each taste

Time: 1557.45

of sweet, umami and low salt are attractive taste qualities.

Time: 1565.34

They evoke appetitive responses, I want to consume them.

Time: 1570.32

And bitter and sour are innately,

Time: 1574.88

predetermined to be aversive.

Time: 1578.09

- Could I interrupt you just briefly

Time: 1579.5

and ask a question about that exact point

Time: 1583.405

for something to be appetitive to and some

Time: 1587.591

other taste to be aversive and for those to be hardwired,

Time: 1590.57

can we assume that the sensation of very bitter

Time: 1595.34

or of activation of bitter receptors

Time: 1597.17

in the mouth activates a neural circuit

Time: 1600.77

that causes closing of the mouth,

Time: 1603.35

retraction of the tongue and retraction of the body

Time: 1606.5

and that the taste of something sweet

Time: 1608.24

might actually induce more licking?

Time: 1610.94

- 100%, you got it,

Time: 1613.31

the activation of the receptors in the tongue

Time: 1616.85

that recognize sweet versus the ones

Time: 1619.43

that recognize bitter activate

Time: 1622.04

an entire behavioral program.

Time: 1625.31

And that program that we can refer as appetitiveness

Time: 1630.35

or aversion it's composed of many different subroutines,

Time: 1636.5

in the case of bitter is very easy to actually look at,

Time: 1640.82

see them happening in animals.

Time: 1642.98

Because the first thing you do is you stop licking.

Time: 1645.71

Then you put unhappy face,

Time: 1648.95

then you squint your eyes and then you start gagging.

Time: 1653.84

And that entire thing happens by the activation

Time: 1658.411

of a bitter molecule

Time: 1659.779

in a bitter sensing cell in your tongue.

Time: 1661.16

- It's incredible. - It's again,

Time: 1663.198

the magic of the brain,

Time: 1665.9

how it it's able to encode and decode these extraordinary

Time: 1670.04

actions and behaviors in response of nothing but a simple,

Time: 1673.52

very unique sensory stimuli.

Time: 1679.04

Now, let me say that this pallet of five basic tastes

Time: 1682.58

accommodates all the dietary needs of the organism.

Time: 1686.03

Sweet to ensure that we get

Time: 1687.167

the right amount of energy,

Time: 1692.12

umami, to ensure that we get proteins

Time: 1694.82

and that essential nutrient, salt,

Time: 1698.42

the three appetitive ones to ensure

Time: 1699.77

that we maintain our electrolyte balance,

Time: 1703.85

bitter to prevent the ingestion

Time: 1707.01

of toxic nauseous chemicals, nearly all bitter tasting

Time: 1710.175

things out in the wild are bad for you,

Time: 1714.26

and sour, most likely to prevent ingestion

Time: 1717.11

of spoil acid,

Time: 1721.28

fermented foods.

Time: 1723.92

And that's it.

Time: 1724.753

That is the pallet that we deal with.

Time: 1728.27

Now, of course,

Time: 1729.103

there's a difference between basic taste and flavor,

Time: 1733.7

flavor is the whole experience.

Time: 1735.86

Flavor is the combination of multiple tastes coming together

Time: 1740.42

together with smell, with texture, with temperature,

Time: 1745.82

with the look of it that gives you what you

Time: 1748.52

and I would call the full sensory experience.

Time: 1751.46

But, but we scientists need to reduce

Time: 1754.67

the problem into its basic elements.

Time: 1757.13

So we can begin to break it apart

Time: 1759.92

before we put it back together.

Time: 1761.81

So when we think about the sense of taste,

Time: 1765.17

and we try to figure out how these lines of information

Time: 1769.22

go from your tongue to your brain

Time: 1770.93

and how they signal and how they could integrate it

Time: 1773.63

and how they trigger all these different behaviors.

Time: 1776.6

We look at them as individual qualities,

Time: 1779.51

so we give the animals sweet,

Time: 1781.01

or we give them a bitter.

Time: 1782.884

We give them sour,

Time: 1783.717

we avoid mixes because the first stage of

Time: 1789.595

discovery is to have that clarity as to what you're trying

Time: 1794.54

to extract so that you can hopefully meaningfully make

Time: 1798.65

a difference by being able to figure out

Time: 1802.19

how easy that A goes to B to C and to D,

Time: 1806.09

does this make sense?

Time: 1806.923

- Yeah, almost like the primary colors

Time: 1808.67

to create the full array of the color spectrum.

Time: 1811.4

- Exactly.

Time: 1812.81

- Before I ask you about the first

Time: 1815.03

and second and third stages of taste and flavor perception,

Time: 1818.69

is there any idea that there may be more than five?

Time: 1823.244

- There is, for example, what about fat?

Time: 1827.15

- I love the taste.

Time: 1828.811

- Oh, I love fat too.

Time: 1830.1

And I love the texture of fat,

Time: 1830.933

especially if it's slightly burnt,

Time: 1832.79

like in South America, when I visited Buenos Aries,

Time: 1836.27

I found that at the end of a meal, they would take a steak,

Time: 1838.79

the trimming off the edge of the steak, burn it slightly,

Time: 1842.72

and then serve it back to me.

Time: 1844.13

And I thought that's disgusting.

Time: 1846.38

And then I tasted it and it's delightful.

Time: 1849.11

- It is.

Time: 1849.943

- There's nothing quite like it.

Time: 1851.69

- This goes back to this notion

Time: 1853.28

before that we like to live on the edge.

Time: 1856.43

And we like to do things

Time: 1858.407

that we should not be doing, Andrew,

Time: 1860.57

but on the other hand, look at those muscles.

Time: 1866.54

- I don't suggest anyone eat pure fat.

Time: 1868.85

The listeners of this podcast will immediately,

Time: 1870.5

I'm sure there'll be a YouTube video soon

Time: 1872.27

that I like eating pure fat.

Time: 1873.65

I'm not on a ketogenic diet, et cetera,

Time: 1878.16

but fat is tasty as evidenced by

Time: 1881.66

the obesity problem that exists in this country.

Time: 1884.487

- We'll talk about that.

Time: 1885.649

You know, in a little bit about

Time: 1886.49

the gut brain axis,

Time: 1887.81

I think it'll be important to cover it

Time: 1890.09

because it's the other side of the taste system.

Time: 1894.985

And so missing taste, one is fat,

Time: 1898.64

although like you clearly highlighted a lot of fat taste

Time: 1905.15

in quotation marks is really

Time: 1907.49

the feeling of fat rolling on your tongue.

Time: 1913.01

And so there is a compelling argument

Time: 1917.81

that a lot of what we call fat taste,

Time: 1921.77

it's really mechanosensory,

Time: 1925.55

it's somatosensory cells,

Time: 1928.07

cells that are not responding to taste,

Time: 1930.8

but they're responding to mechanical stimulation

Time: 1934.31

of fat molecules rolling on the tongue

Time: 1937.07

that gives you that perception of fat.

Time: 1940.43

- I love the idea that there is a perception of fat,

Time: 1943.1

regardless of whether or not

Time: 1944.18

there's a dedicated receptor for fat,

Time: 1947.15

mostly because it's evoking sensations

Time: 1950.19

and imagery of the taste of slightly burnt fat.

Time: 1954.343

- For example.

Time: 1955.176

And another one you could argue is metallic taste.

Time: 1959.27

You know, I know exactly what it tastes like.

Time: 1962.3

You know, if you ask me to explain it,

Time: 1964.1

I will have a hard time,

Time: 1966.92

what are the palettes of that color

Time: 1970.137

that can allow me to define it?

Time: 1972.02

It wouldn't be easy,

Time: 1974.317

but I know precisely what it tastes like,

Time: 1978.272

take take a penny, put it in your mouth

Time: 1979.7

and you know what it tastes like.

Time: 1982.323

- Or blood. - Or blood.

Time: 1983.156

That's another very good example.

Time: 1986.646

And is there really a receptor

Time: 1988.7

for metallic taste or it's nothing

Time: 1991.25

but this magical combination

Time: 1993.26

of the activation of the existing lines,

Time: 1996.74

think of it as lines of information,

Time: 1998.39

just separate lines, like the keys of a piano.

Time: 2001.63

Sweet, sour, bitter, salt, umami,

Time: 2003.46

you play the key and you activate a one chord.

Time: 2006.61

And that one chord in the case of a piano leads to a note,

Time: 2009.88

you know a tune and in the case

Time: 2012.43

of taste leads to an action and a behavior,

Time: 2015.94

but you play many of them together.

Time: 2017.65

And something emerges

Time: 2018.91

that it's different than any one of the pieces.

Time: 2025.772

And it's possible that metallic, for example,

Time: 2028.3

represents the combination of the activity just

Time: 2031.3

in the right ratio makes of these other lines.

Time: 2036.61

- Makes sense.

Time: 2037.78

And it actually provides a perfect,

Time: 2040.256

your example of the piano provides

Time: 2041.71

a perfect segue for what I'd like to touch on next,

Time: 2044.35

which is if you would describe the sequence

Time: 2050.025

of neural events leading to a perceptual event of taste.

Time: 2053.26

And I'm certain that somewhere in there you'll embed

Time: 2057.37

an answer to the question of whether

Time: 2060.54

or not we indeed have different taste receptors

Time: 2062.62

distributed in different locations on our tongue.

Time: 2066.19

Or elsewhere in the mouth.

Time: 2067.57

- Yes.

Time: 2068.56

So let's start by the banking

Time: 2073.06

that old tale and myth.

Time: 2076.12

- Who came up with that?

Time: 2078.88

- There are many views,

Time: 2081.135

but the most prevalent is that there was an original drawing

Time: 2085.96

describing the sensitivity

Time: 2088.57

of the tongue to different tastes.

Time: 2092.5

So imagine I can take a Q-Tip,

Time: 2095.56

this is a thought experiment,

Time: 2098.574

and I'm going to dip that Q-Tip in salt and in quinine

Time: 2103.244

as something bitter and glucose as something sweet.

Time: 2106.69

And I'm going to take that Q-tip,

Time: 2110.042

ask you to stick your tongue out

Time: 2111.43

and start moving around your tongue and ask you,

Time: 2114.31

what do you feel?

Time: 2117.73

And then I'm going to change the concentration

Time: 2120.52

of the amount of salt or the amount of bitter and ask,

Time: 2125.23

can I get some sort of a map of sensitivity

Time: 2128.978

to the different tastes?

Time: 2132.483

And the argument that has emerged is that there

Time: 2134.953

is a good likelihood that the data

Time: 2138.1

was simply mistranslated as it was being drawn.

Time: 2144.015

And of course, that led to an entire industry.

Time: 2146.65

This is the way you maximize your wine experience,

Time: 2151.57

because now we're going to form the vessel

Time: 2155.35

that you're going to drink from

Time: 2156.73

so that it acts maximally on the receptors which happen.

Time: 2162.25

Now, there is no tongue map.

Time: 2166.84

All right, we have taste buds distributed

Time: 2169.81

in various parts of the tongue.

Time: 2173.005

So there is a map on the distribution of taste bud,

Time: 2177.37

but each taste bud has around

Time: 2178.99

a hundred taste receptor cells.

Time: 2182.32

And those taste receptor cells can be of five types,

Time: 2187.87

sweet, sour, bitter, salty, or umami.

Time: 2191.08

And for the most part,

Time: 2195.13

all taste buds have the representation

Time: 2198.22

of all five taste qualities.

Time: 2201.16

Now there's no question that there is a slight bias

Time: 2203.68

for some taste, like bitter,

Time: 2206.14

is particularly rich at the very back of your tongue.

Time: 2211.06

And there is a teleological basis for that,

Time: 2213.34

actually a biological basis for that.

Time: 2216.07

That's the last line of defense

Time: 2217.87

before you swallow something bad.

Time: 2221.68

And so let's make sure that the very back of your tongue

Time: 2225.07

has plenty of these bad news receptors

Time: 2229.48

so that if they get activated,

Time: 2231.37

you can trigger a gagging reflex

Time: 2235.15

and get rid of this that otherwise may kill you.

Time: 2239.08

- Makes good sense.

Time: 2240.04

- But the notion that all sweet is

Time: 2243.735

in the front and salt is on the side.

Time: 2246.864

It's not real.

Time: 2251.47

Go ahead.

Time: 2252.303

- Oh, I was just going to ask, first of all,

Time: 2254.08

thank you for dispelling that myth.

Time: 2256.39

And we will propagate

Time: 2257.62

that information as far and wide as we can.

Time: 2260.26

'Cause I think that's the number one myth related to taste.

Time: 2263.38

The other one is,

Time: 2264.67

are there taste receptors anywhere else in the mouth,

Time: 2267.79

for instance, on the lips?

Time: 2269.907

- The palette, the palette, not the lips.

Time: 2271.505

So it's in the pharynx at the very back of the oral cavity,

Time: 2276.46

the tongue and the palette and the palette

Time: 2279.31

is very rich in sweet receptors.

Time: 2282.37

- I'll have to pay attention to this

Time: 2283.51

the next time I eat something sweet.

Time: 2285.67

- When you pull it up, yeah.

Time: 2287.53

Now the important thing is that

Time: 2293.26

after the receptors for these five, the detectors,

Time: 2297.88

the molecules that send sweet, sour, bitter, salt, umami,

Time: 2302.26

these are receptors, proteins found on the surface

Time: 2305.35

of taste receptor cells that interact with these chemicals.

Time: 2309.31

And once they interact,

Time: 2310.48

then they trigger the cascade of events, biochemical events,

Time: 2314.47

inside cell that now sends an electrical signal

Time: 2317.92

that says there is sweet here, or there is salt here.

Time: 2323.11

Now having these receptors and my laboratory identify

Time: 2326.53

the receptors for all five basic taste classes,

Time: 2329.65

sweet, bitter, salt, umami,

Time: 2331.18

and most recently sour.

Time: 2334.15

Now completing the palette.

Time: 2335.59

You can now use these receptors to really map

Time: 2338.32

where are they found in the tongue in a very rigorous way.

Time: 2342.7

This is no longer about using a Q-Tip

Time: 2345.22

and trying to figure out what you're feeling,

Time: 2349.27

but rather what you have in your tongue.

Time: 2354.23

This is not a guess.

Time: 2355.503

This is now a physical map that says

Time: 2359.38

the sweet receptors are found here.

Time: 2362.5

The bitter are found here.

Time: 2363.88

And when you do that, you find that in fact,

Time: 2365.98

every taste bud throughout your oral cavity

Time: 2370.75

has receptors for all of the basic taste classes.

Time: 2375.25

- Amazing.

Time: 2376.48

And as it turns out,

Time: 2378.16

and I'm sure you'll tell us important in terms of thinking

Time: 2380.62

about how the brain computes,

Time: 2383.17

encodes and decodes this thing we call taste.

Time: 2386.2

I'm going to inject a quick question that I'm sure is on

Time: 2388.93

many people's minds before we get back

Time: 2390.97

into the biological circuit,

Time: 2392.86

which is many people, including myself,

Time: 2394.99

are familiar with the experience of drinking

Time: 2396.7

a sip of tea or coffee that is too hot,

Time: 2400.39

and burning my tongue is the way I would describe it.

Time: 2403.343

- Horrible. - Horrible.

Time: 2404.97

And then disrupting my sense of taste

Time: 2406.18

for some period of time afterward.

Time: 2408.82

When I experienced that phenomenon,

Time: 2412.09

that unfortunate phenomenon have

Time: 2415.212

I destroyed taste receptors that regenerate,

Time: 2418.06

or have I somehow used temperature

Time: 2420.67

to distort the function of the circuit

Time: 2422.59

so that I no longer taste the way I did before?

Time: 2426.13

- Excellent question.

Time: 2427.18

And the answer is both.

Time: 2429.13

It turns out that your taste receptors

Time: 2432.67

only live for around two weeks, and this, by the way,

Time: 2437.62

makes sense because here you have an organ, the tongue,

Time: 2443.35

that is continuously exposed to everything

Time: 2446.65

you could range from the nicest,

Time: 2448.18

to the most horrible possible things.

Time: 2450.25

- Use your imagination.

Time: 2452.29

- And so you need to make sure

Time: 2456.64

that these cells are always coming back

Time: 2458.77

in a way that can reexperience the world

Time: 2461.86

in the right way and there are other organs

Time: 2464.98

that have the same underlying logic.

Time: 2469

Your gut, your intestines are the same way.

Time: 2471.61

Amazing.

Time: 2472.66

Again, they're receiving everything that you ingest.

Time: 2476.59

God forbid what's there, from the spiciest

Time: 2481.058

to the most horrible tastings or the most delicious.

Time: 2485.86

And again,

Time: 2486.88

those intestinal cells whose role is to ultimately

Time: 2491.2

take all these nutrients and bring them into the body.

Time: 2494.98

Also renew in a very, very fast cycle,

Time: 2502.868

olfactory neurons in your nose is the example.

Time: 2508

So then A, yes,

Time: 2509.83

you're burning a lot of your cells and is over for those.

Time: 2514.72

The good news is that they're going to come back,

Time: 2517.51

but we know that when you burn yourself with tea,

Time: 2520.694

they come back within 20 minutes,

Time: 2523.514

30 minutes, an hour,

Time: 2525.04

and these cells are not renewing in that timeframe.

Time: 2529.57

They're not listening to your needs.

Time: 2531.55

They have their own internal clock.

Time: 2535.24

And so you are really affecting,

Time: 2542.02

you're damaging them in a way that they can recover.

Time: 2546.7

And then they come back

Time: 2547.81

and you also damage your somatosensory cells.

Time: 2550.42

These are the results that feel things, not taste things.

Time: 2556.342

And then you wait half an hour or so,

Time: 2559.81

and then my goodness, thank God, it's back to normal.

Time: 2564.55

- And most of the time,

Time: 2565.48

I don't even notice the transition realizing,

Time: 2568.157

as you tell me, and I'll later,

Time: 2571.36

I'll ask you about the relationship between odor and taste.

Time: 2574.75

But as a next step, along this circuit,

Time: 2578.92

let's assume I ingest some, let's keep it simple.

Time: 2581.86

A sweet taste.

Time: 2583.27

- Let's make it even simpler.

Time: 2587.74

But at the same time, perhaps more informative,

Time: 2591.49

let's compare and contrast sweet and bitter

Time: 2595.09

as we follow their lines from the tongue to the brain.

Time: 2600.25

So the first thing is that

Time: 2601.353

the two evoke diametrically opposed behaviors.

Time: 2605.14

If we have to come up with two sensory experience

Time: 2608.35

that represent polar opposites,

Time: 2609.94

it will be sweet and video.

Time: 2611.35

There are not two colors that represent polar opposites

Time: 2613.75

because you could say black and white,

Time: 2615.73

they are polar opposites,

Time: 2616.78

one detects only one thing.

Time: 2619.046

The other one detects everything,

Time: 2620.62

but they don't evoke different behaviors.

Time: 2624.97

- Even political parties have some overlap.

Time: 2628.66

- Sweet and bitter are the two opposite ends

Time: 2631.03

of the sensory spectrum,

Time: 2633.61

now, a taste can be defined by two features.

Time: 2640.355

Again, I'm a reductionist,

Time: 2642.61

so I'm reducing it in a way that I think

Time: 2645.4

it's easier to follow the signal.

Time: 2647.71

And the two features are its quality and its valance

Time: 2653.05

and valance with a little V

Time: 2656.2

that's what we say in Spanish, with a V,

Time: 2660.7

means the value of that experience, right?

Time: 2667.06

Or in this case of that stimuli and you take sweet,

Time: 2672.46

sweet has a quality, an identity,

Time: 2676.63

and that's what you and I will refer

Time: 2678.43

to as the taste of sweet.

Time: 2680.53

We know exactly what it tastes like,

Time: 2684.22

But sweet also has a positive valance,

Time: 2689.53

which makes it incredibly attractive and appetitive,

Time: 2693.31

but it's attractive and appetitive,

Time: 2695.41

as I'll tell you in a second independent

Time: 2698.967

of its identity and quality.

Time: 2700.81

In fact, we have been able

Time: 2701.89

to engineer animals where we completely

Time: 2705.25

remove the valance from the stimuli.

Time: 2708.19

So these animals can taste sweet,

Time: 2710.86

can recognize it as sweet,

Time: 2714.28

but it's no longer attractive.

Time: 2716.71

It's just one more chemical stimuli.

Time: 2720.49

And that's because the identity and the valence

Time: 2724.39

are encoded in two separate parts of the brain.

Time: 2728.65

In the case of bitter,

Time: 2730.99

again, it has on the one hand its identity,

Time: 2734.012

its quality, and you know exactly what bitter tastes like.

Time: 2738.88

- I can taste it now, even as you describe it.

Time: 2741.46

- But it also has a valence and that's

Time: 2743.77

a negative valance because it evokes aversive behaviors.

Time: 2750.1

Are we on?

Time: 2750.933

- Absolutely.

Time: 2751.81

And it comes to mind.

Time: 2752.643

I remember telling some kids recently

Time: 2754.12

that we're going to go get ice cream and it was interesting.

Time: 2755.95

They looked up and they started smacking

Time: 2758.311

their like, they'll actually.

Time: 2759.61

- The anticipatory response.

Time: 2763.06

Absolutely.

Time: 2764.86

When we talk about the gut brain maybe we'll get there.

Time: 2767.92

So then the signals, if we follow now, these two lines,

Time: 2770.89

they're really like two separate keys

Time: 2773.14

at the two ends of this keyboard.

Time: 2776.71

And you press one key and you activate this cord.

Time: 2781.21

So you activate the sweet cells throughout your oral cavity.

Time: 2785.14

And they all converge into a group of sweet neurons

Time: 2789.31

in the next station, which is still outside the brain,

Time: 2794.65

it's one of the taste ganglia.

Time: 2798.16

These are the neurons that innervate your tongue

Time: 2801.503

and the oral cavity.

Time: 2802.6

- Where do they sit approximately?

Time: 2804.13

- Around there. - Right here around there.

Time: 2806.11

The lymph nodes, more or less?

Time: 2807.25

- You got it.

Time: 2809.958

And there are two main ganglia that innervate

Time: 2814.15

the vast majority of all taste buds in the oral cavity.

Time: 2819.28

And then from there that sweet signal

Time: 2822.58

goes onto the brain stem.

Time: 2824.8

The brain stem is the entry of the body into the brain.

Time: 2830.08

And there are different areas of the brain stem.

Time: 2832.96

And there are different groups of neurons in the brain stem.

Time: 2835.78

And there's this unique area in a unique,

Time: 2841.711

a topographically defined location in the rostral side

Time: 2848.26

of the brain that receives all of the taste input.

Time: 2852.7

- A very dense area of the brain.

Time: 2854.513

- A very rich area of the brain.

Time: 2857.8

Exactly.

Time: 2859.03

And from there,

Time: 2860.508

the sweet signal goes to this other area

Time: 2862.81

higher up on the brain stem.

Time: 2866.95

And then it goes through a number of stations where

Time: 2871.21

that sweet signal goes from sweet neuron to sweet neuron,

Time: 2875.44

to sweet neuron, to eventually get to your cortex.

Time: 2880.3

And once it gets to your taste cortex,

Time: 2883.18

that's where meaning is imposed into that signal.

Time: 2888.58

It's then, and only then this is what

Time: 2893.11

the data suggests that now you

Time: 2896.17

can identify this as a sweet stimuli.

Time: 2900.76

- [Andrew] And how quickly does that all happen?

Time: 2904.399

- You know, the time scale of the nervous system it's fast.

Time: 2909.832

- So within less than a second.

Time: 2912.07

- [Charles] Yeah, absolutely.

Time: 2914.409

- I rarely mistake bitter for sweet.

Time: 2916.48

Maybe with respect to people and my own poor judgment,

Time: 2919.48

but not with respect to taste.

Time: 2923.11

- And in fact,

Time: 2924.803

we can demonstrate this because we can stick electrodes

Time: 2926.83

at each of these stations, conceptually,

Time: 2930.007

and we can stimulate the tongue,

Time: 2931.96

and then we can record the signals pretty much

Time: 2934.57

time log the stimulus delivery.

Time: 2937.42

You deliver the stimuli and within a fraction of a second,

Time: 2941.5

you see now the response in this following stations,

Time: 2945.73

now it gets to the cortex,

Time: 2948.1

and now in there you impose meaning to that taste.

Time: 2952.63

There is an area of your brain that represents

Time: 2957.1

the taste of sweet in taste cortex

Time: 2960.49

and a different area that represents the taste of bitter.

Time: 2966.1

In a sense, there is a topographic map

Time: 2969.07

of these taste qualities inside your brain.

Time: 2973.09

Now we're going to do a thought experiment.

Time: 2976.27

Now, if this group of neurons

Time: 2977.95

in your cortex really represents

Time: 2980.89

the sense of sweet and this added different group

Time: 2984.34

of neurons in your brain represents the taste,

Time: 2989.32

the perception of bitter,

Time: 2992.417

then we should be able to do two things.

Time: 2995.83

First, I should be able to go into your brain,

Time: 2998.77

somehow silence those neurons,

Time: 3003

find a way to prevent them from being activated.

Time: 3009.216

And I can give you all the sweet you want,

Time: 3010.049

and you'll never know that you're tasting sweet.

Time: 3014.19

And conversely, I should be able to go into your brain,

Time: 3018.3

come up with a way to activate those neurons

Time: 3021.75

while I'm giving you absolutely nothing.

Time: 3025.17

And you're going to think that you are getting

Time: 3028.02

that full percept and that's precisely what we have done.

Time: 3034.08

And that's precisely what you get.

Time: 3036.75

This of course is in the brain of mice.

Time: 3039.24

- But presumably in humans it would work similarly.

Time: 3041.67

- Absolutely the same, zero doubt.

Time: 3044.19

I have no questions.

Time: 3045.57

So this attests to two important things,

Time: 3050.1

the first to the predetermined nature of the sense of taste,

Time: 3055.56

because it means I can go to these parts

Time: 3057.6

of your brain in the absence of any stimuli.

Time: 3061.2

And have you throw the full behavioral experience.

Time: 3066.12

In fact, when we activate in your cortex,

Time: 3068.43

these bitter neurons, the animal can start gagging,

Time: 3074.16

but it's drinking only water,

Time: 3078.39

but the animal thinks that's getting a bitter stimuli.

Time: 3084.112

[audience applauds] This is amazing.

Time: 3085.11

- And so, and the second,

Time: 3087.197

just to finish the line so

Time: 3088.03

that it doesn't sound like it teaches two things

Time: 3090.21

and then I only give you one lesson.

Time: 3096.342

It is that it substantiates

Time: 3100.372

this capacity of the brain to segregate,

Time: 3102.542

to separate in this nodes of action,

Time: 3106.17

the representation of these

Time: 3107.88

two diametrically opposed percepts.

Time: 3111.93

Which is sweet, for example, versus bitter.

Time: 3114.99

- The reason I say amazing,

Time: 3116.25

and that is also amazing is the following,

Time: 3120.51

you told us earlier, and you're absolutely correct,

Time: 3122.79

of course, that the end of the day,

Time: 3125.94

whether or not it's one group of neurons over here,

Time: 3127.86

and another group of neurons over there,

Time: 3130.35

which is the way it turns out to be,

Time: 3133.08

electrical activity is the generic,

Time: 3135.66

common language of both sets of neurons.

Time: 3139.638

So that raises the question for me,

Time: 3140.471

of whether or not those separate sets of neurons

Time: 3143.19

are connected to areas of the brain

Time: 3144.96

that create this sense of valence or whether

Time: 3148.59

or not they're simply created connected, excuse me,

Time: 3151.86

to sets of neurons that evoke distinct behaviors of moving

Time: 3154.95

towards and inhaling more and licking or aversive are

Time: 3157.95

we essentially interpreting our behavior

Time: 3160.44

and our micro responses or are micro responses

Time: 3164.1

and our behaviors the consequence of the percept?

Time: 3166.38

- Excellent, excellent question.

Time: 3169.736

So first answer is they go into

Time: 3174.895

an area of the brain where valence is imposed,

Time: 3180.24

and that area is known as the amygdala and the sweet neurons

Time: 3185.34

go to a different area than the bitter neurons.

Time: 3188.67

Now I want to do a thought experiment

Time: 3190.56

because I think your audience might appreciate this.

Time: 3192.75

Let's say I activate this group of neurons

Time: 3196.59

and the animal increases licking

Time: 3199.95

and I'm activating the sweet neurons.

Time: 3202.08

And so that's expected because now it's tasting

Time: 3205.5

this water as if it was sugar.

Time: 3209.407

Now this Moses transforming water into wine

Time: 3212.13

in this case we're going to, and today's Passover,

Time: 3214.29

so this is an appropriate example,

Time: 3218.7

we're transforming it into sweet.

Time: 3224.614

But how do I know that activating them is evoking

Time: 3228.36

a positive feeling inside, a goodness, a satisfaction,

Time: 3233.43

or I love it versus I'm just increasing licking,

Time: 3238.517

which is the other option,

Time: 3240.3

because all we're seeing is that the animal is leaking more.

Time: 3242.79

And we're trying to infer that that means

Time: 3245.67

that he's feeling something

Time: 3246.87

really good versus you know what,

Time: 3248.94

that piano line is going back straight into the tongue.

Time: 3251.73

And all it's doing is forcing it to move faster.

Time: 3255.75

Well, we can actually separate this

Time: 3258

by doing experiments that allows

Time: 3260.22

to fundamentally distinguish them

Time: 3264.03

and imagine the following experiment,

Time: 3266.07

I'm going to take the animal and I'm going to

Time: 3267.87

put them inside a box that has two sides.

Time: 3272.73

And the two sides have features that make him different.

Time: 3275.52

One has yellow little toys.

Time: 3278.91

The other one has green toys.

Time: 3281.34

One has little black stripes.

Time: 3283.74

The other one has blue stripes.

Time: 3285.33

So the animal can tell the two halves, I take the mouse,

Time: 3289.23

put them inside this arena, this play arena.

Time: 3293.61

And it will explore and potter

Time: 3295.62

around both sides with equal frequency.

Time: 3300.198

And now what I'm going to do

Time: 3301.615

is I'm going to activate these neurons,

Time: 3303.63

these sweet neurons,

Time: 3306.06

every time the animal

Time: 3307.14

is on the side with the yellow stripes.

Time: 3316.051

And if that is creating a positive internal state,

Time: 3321.09

what would the animal now want to do?

Time: 3324.06

It will want to stay on the side with the yellow stripes.

Time: 3330.09

There's no licking here.

Time: 3331.808

The animal is not extending its tongue

Time: 3333.33

every time I'm activating this neurons.

Time: 3336.36

This is known as a place preference test.

Time: 3340.53

And it's generally used,

Time: 3342.37

it's just one form of many different tests to demonstrate

Time: 3348.69

that the activation of a group

Time: 3352.59

of neurons in the brain is imposing,

Time: 3355.62

for example, a positive versus a negative valance.

Time: 3359.79

Whereas if I do the same thing

Time: 3361.56

by activating the bitter neurons,

Time: 3363.42

the animal will actively want now to stay away

Time: 3366.75

from the side where these neurons are being activated.

Time: 3372.36

- And that's precisely what you see.

Time: 3374.33

- [Charles] And that's precisely what we see.

Time: 3376.41

- Many people, including myself,

Time: 3377.88

are familiar with the experience

Time: 3379.29

of going to a restaurant, eating a variety of foods.

Time: 3384.167

And then fortunately doesn't happen that often,

Time: 3385.26

but then feeling very sick.

Time: 3387.6

I learned coming up in neuroscience

Time: 3389.55

that this is one strong example of one trial learning,

Time: 3394.53

that from that point on,

Time: 3397.29

it's not the restaurant or the waitress or waiter,

Time: 3401.04

or the date,

Time: 3402.45

but it's my notion of it had to have been the shrimp

Time: 3407.22

that leads me to then want to avoid shrimp in every context,

Time: 3410.28

maybe even shrimp powder.

Time: 3412.05

- You got it.

Time: 3412.95

- For a very long time.

Time: 3414.39

I can imagine all the evolutionarily

Time: 3416.79

adaptive reasons why this such a phenomenon would exist.

Time: 3420.39

Do we have any concept of where in this pathway it exists?

Time: 3424.049

- We do, we know actually a significant amount

Time: 3427.994

at a general level.

Time: 3431.1

In fact, more than shrimp,

Time: 3432.84

oysters are even a more dramatic example,

Time: 3435.81

one bad oyster is all you need to be driven

Time: 3440.13

that way for the next six months.

Time: 3442.993

- I think because the texture alone is something

Time: 3444.54

that one learns to overcome.

Time: 3445.71

I actually really enjoy oysters.

Time: 3447.63

I despise mussels, despise shrimp, not the animal,

Time: 3452.52

but the taste and yet oysters, for some reason,

Time: 3455.28

I've yet to have a bad experience.

Time: 3456.93

- It's like, uni, by the way

Time: 3458.834

texture is hard to get over, but once you get over,

Time: 3462.15

it's delicious.

Time: 3463.23

- That's what they tell me.

Time: 3464.82

We were both in San Diego at one point,

Time: 3466.53

and I'll give a plug to Sushi Ota,

Time: 3468.72

is kind of the famous place,

Time: 3471.047

and they have amazing uni and I've tried it twice.

Time: 3473.28

And I'm oh for two.

Time: 3476.206

It somehow the texture outweighs

Time: 3477.69

any kind of the deliciousness that people report.

Time: 3480.585

- It's a very acquired taste.

Time: 3482.1

It's like beer, I grew up in Chile,

Time: 3485.79

that's where the accent comes from in case anyone wonder.

Time: 3489.96

And by the time I came here to graduate school,

Time: 3492.36

I was 19, too old to overcome my heavy Chilian accent.

Time: 3501.327

So here I am, 40 years, 50 years late,

Time: 3502.59

not quite 40 plus-

Time: 3503.979

- We appreciate it.

Time: 3504.812

- And I still sound like I just came off the boat.

Time: 3509.07

So in Chile, you don't drink beer when you're young,

Time: 3511.68

you drink wine.

Time: 3512.82

Chili's a huge wine producer.

Time: 3516.03

So when I came to the US,

Time: 3520.144

all of my classmates were drinking beer because

Time: 3524.543

they had finished college where they were all

Time: 3526.65

you're drinking and graduate school,

Time: 3529.56

you're working 18 hours a day every day,

Time: 3532.89

the way they relax, let's go and have some beers.

Time: 3535.98

- And beer is cheaper.

Time: 3537.06

- And beer is cheap,

Time: 3538.77

and we were being clearly underpaid may I add.

Time: 3544.53

I couldn't do it.

Time: 3545.67

It's an acquired taste.

Time: 3547.53

It was too late by then.

Time: 3549.57

And here I am 60 plus.

Time: 3552.63

And if you take all the beer I've drank in my entire life,

Time: 3556.11

I would say they add to less than

Time: 3559.643

an eight ounce glass of water.

Time: 3561.39

- Impressive.

Time: 3562.223

Well, your health is probably better for it.

Time: 3564

- I'm not sure.

Time: 3566.01

- Your physical health anyway.

Time: 3567.3

- So it goes back to acquired taste.

Time: 3570.54

This is the connection to uni and to oysters.

Time: 3574.912

Now, going back to the one trial learning

Time: 3576.57

this is the great thing about our brains,

Time: 3578.85

certain things we need to repeat

Time: 3580.5

a hundred times to learn them, hello operator.

Time: 3584.552

Can I have the phone number for sushi Ota, please?

Time: 3587.19

And then she'll give it to you over the phone,

Time: 3589.47

at least in the old days.

Time: 3591.27

And then you need to repeat it to yourself over

Time: 3594.42

and over and over, over the next minute.

Time: 3596.894

So you can dial sushi Ota,

Time: 3599.07

and five minutes later, it's gone.

Time: 3603.15

That's what we call working memory.

Time: 3606.57

Then there is the short term memory.

Time: 3608.52

We park our car.

Time: 3610.65

And if we're lucky, by the end of the day,

Time: 3612.21

we remember where it is.

Time: 3615.06

And then there is the long term memory.

Time: 3616.74

We remember the birthdays of every one

Time: 3618.66

of our children for the rest of our lives.

Time: 3622.68

Well, there are events that a single event is so traumatic

Time: 3628.17

that it activates the circuits in a way

Time: 3630.453

that it's a one trial learning.

Time: 3633

And the taste system is literally

Time: 3636.48

at the top of that food chain.

Time: 3639.3

And there is a phenomenon known

Time: 3642.73

as conditioned taste aversion.

Time: 3644.46

You can pair an attractive stimuli with a really bad one,

Time: 3653.011

and you can make an animal begin

Time: 3655.35

to vehemently dislike for example, sugar.

Time: 3660.03

And that's because you've conditioned the animals

Time: 3663

to now be averse to these otherwise nice taste

Time: 3668.52

because it's been associated with malaise.

Time: 3671.7

And when you do that,

Time: 3672.72

now you could begin to ask why has change in the signal

Time: 3677.46

as it travels from the tongue

Time: 3679.5

to the brain in a normal animal

Time: 3682.65

versus an animal where you have now transformed sweet

Time: 3686.16

from being attractive to being immersive.

Time: 3688.92

And this is the way now you begin to explore

Time: 3691.56

how the brain changes, the nature, the quality,

Time: 3696.48

the meaning of a stimuli as a function of its state.

Time: 3703.71

- I have a number of questions related to that,

Time: 3708.06

all of which relate to this idea of context,

Time: 3712.68

because you mentioned before that flavor is distinct

Time: 3714.93

from taste because flavor involves smell,

Time: 3716.79

texture, temperature, and some other features,

Time: 3719.305

uni, sea urchin being a good example of,

Time: 3723.63

I can sense the texture.

Time: 3725.34

It actually it's, nah, I won't describe

Time: 3727.44

what it reminds me of, for various reasons.

Time: 3732.75

The ability to place context into,

Time: 3735.48

to insert context into a perception,

Time: 3737.55

or rather to insert a perception

Time: 3739.59

into context is so powerful.

Time: 3744.344

And there's an element of kind of mystery about it.

Time: 3747.3

But if we start to think about some of the more nuance

Time: 3750.81

that we like to live at the edge, as you say,

Time: 3755.22

how many different tastes on the taste dial

Time: 3759.39

to go back to your analogy earlier, the color dial,

Time: 3761.7

do you think that there could be for something

Time: 3763.5

as fixed as bitter, so for instance,

Time: 3766.53

I don't think I like bitter taste,

Time: 3769.44

but I like some fermented foods that seem to have a little

Time: 3772.65

bit of sour and have a little bit

Time: 3773.97

of that briny flavor.

Time: 3776.7

How much plasticity do you think there is there

Time: 3778.62

and in particular across the lifespan,

Time: 3781.05

because I think one of the most salient examples

Time: 3783

of this is that kids don't seem to like certain vegetables,

Time: 3787.56

but they all are hardwired to like sweet tastes.

Time: 3791.25

And yet you could also imagine that one of the reasons

Time: 3793.8

why they may eventually grow to incorporate vegetables is

Time: 3797.79

because of some knowledge that vegetables might be-

Time: 3800.01

- Good for you. - Better for them.

Time: 3802.823

So is there a change in the receptors, the distribution,

Time: 3806.43

the number, the sensitivity, et cetera,

Time: 3808.17

that can explain the transition

Time: 3810.24

from wanting to avoid vegetables,

Time: 3812.19

to being willing to eat vegetables

Time: 3814.924

simply in childhood to early development.

Time: 3817.5

Yes, so I going to take the question slightly differently,

Time: 3820.076

but I think it would illustrate the point.

Time: 3821.88

And I want to just

Time: 3824.565

use the difference between the olfactory system

Time: 3828.66

and the taste system to make the point,

Time: 3832.41

taste system five basic pallets,

Time: 3834.75

sweet, sour, bitter, salt, umami,

Time: 3836.4

each of them has a predetermined identity.

Time: 3838.71

We know exactly what and valence.

Time: 3841.159

These are attractive.

Time: 3842.73

These are aversive.

Time: 3844.5

In the olfactory system,

Time: 3845.82

it's claimed that we can smell millions

Time: 3847.89

of different odors, yet for the most part,

Time: 3854.07

none of them have an innate predetermined meaning,

Time: 3859.222

in olfactory system,

Time: 3861.99

meaning is imposed by learning and experience.

Time: 3866.13

- Even the smell of smoke?

Time: 3868.268

- So I'm going to give you,

Time: 3869.101

I'm going to make it differently.

Time: 3870.63

They are a handful of the millions

Time: 3873.93

of odors that were claimed that,

Time: 3876.543

that you could immediately tell me these

Time: 3879.06

are adversive and these are attractive.

Time: 3881.769

- Vomit.

Time: 3883.393

- So vomit, it's not correct

Time: 3886.726

because I can assure you that they're

Time: 3888.78

cultures and societies where things which are far less

Time: 3892.68

appealing than vomit do not evoke a adverse reaction.

Time: 3896.875

- Really?

Time: 3897.866

- Really, sulfur would be maybe a universal.

Time: 3900.12

I'm not talking pheromones.

Time: 3902.571

pheromones are in a different category

Time: 3904.231

that trigger innate responses,

Time: 3906.66

but nearly every other is afforded meaning

Time: 3912.48

by learning and experience.

Time: 3914.28

And that's why you like broccoli.

Time: 3916.56

And I despise broccoli because I remember

Time: 3919.44

my mother forcing me to eat broccoli.

Time: 3923.132

[audience applauds] I'm so sorry.

Time: 3924.073

- Same sensory experience.

Time: 3928.08

This accommodates two important things.

Time: 3931.745

In the case of taste,

Time: 3933.373

you have neurons at every station that are for sweet,

Time: 3935.19

for sour, for bitter, for salty, and umami,

Time: 3937.65

it's only five classes.

Time: 3938.91

So it's not going to take a lot of your brain.

Time: 3941.13

If we can, in fact, smell a million others,

Time: 3944.1

and everyone else of others had to have predetermined,

Time: 3946.5

meaning there's not going to be enough brain

Time: 3949.083

just to accommodate that one sense.

Time: 3953.76

And so evolution in its infinite wisdom,

Time: 3958.44

evolved a system where you put together

Time: 3963.395

a pathway and a cortex, olfactory cortex,

Time: 3966.407

where you have the capacity to associate every other

Time: 3973.8

in a specific context that now gives it the meaning.

Time: 3979.32

Now let's go back to the original question then.

Time: 3983.85

So other than clearly plastic, mega plastic,

Time: 3988.2

because it's fundamental basis and neural organization,

Time: 3993.72

but taste,

Time: 3994.553

we just told you that predetermined, hardwired,

Time: 3997.38

but predetermined, hardwired doesn't mean

Time: 3999.667

that it's not modulated by learning or experience.

Time: 4003.38

It only means that you are born liking sweet

Time: 4007.64

and disliking bitter.

Time: 4010.67

And we have many examples of plasticity,

Time: 4014.06

beer being one example.

Time: 4016.25

So why do we learn to love beer and coffee.

Time: 4020

It's because it has an associated gain to the system

Time: 4027.35

and that gain to the system, that positive valance

Time: 4031.46

that emerges out of that negative signal

Time: 4034.43

is sufficient to create that positive association.

Time: 4039.23

And in the case of beer, of course, it's alcohol.

Time: 4042.74

The feeling good that we get after

Time: 4045.23

is more than sufficient to say, I want to have more of this.

Time: 4050.348

And in the case of coffee, of course,

Time: 4051.35

is caffeine activating a whole group of neurotransmitter

Time: 4054.44

systems that give you that high associated with coffee.

Time: 4059.39

So yes, this taste system is changeable.

Time: 4062.841

It's malleable, and it's subject subjected

Time: 4065.179

to learning and experience.

Time: 4067.951

But I like the olfactory system is restricted

Time: 4071.57

in what you could do with it

Time: 4073.22

because its goal is to allow you

Time: 4077.12

to get nutrients and survive.

Time: 4079.58

The goal of the olfactory system is very different.

Time: 4082.64

It's being used, not in our case,

Time: 4084.77

but in every animal species

Time: 4087.603

to identify friend versus foe,

Time: 4091.598

to identify mate,

Time: 4095.258

to identify ecological niches they want to be in.

Time: 4098.87

So it plays a very broad role that then requires

Time: 4104.39

that it be set up organized

Time: 4106.82

and function in a very different type of context.

Time: 4110.6

Taste is about, can we get the nutrients we need to survive?

Time: 4115.16

And can we ensure that we are attracted to the ones we need?

Time: 4118.79

And we adverse to the ones that are going to kill us,

Time: 4122.42

I'm being overly simplistic and reductionist.

Time: 4126.031

But I think it illustrates the huge difference

Time: 4127.34

between these two chemo sensory systems.

Time: 4130.64

- I don't think you're being overly simplicity.

Time: 4133.01

I think it illustrates

Time: 4134.657

the key intractable nature of this system

Time: 4138.017

and the way you've approached it.

Time: 4140.154

And I think it's important for people to hear that,

Time: 4141.89

because everybody,

Time: 4142.91

as we are as mystified with empathy and love, et cetera.

Time: 4147.44

So in a fairness to that,

Time: 4149.3

I'm going to ask a sort of

Time: 4151.824

a high level question or abstract question.

Time: 4155.24

This was based on a conversation I had

Time: 4156.92

with a former girlfriend

Time: 4158.54

where we were talking about chemistry between individuals.

Time: 4162.26

Very complicated topic on the one hand.

Time: 4166.22

But on the other hand, quite simple in that certain people,

Time: 4171.11

for whatever reason, evoke a tremendous sense of arousal,

Time: 4175.01

for lack of a better word, between two people,

Time: 4177.86

one would hope,

Time: 4180.83

at least for some period of time.

Time: 4182.96

- I didn't know this was that kind of a podcast.

Time: 4185.03

- No, well, the reason I,

Time: 4187.31

but this has to do with taste because she said something,

Time: 4189.92

I think in part to maybe irritate me a bit,

Time: 4194.18

but we were commenting not

Time: 4196.52

about our own experience of each other,

Time: 4200.234

but of someone that she was now very excited

Time: 4202.52

about we're on good terms.

Time: 4205.82

And she said, "What do you think it is,

Time: 4208.82

this thing of chemistry?"

Time: 4210.17

So maybe she was trying to-

Time: 4212.21

- Warn you of what's coming.

Time: 4213.83

- Warn me what's coming.

Time: 4215.15

And she said,

Time: 4216.943

"I have a feeling something about it is in smell

Time: 4220.34

and something about it is actually in taste,

Time: 4223.37

literally the taste of somebody's breath."

Time: 4225.59

That's the way she described it.

Time: 4227.643

And I thought that it was a very interesting example

Time: 4231.05

for a number of reasons, but in particular,

Time: 4233.63

because it gets to the merging of odor and taste.

Time: 4237.38

But also to the idea that of course the context

Time: 4240.95

of a new relationship, I'm assuming that, and in fact,

Time: 4243.86

they're both attractive people, et cetera.

Time: 4245.54

There's a whole context there,

Time: 4246.71

but I've had the experience of the odor

Time: 4252.26

of somebody's breath being aversive.

Time: 4253.7

Not because I could identify it as aversive.

Time: 4257.3

- Because you just didn't like it.

Time: 4259.064

- But because I just didn't like it.

Time: 4261.2

- But that's because you associated

Time: 4264.59

with added odors that trigger

Time: 4270.65

that negative aversive reaction, by the way.

Time: 4276.14

- Absolutely.

Time: 4277.807

There are certain perfumes to me that are aversive.

Time: 4278.99

- [Charles] You got it.

Time: 4280.893

- And there are other sense,

Time: 4282.483

I can recall a sense of skin, of foods,

Time: 4285.68

et cetera, that are immensely appetitive.

Time: 4288.29

So I've experienced both sides of this equation myself.

Time: 4291.47

And she was describing this

Time: 4292.88

and to me more than tasting wine,

Time: 4295.7

which is the typical example

Time: 4297.2

where people inhale it and then they drink it, to me,

Time: 4299.72

this seems like something

Time: 4302.61

that more people might be able to relate to,

Time: 4305.123

that certain things and people smell delicious.

Time: 4307.52

Even mothers describing the smell of their baby's head.

Time: 4309.86

- A mother, or us.

Time: 4310.91

- [Andrew] Of course, men too, yeah.

Time: 4313.38

- I mean our own babies when in their necks,

Time: 4315.35

that's the magical place.

Time: 4316.85

- Their neck.

Time: 4318.289

- The back of their neck.

Time: 4319.397

- There you go. - Oh my goodness.

Time: 4320.54

I have a grandchild now.

Time: 4321.56

So I know exactly what, Rio, that's his name, smells like.

Time: 4325.49

- Okay.

Time: 4326.75

So more beautiful examples.

Time: 4327.583

It's always more fun to think

Time: 4329.024

about the beautiful positively appetitive examples.

Time: 4331.58

The smell of the back of your grandson's neck.

Time: 4335.54

I mean, you could get more specific than that,

Time: 4338.957

but not a lot more specific exactly.

Time: 4340.46

So what is going on in terms of the combination of odor

Time: 4344.03

and taste given that these two systems are so different?

Time: 4347.63

- Yes, and they come together,

Time: 4352.61

ultimately there is a place in the brain where they come

Time: 4356.57

together to integrate the two into

Time: 4360.32

what we would call that sensory experience.

Time: 4365.24

And I'll tell you an experiment

Time: 4366.62

that you could do that demonstrates this.

Time: 4370.239

I think it's good for the,

Time: 4372.494

for your audience here to get a sense of how we approach

Time: 4375.32

these problems so that we can get

Time: 4377.21

in a meaningful scientific answers.

Time: 4381.659

So we know where the olfactory cortex is in the brain.

Time: 4385.13

We know where the taste cortex is in the brain.

Time: 4387.08

They're in two different places.

Time: 4389.21

We can go to each of these two, cortexes,

Time: 4393.77

put color traces, we put green in one,

Time: 4397.52

we put red in the other and we see where the colors go to.

Time: 4401.96

That's a reflection of where

Time: 4405.263

those neurons are projecting into their next targets.

Time: 4409.16

Once they get the signal, where do they send the signal to?

Time: 4413.27

And then we reason that if odor

Time: 4416.24

and taste come together somewhere in the brain,

Time: 4418.97

we should find an area that now

Time: 4420.59

it's getting red and green color.

Time: 4424.19

And we found such an area.

Time: 4427.61

And next we anticipated,

Time: 4429.89

we hypothesized that maybe this is the area

Time: 4433.07

in the brain of the mouse,

Time: 4435.26

corresponding area in the brain of humans,

Time: 4438.907

that integrates odor and taste.

Time: 4443.09

It's known as,

Time: 4444.5

the term normally used is multisensory integration.

Time: 4448.97

And if this is true, we could do the following experiment.

Time: 4452.96

We can train a mouse

Time: 4457.04

to lick sweet.

Time: 4462.845

And if they guess correctly,

Time: 4463.97

that that is supposed to be sweet.

Time: 4465.86

They should go now to the right port, to the right side,

Time: 4471.26

to get a water reward.

Time: 4474.53

If they go to the lift, when it was sweet,

Time: 4478.16

then they're incorrect and they get no reward

Time: 4482.18

and they actually get a time out.

Time: 4484.88

Now the mice are thirsty,

Time: 4486.62

so they're very motivated to perform.

Time: 4489.02

And if you repeat this task a hundred times,

Time: 4492.44

a hundred trials, incredibly enough,

Time: 4495.11

this animal learned to recognize the sweet

Time: 4498.05

and execute the right action.

Time: 4500.12

And by their action,

Time: 4502.13

we now are being told what that animal is tasting.

Time: 4508.01

We can make it more interesting

Time: 4509.33

and we can give him sweet and bitter and say,

Time: 4511.19

if it's sweet go to the right

Time: 4513.764

and if it's bitter go to the left.

Time: 4515.36

And after you train him this mice with 90% accuracy,

Time: 4518.63

it'll tell you, when you randomize now the stimuli,

Time: 4522.2

what was sweet and what was bitter?

Time: 4525.56

We can now do the same experiment,

Time: 4528.32

but now mix taste with odor and say,

Time: 4533.66

if you got odor alone,

Time: 4538.37

go to the right or push this lever in mice.

Time: 4543.44

If you get taste alone,

Time: 4545.87

go to this other part or push this other lever.

Time: 4549.65

And if you get the two together, do this something else.

Time: 4553.58

And if you train the mice,

Time: 4555.02

the mice are able now to report back to you when it's

Time: 4558.14

sensing taste alone, odor alone, or the mix.

Time: 4565.01

Makes sense? - Makes sense.

Time: 4567.22

- Now we can go to the brain of this mice and go

Time: 4570.65

to this area that we now uncover,

Time: 4573.05

discover as being the side of multisensory integration

Time: 4577.16

between taste and odor, and silence it,

Time: 4582.02

prevent it from being activated experimentally.

Time: 4586.375

And if that area really represented

Time: 4589.52

the integration of these two,

Time: 4591.62

the animals should still be able

Time: 4593.09

to recognize the taste alone.

Time: 4595.16

They still should be able to recognize the other alone,

Time: 4598.19

but should be incapable now to recognize the mix,

Time: 4604.55

and exactly as predicted.

Time: 4606.95

That's exactly what you get.

Time: 4609.335

All right?

Time: 4610.22

- The brain is basically

Time: 4611.72

a series of engineered circuits, complex.

Time: 4615.47

- You got it.

Time: 4616.812

And our task is to figure out how can we extract

Time: 4623.18

this amazing architecture of these circuits

Time: 4627.033

in a way that we can begin to uncover

Time: 4630.02

the mysteries of the brain.

Time: 4632.96

- And why certain people's breath tastes so good

Time: 4636.506

and other people's not so good.

Time: 4638.75

- Exactly.

Time: 4639.583

So I never answered that,

Time: 4640.416

but I told you how we can figure out

Time: 4642.08

where in the brain it's happening.

Time: 4645.86

- As we've been having this discussion,

Time: 4648.185

I thought a few times about similarities

Time: 4652.106

to the visual system or differences to the visual system,

Time: 4654.11

the visual system,

Time: 4654.943

there are a couple of phenomenon that I wonder

Time: 4659.402

if they also exist in the taste system,

Time: 4661.693

and the visual system we know for instance,

Time: 4663.86

that if you look at something long enough

Time: 4664.693

and activate the given receptors long enough,

Time: 4667.04

that object will actually disappear.

Time: 4669.35

We offset this with little micro eye movements,

Time: 4671.36

et cetera, but the principle is a fundamental one.

Time: 4673.73

This habituation or desensitization,

Time: 4675.77

everyone seems to call it something different,

Time: 4677.63

but you get the idea of course,

Time: 4680.87

in the taste system,

Time: 4683.21

I'm certainly familiar with eating something very,

Time: 4685.25

very sweet for the first time in a long time.

Time: 4687.95

And it feel tastes very sweet, but a few more licks,

Time: 4690.8

a few more bites,

Time: 4691.67

and now it tastes not as sweet, with olfaction,

Time: 4695.09

I'm familiar with the odor in a room I don't like,

Time: 4697.52

or I like, and then it disappearing.

Time: 4698.93

So similar phenomena, where does that occur?

Time: 4704.949

And can you imagine a sort of a system

Time: 4709.01

by which people could leverage that?

Time: 4711.62

Because I do think that most people are interested

Time: 4715.448

in eating, not more sugar, but less sugar.

Time: 4718.638

- I think we have better ways to approach that.

Time: 4719.537

And we can transition from taste into these other circuits

Time: 4724.55

that makes sugar so extraordinarily

Time: 4730.04

impossible not to consume.

Time: 4733.58

- Impossible, exactly.

Time: 4737.658

- So where does the desensitizing happen.

Time: 4742.01

That's the term that we use, and it's,

Time: 4748.598

I think happening at multiple stations,

Time: 4751.97

it's happening at the receptor level.

Time: 4755.57

I.e. the salt in your tongue

Time: 4758.42

that are sensing the sugar,

Time: 4761.54

as you activate this receptor

Time: 4763.34

and it's triggering activity after activity after activity,

Time: 4767.06

eventually you exhaust the receptor again,

Time: 4770.12

I'm using terms which are extraordinarily loose.

Time: 4774.92

- But for sake of this discussion.

Time: 4776.867

- For the sake of this discussion the receptor

Time: 4780.2

gets to a point where he undergoes a set of changes,

Time: 4784.01

chemical changes, where it now signals far less efficiently,

Time: 4790.64

or it even gets removed from the surface of the cell.

Time: 4795.587

And now what will happen is that the same amount

Time: 4798.35

of sugar will trigger far less of a response.

Time: 4803.869

And that is a huge side of this modulation.

Time: 4809.24

And then the next, I believe is the integrated again,

Time: 4812.93

loss of signaling that happens by continuous activation

Time: 4817.31

of the circuit at each of these different neural stations.

Time: 4820.91

You know, there is from the tongue to the ganglia,

Time: 4823.76

from the ganglia to the first station

Time: 4826.078

in the brain stem, a second station in the brain stem,

Time: 4829.13

to the thalamus, then to the cortex.

Time: 4832.25

So there are multiple steps that this signal is traveling.

Time: 4835.19

Now, you might say why, if this is a label line,

Time: 4837.89

why do you need to have so many stations?

Time: 4841.07

And that's because the taste system is so important to

Time: 4843.89

ensure that you get what you need to survive,

Time: 4848.03

that it has to be subjected

Time: 4849.53

to modulation by the internal state.

Time: 4853.22

And each of these nodes provides a new site

Time: 4857.33

to give it plasticity and modulation,

Time: 4860.93

not necessarily to change the way that something tastes,

Time: 4865.04

but to ensure that you consume more

Time: 4868.73

or less or differently of what you need.

Time: 4872.9

I'm going to give you one example

Time: 4874.91

of how the internal state changes

Time: 4876.44

the way the taste system works.

Time: 4879.35

Salt is very appetitive at low concentrations.

Time: 4885.38

And that's because we need it.

Time: 4888.14

It's our electro lite balance requires salt.

Time: 4891.02

Every one of our neurons uses salt

Time: 4893.09

as the most important of the ions

Time: 4895.61

with potassium to ensure that you can transfer

Time: 4898.46

these electrical signals within and between neurons,

Time: 4904.4

but at high concentrations,

Time: 4906.11

let's say ocean water, it's incredibly aversive.

Time: 4910.85

And we all know this because we are going to the ocean.

Time: 4913.705

And then when you get it in your mouth, it's not that great.

Time: 4917.39

However, if I salt deprive you, and we can do this

Time: 4922.04

in experimental models quite readily,

Time: 4927.44

now this incredibly high concentration of salt,

Time: 4931.269

one molar sodium chloride becomes

Time: 4933.5

amazingly appetitive and attractive.

Time: 4937.04

What's going on in here.

Time: 4939.17

Your tongue is telling you, this is horrible,

Time: 4942.5

but your brain is telling you, I don't care.

Time: 4946.4

You need it.

Time: 4949.73

And this is what we call the modulation

Time: 4953.36

of the taste system by the internal state.

Time: 4957.65

- And presumably if one is hungry enough,

Time: 4959.42

even uni will taste good.

Time: 4962.822

Just hit you hit it right on the money.

Time: 4963.904

No, no, this is exactly correct.

Time: 4965.99

Or if you're thirsty and hungry,

Time: 4969.74

you suppress hunger so that you don't

Time: 4973.07

waste water molecules in digesting food.

Time: 4976.888

Why?

Time: 4977.96

Because if you're thirsty and you have no water,

Time: 4981.38

you will die within a week or so,

Time: 4984.38

but you can on a hunger strike as long

Time: 4986.57

as you have water for months,

Time: 4990.68

because you're going to eat up all your energy reserves.

Time: 4994.58

Water is a different story.

Time: 4996.02

So you could see,

Time: 4996.95

or that there are multiple layers

Time: 5000.22

at which the taste system that guides

Time: 5005.05

our drive and our motivation to consume

Time: 5008.26

the nutrients we need has to be modulated

Time: 5013.03

in response to the internal state.

Time: 5015.79

And of course,

Time: 5017.256

internal state itself has

Time: 5018.089

to be modulated by the external world.

Time: 5021.783

And so that I think is a reason why, what could otherwise,

Time: 5025.63

would've been an incredibly simple system

Time: 5027.58

from the tongue to the cortex in one just wire.

Time: 5032.53

It's not, because you have to ensure

Time: 5036.084

that each step you give the system

Time: 5039.31

that level of flexibility

Time: 5041.47

or what we call in neuroscience, plasticity.

Time: 5045.16

- I think we're headed into the gut.

Time: 5047.26

But I have a question that has just been

Time: 5050.29

on my mind for a bit now,

Time: 5052.87

because I was drinking this water

Time: 5055

and it has essentially no taste.

Time: 5058.33

Is there any kind of signal for the absence of taste despite

Time: 5062.5

having something in the mouth and here is why I ask,

Time: 5066.458

what I'm thinking about is saliva.

Time: 5069.729

And while it's true that if I eat a lot of very highly

Time: 5071.62

palatable foods that does change

Time: 5074.32

how I experience more bland foods,

Time: 5077.62

I must confess when I eat a lot of these highly processed

Time: 5080.38

foods I don't particularly like them.

Time: 5081.79

I tend to crave healthier foods,

Time: 5083.17

but that's probably for contextual reasons about nutrients,

Time: 5086.29

et cetera.

Time: 5088.15

But I could imagine an experiment where-

Time: 5090.61

- You see a taste of no taste.

Time: 5093.137

- Right, is there a taste of no taste,

Time: 5093.97

because in the visual system there is.

Time: 5096.22

You close the eyes and you start getting increases in

Time: 5098.68

activity in the visual system, as opposed to decreases,

Time: 5101.05

which often surprises people.

Time: 5102.61

But there are reasons for that because everything is about

Time: 5105.07

signal to noise, signal to background.

Time: 5107.77

- It's a good question.

Time: 5109.387

I can tell you that most of our work is trying to focus on

Time: 5112.03

how the taste system works, not how it doesn't work well,

Time: 5117.16

but you know.

Time: 5117.993

- Yeah, I know you're being playful.

Time: 5121.066

And I knew when inviting you here today,

Time: 5121.899

I was setting myself up for,

Time: 5123.43

actually on a different-

Time: 5125.11

- We're trying to learn things.

Time: 5127.066

- Yeah, I know. - However-

Time: 5128.108

- All right, listen, I was weaned in this system of a,

Time: 5130.69

and I'll say it here for the second.

Time: 5132.61

Actually I recorded a podcast recently with a very prominent

Time: 5135.82

podcast with Lex Friedman Podcast.

Time: 5137.59

And I made reference to the so-called

Time: 5140.357

New York neuroscience mafia.

Time: 5141.61

I won't say whether or not we are sitting in the presence of

Time: 5144.13

the New York neuroscience mafia member, but in any event,

Time: 5148.33

I know the sorts of ribbing

Time: 5150.01

that they provide for those listening,

Time: 5152.35

this is the kind of hazing that happens.

Time: 5154.21

Benevolent hazing in academia.

Time: 5157.06

I'm the target.

Time: 5157.93

- Of course, it's-

Time: 5159.34

- It's a sign of love.

Time: 5160.57

- Exactly. - He's going to tell me that.

Time: 5162.19

- And it's always about the science in the end, right.

Time: 5166.308

But it's an interesting question.

Time: 5167.41

Look, I am,

Time: 5169.57

I don't know the answer

Time: 5173.815

and I don't even know how I would explore it in a way

Time: 5176.823

that it will rigorously teach me, eh, but.

Time: 5186.49

- Let me tell you why I'm asking.

Time: 5187.87

And then I'll offer an experiment

Time: 5189.46

that I'd love to see someone here in the lab do.

Time: 5192.941

I'm thinking about saliva.

Time: 5194.29

- No, no, no, but that we know,

Time: 5195.55

that we can figure it out.

Time: 5197.459

- Obviously.

Time: 5198.859

But the question is whether or not the saliva in a fed state

Time: 5202.96

is distinct from the saliva in an unfed state,

Time: 5206.47

such that it modulates the sensitivity, the response.

Time: 5208.396

- It's not the sensitivity,

Time: 5209.585

the experiment has been done.

Time: 5211.124

- [Andrew] It has been done.

Time: 5211.974

And so the answer is no.

Time: 5213.25

- It's not.

Time: 5214.083

And the way you could do the experiment is

Time: 5215.966

because we use artificial saliva.

Time: 5218.771

- There's such a thing?

Time: 5219.604

I know there's artificial tears, but.

Time: 5221.5

- No, no, we,

Time: 5222.43

I don't mean that you go to Walgreens that you get, I mean,

Time: 5226.15

we, in my laboratory,

Time: 5227.89

we know the composition of saliva

Time: 5230.681

and so you can make such a thing,

Time: 5234.07

and you can take taste cells in culture

Time: 5237.37

or in a tongue where you wash it out,

Time: 5240.43

and then you can apply artificial saliva.

Time: 5243.34

And what happens is that the system

Time: 5246.232

is being engineered to desensitize,

Time: 5249.07

to become agnostic for saliva to become invisible.

Time: 5257.681

And there is no difference on the state of the animal.

Time: 5261.2

- Great, well, this is the reason to do experiments.

Time: 5262.24

- [Charles] Yeah, absolutely.

Time: 5263.827

- So it doesn't defeat any grand hypothesis.

Time: 5264.82

It's just a pure curiosity.

Time: 5268

- You know that curiosity kills the cat?

Time: 5269.92

- I do,

Time: 5273.97

but saves the career of a scientists every single time.

Time: 5278.114

- That's what drives us, absolutely.

Time: 5279.382

- Every single time.

Time: 5280.383

- It's the story of our lives. - Exactly.

Time: 5282.1

So if it's not saliva and apparently it is not,

Time: 5286.913

what about internal state and what aspects of this,

Time: 5291.97

the internal milieu are relevant because there's autonomic,

Time: 5295.15

there's a sleep and awake, there's stress.

Time: 5297.16

One of the questions that

Time: 5299.083

I got from hundreds of people when I solicited questions in

Time: 5301.57

advance of this episode was why do

Time: 5303.49

I crave sugar when I'm stressed, for instance,

Time: 5306.19

and that could be contextual,

Time: 5307.24

but what are the basic elements-

Time: 5309.04

- Because it makes us feel good by the way,

Time: 5310.72

we'll get to that.

Time: 5312.287

That's his answer. - Soothing.

Time: 5314.047

- It activates,

Time: 5316.18

what I'm going to generically refer to as reward pleasure

Time: 5322.03

centers in a way that it dramatically,

Time: 5325.958

changes our internal state.

Time: 5328.51

This is in why do we eat a gallon ice cream

Time: 5330.7

when we're very depressed?

Time: 5333.46

In fact, this is a good segue to go

Time: 5336.55

into this an entire different world

Time: 5340.84

of the body telling your brain

Time: 5345.76

what you need in important things like sugar and fat,

Time: 5351.31

but anyways, go ahead, you were going to ask something.

Time: 5354.618

- Well, no, I would like to discuss

Time: 5356.806

the most basic elements of internal state in particular,

Time: 5360.64

the ones that are below our conscious detection.

Time: 5363.19

And this is a,

Time: 5365.23

of course is a segue into this incredible landscape,

Time: 5369.22

which is the gut brain axis,

Time: 5371.11

which I think 15 years ago was almost a,

Time: 5375.55

maybe it was posters at a meeting.

Time: 5379.498

And then now I believe you and others,

Time: 5382.754

there are companies, there have companies,

Time: 5384.16

there are active research programs,

Time: 5387.58

and beautiful work.

Time: 5389.47

Maybe you could describe some of that work

Time: 5390.85

that you and others have been involved in.

Time: 5392.8

And a lot of the listeners of this podcast will have heard

Time: 5397.24

of the gut brain axis.

Time: 5398.38

And there are a lot of misconceptions

Time: 5400.63

about the gut brain axis.

Time: 5401.62

Many people think that this means that we think with our

Time: 5404.26

stomach because of the quote unquote, gut feeling aspect,

Time: 5409.702

but I'd love for you to talk about the aspects of gut brain

Time: 5412.24

signaling that drive or change our perceptions

Time: 5415.12

and behaviors that are completely beneath our awareness.

Time: 5418.3

- Yes. Excellent.

Time: 5419.83

So let me begin maybe by stating that,

Time: 5425.77

the brain needs to monitor

Time: 5430.66

the state of every one of our organs,

Time: 5434.59

it has to do it.

Time: 5436

This is the only way that the brain can ensure that every

Time: 5441.04

one of those organs are working together in a way that we

Time: 5445.63

have healthy physiology.

Time: 5449.38

Now,

Time: 5450.67

this monitoring of the brain has been known for a long time,

Time: 5455.38

but I think what hadn't been fully appreciated that this is

Time: 5458.26

a two-way highway where the brain is not only monitoring,

Time: 5464.14

but is now modulating back what the body needs to do.

Time: 5471.437

And that includes all the way from monitoring the frequency

Time: 5474.55

of heartbeats and the way that inspiration and aspirations

Time: 5479.62

in the breeding cycle operate to what happens when you

Time: 5483.64

ingest sugar and fat.

Time: 5488.32

Now, let me give you an example again,

Time: 5496.821

of how the brain can take

Time: 5501.07

what we would refer to contextual associations and transform

Time: 5506.77

it into incredible changes in physiology and metabolism.

Time: 5512.95

Remember Pavlov?

Time: 5515.013

So Pavlov in his classical experiments in conditioning,

Time: 5518.35

associative conditioning, he would take a bell,

Time: 5522.37

he would ring the bell every time

Time: 5525.595

he was going to feed the dog.

Time: 5528.383

And eventually the dog learned to associate

Time: 5530.8

the ringing of the bell with food coming.

Time: 5534.467

Now, the first incredible finding

Time: 5538.33

he made is the fact that the dog now

Time: 5541.9

in the presence of the bell alone

Time: 5544.48

will start to salivate.

Time: 5547.18

And we will call that

Time: 5548.77

neurologically speaking an anticipatory response.

Time: 5554.14

Okay, I could understand it.

Time: 5555.64

I get it.

Time: 5556.93

You know, neurons in the brain that form

Time: 5559

that association now represent food is coming.

Time: 5562.9

And they're sending a signal to motor neurons to go into

Time: 5567.7

your salivary glands to squeeze them.

Time: 5570.07

So you release saliva because you know food is coming,

Time: 5576.01

but what's even more remarkable is that those animals are

Time: 5579.49

also releasing insulin in response to a bell.

Time: 5586.24

Okay.

Time: 5589.474

This illustrates one part of this two-way highway,

Time: 5593.08

the highway going down,

Time: 5595.78

somehow the brain created these associations and there are

Time: 5598.63

neurons in your brain now that no food is coming and send a

Time: 5603.22

signal somehow all the way down to your pancreas that now it

Time: 5608.062

says release insulin because sugar is coming down.

Time: 5613.294

This goes back to the magic of the brain.

Time: 5616.227

It's a never ending source of both joy and intrigue.

Time: 5620.35

How the hell do they do this?

Time: 5623.23

I mean, the neurons.

Time: 5625.003

- I share your delight and fascination.

Time: 5627.85

There's not a day or a lecture or some talks are better than

Time: 5633.76

others or talk where I don't sit back and just think,

Time: 5637.398

it's absolutely amazing.

Time: 5638.964

- How. - It's amazing.

Time: 5640.52

- Now over the past, I don't know,

Time: 5642.79

dozen years it and with great force

Time: 5646.222

over the last five years

Time: 5649.78

now, the main highway that is communicating

Time: 5652.9

the state of the body with the brain,

Time: 5657.304

it has been uncovered as being

Time: 5659.863

what we now refer to as the gut brain axis.

Time: 5663.73

And the highway is a specific bundle of nerves

Time: 5669.4

which emerged from the Vagal ganglia, the nodus ganglia.

Time: 5673.03

And so it's the vagus nerve that is innervating

Time: 5677.71

the majority of the organs in your body.

Time: 5682.12

It's monitoring their function,

Time: 5684.55

sending a signal to the brain.

Time: 5686.89

And now the brain going back down and saying,

Time: 5690.4

this is going alright, do this,

Time: 5692.23

or this is not going too well, do that.

Time: 5695.5

- And I should point out as you well know, every organ,

Time: 5698.59

spleen, pancreas, lung.

Time: 5701.68

- They all must be monitored.

Time: 5704.92

In fact I now,

Time: 5708.389

I have no doubt that the diseases that we have normally

Time: 5711.67

associated with metabolism, physiology,

Time: 5716.193

and even immunity are likely to emerge as diseases,

Time: 5723.22

conditions, states of the brain.

Time: 5727.45

I don't think obesity is a disease of metabolism.

Time: 5731.56

I believe obesity is a disease of brain circuits.

Time: 5735.82

- [Andrew] I do as well.

Time: 5737.23

- And so this view that we have,

Time: 5741.145

been working on for the longest time, because,

Time: 5746.08

the molecules that we're dealing with are in the body,

Time: 5748.54

not in the head, led us to to view,

Time: 5752.86

of course this issues and problems has been one of

Time: 5756.58

metabolism, physiology, and so forth.

Time: 5758.56

They remain to be the carriers of the ultimate signal,

Time: 5763.24

but the brain ultimately appears to be the conductor of this

Time: 5767.8

orchestra of physiology and metabolism.

Time: 5771.46

All right, now let's go to the gut brain and sugar,

Time: 5776.71

maybe.

Time: 5777.7

- Please, please.

Time: 5780.913

No, I mean,

Time: 5782.02

the vagus nerve has in popular culture has been kind of

Time: 5788.95

converted into this single meaning of calming pathways,

Time: 5794.02

mostly because I actually have to tip my hat

Time: 5797.11

to the yogic community was

Time: 5799.21

among the first to talk about vagus on and on and on,

Time: 5804.34

there are calming pathways,

Time: 5807.28

so-called parasympathetic pathways within the vagus.

Time: 5811.287

But I think that the more we learn about the vagus,

Time: 5812.8

the more it seems like an entire set of neural connections,

Time: 5817.06

as opposed to one nerve.

Time: 5818.53

I just wanted to just mention that because I think

Time: 5820.18

a lot of people have heard

Time: 5821.26

about the vagus turns out experimentally

Time: 5823.36

in the laboratory,

Time: 5824.56

many neuroscientists will stimulate the vagus

Time: 5827.05

to create states of alertness and arousal when animals,

Time: 5831.22

or even people believe it or not are close to dying

Time: 5833.95

or going into coma stimulation

Time: 5835.66

of the vagus is one of the ways

Time: 5836.77

to wake up the brain counter to the idea

Time: 5839.62

that it's just this I way of calming oneself down.

Time: 5842.2

- And also of course, I mean,

Time: 5844.06

one has to be cautious they're in that.

Time: 5847.664

So the vagus nerve is made

Time: 5850.75

out of many thousands of fibers,

Time: 5854.71

individual fibers that make this gigantic bundle.

Time: 5858.19

And it's likely as we're speaking,

Time: 5860.5

that each of these fibers carries

Time: 5863.837

a slightly different meaning,

Time: 5865.51

not necessarily one by one, maybe five is 10,

Time: 5868.84

five is 20, do all right,

Time: 5871.393

but they carry meaning that's associated

Time: 5875.02

with their specific task.

Time: 5878.14

This group of fibers is telling the brain

Time: 5880.81

about the state of your heart.

Time: 5883.78

This group of fiber is telling

Time: 5885.4

the brain about the state of your gut.

Time: 5888.88

This is telling your brain about its nutritional state,

Time: 5893.05

this, your pancreas, this your lungs.

Time: 5897.43

And they are again, to make the same simple example,

Time: 5903.457

the keys of this piano.

Time: 5906.555

Yes, you're right.

Time: 5908.26

There is a lot of data,

Time: 5913.06

showing that activating the entire vagal bundle has very

Time: 5919.39

meaningful effects in a wide range of conditions.

Time: 5922.9

In fact, it's being used to treat untractable depression.

Time: 5929.02

- A little stimulator.

Time: 5931.54

- Epileptics seizures.

Time: 5935.02

But again, there are thousands

Time: 5937.03

of fibers carrying different functions.

Time: 5939.58

So to some degree this is like

Time: 5946.72

turning the lights on the stadium,

Time: 5952.57

because you need to illuminate

Time: 5955.15

where you lost your keys under your seat.

Time: 5958.72

Yet 10,000 bulbs of a thousand Watts each have just come on.

Time: 5966.43

Only one of this is pointing to where,

Time: 5970.577

and so I'm lucky enough that one

Time: 5971.86

of them happened to point to my side.

Time: 5977.844

So here you activate the bundle thousands of fibers.

Time: 5979.93

I'm lucky enough that some of those happen to do something,

Time: 5985.06

to make a meaningful difference in depression,

Time: 5988.9

or to make a meaningful difference in epileptics.

Time: 5991.57

But it should not be misconstrued

Time: 5995.86

as arguing that this broad activation

Time: 6000.96

has any type of selectivity or specificity,

Time: 6005.04

we're just lucky enough that among all the things

Time: 6007.806

that are being done, some of those happen to change

Time: 6012.21

the biology of these processes.

Time: 6014.64

Now, the reason this is relevant

Time: 6016.98

because the magic of this gut brain axis

Time: 6021.54

is the fact that you have these thousands

Time: 6023.25

of fibers really doing different functions and our goal.

Time: 6029.46

And along with many other great scientists,

Time: 6037.26

including Steve Lieder Lee,

Time: 6038.97

that started a lot of this molecular dissection

Time: 6044.005

on this vagal gut brain communication line at Harvard,

Time: 6050.122

is trying to uncover what

Time: 6052.2

each of those lines doing,

Time: 6054.51

what are each of those keys of this piano playing?

Time: 6060.014

- What's the latest there?

Time: 6060.995

Just as a brief update.

Time: 6061.828

I know Steven Lieder Lee,

Time: 6062.76

I think I was there when he got

Time: 6065.19

his Howard Hughes and I did not.

Time: 6066.72

So that was fun.

Time: 6067.8

Always great to get beat by excellent people.

Time: 6071.018

- First of all, I'm happy you didn't,

Time: 6071.851

because that way you can focus on this amazing podcast.

Time: 6075.84

- Well, thank you.

Time: 6077.158

That's very gracious of you.

Time: 6078.09

It's always feels better if not good

Time: 6081.87

to get beat out by excellent people.

Time: 6083.28

Steven is second to none.

Time: 6086.82

And he is defining, as you said,

Time: 6088.98

the molecular constituents

Time: 6091.26

of different elements of these many, many fibers,

Time: 6093.66

is there an update there,

Time: 6094.59

are they finding multiple parallel pathways?

Time: 6097.02

- They are, they are, some that control heartbeat.

Time: 6100.831

Some that control the respiratory cycle.

Time: 6103.02

Some that might be involved in a gastric movement.

Time: 6109.56

You know, this notion that you're full

Time: 6112.41

and you feel full in part

Time: 6115.533

because your gut gets distended your stomach, for example.

Time: 6120

And then there are little sensors that are reading

Time: 6122.13

that and telling the brain you're full.

Time: 6125.876

- So the textbooks will soon change

Time: 6128.55

on the basis of the Lieder Lee and other work.

Time: 6132.406

- In essence, I think we are learning enough

Time: 6133.86

about these lines, that could really help put together

Time: 6142.2

this holistic view of how the brain

Time: 6147.3

it's truly changing body physiology,

Time: 6151.38

metabolism, and immunity,

Time: 6154.23

the part that hasn't been yet developed,

Time: 6158.749

and that it needs a fair amount of work,

Time: 6159.939

but it's an exciting thrilling,

Time: 6163.92

journey of discovery is how

Time: 6165.447

the signal comes back to now change that biology.

Time: 6170.819

You know, the example I gave you before with Pavlov's dog.

Time: 6174.96

I figure out how the association created

Time: 6178.986

this link between the bell,

Time: 6180.9

but then how does the brain tell the pancreas

Time: 6184.11

to release in the right amount of insulin?

Time: 6188.28

So,

Time: 6191.601

let me tell you about the gut brain axis

Time: 6197.588

and our insatiable appetite for sugar and fat.

Time: 6203.37

Insatiable for sugar,

Time: 6205.41

unquenchable for fat.

Time: 6210.212

And this is a story about the fundamental difference

Time: 6215.812

between liking and wanting.

Time: 6221.07

Liking sugar is the function of the taste system.

Time: 6229.53

And it's not really liking sugar,

Time: 6231.6

it's liking sweet,

Time: 6235.68

wanting sugar,

Time: 6239.43

our never ending appetite for sugar is the story

Time: 6245.16

of the gut brain axis,

Time: 6249.191

liking versus wanting.

Time: 6251.777

And this work is work of my own laboratory.

Time: 6254.4

You know, that began long ago

Time: 6256.56

when we discovered the sweet receptors

Time: 6260.58

and you can now engineer mice that lack these receptors.

Time: 6266.58

So in essence,

Time: 6267.63

these animals will be unable

Time: 6269.22

to taste sweet, a life without sweetness.

Time: 6274.95

How horrible.

Time: 6277.2

And if you give a normal mouse, a bottle containing sweet,

Time: 6282.51

and we're going to put either sugar or an artificial sweetener,

Time: 6286.83

they bottle sweet.

Time: 6288.33

They have slightly different tastes,

Time: 6290.88

but that's simply because artificial sweeteners

Time: 6295.32

have some off tastes.

Time: 6297.63

But as far as the sweet receptor is concerned,

Time: 6301.398

they both activate the same receptor,

Time: 6303.03

trigger the same signal.

Time: 6305.73

And if you give an animal,

Time: 6306.75

an option of a bottle containing sugar

Time: 6308.91

or a sweetener versus water,

Time: 6311.97

this animal will drink 10 to one

Time: 6314.61

from the bottle containing sweet.

Time: 6317.04

That's the taste system, animal goes samples, each one,

Time: 6320.58

licks a couple of licks and then says,

Time: 6322.89

that's the one I want because it's appetitive.

Time: 6324.86

And because I love it.

Time: 6328.005

- So it prefers sugar to artificial sweetener?

Time: 6329.97

- No, no, no, no, no, no.

Time: 6332.219

In this experiment, this experiment,

Time: 6333.93

I'm going to put only sweet in one bottle

Time: 6336.78

and it could be either sugar or artificial sweetener.

Time: 6339.39

It doesn't matter which one.

Time: 6341.31

We're going to do the next experiment

Time: 6343.05

where we separate those two,

Time: 6344.37

for now is sweet versus water.

Time: 6349.13

And sweet means sweet, not sugar.

Time: 6351.21

Sweet means anything that tastes sweet.

Time: 6355.921

And sugar is one example and Splenda is another example.

Time: 6360.071

- Aspartame, monk fruit, Stevia, doesn't matter.

Time: 6363.81

- I mean, there's some that only humans can taste.

Time: 6367.615

Mice cannot taste because their receptors

Time: 6369.96

between humans and mice are different,

Time: 6372.6

but we have put the human receptor into mice.

Time: 6376.98

We engineer mice and we completely

Time: 6379.44

humanize this mouse's taste world.

Time: 6386.735

But for the purpose of this conversation,

Time: 6389.52

we're only comparing sweet versus water, an option.

Time: 6393

My goodness,

Time: 6394.731

they will lick to know and from the sweet side 10 to one,

Time: 6399.18

at least versus the water, makes sense?

Time: 6402.57

All right, now we're going to take the mice

Time: 6404.79

and we're going to genetically engineer it

Time: 6407.61

to remove the sweet receptors.

Time: 6410.76

So this mice no longer have in their oral cavity,

Time: 6413.73

any sensors that can detect sweetness,

Time: 6416.79

be that sugar molecule be an artificial sweetener,

Time: 6420.27

be anything else that tastes sweet.

Time: 6424.205

And if you give this mice an option

Time: 6425.4

between sweet versus water,

Time: 6428.28

sugar versus water, artificial sweetener versus water,

Time: 6431.88

it will drink equally well from both

Time: 6434.959

because he cannot tell them apart

Time: 6436.355

because it doesn't have the receptors for sweet.

Time: 6437.34

So that sweet bottle tastes just like water.

Time: 6441.72

Makes sense? - Makes sense.

Time: 6443.01

- Very good.

Time: 6445.731

Now we're going to do the experiment

Time: 6447.623

with sugar from now on let's focus on sugar.

Time: 6449.16

So I'm going to give a mouse out sugar versus water.

Time: 6454.17

Normal mouse will drink from the sugar,

Time: 6456.51

sugar, sugar, sugar, very little from the water,

Time: 6461.04

knock out the sweet receptors, eliminate them.

Time: 6465.133

Mouse can no longer tell them apart.

Time: 6466.14

And they will drink from both.

Time: 6469.23

But if I keep the mouse in that case for the next 48 hours,

Time: 6475.56

something extraordinary happens when I come 48 hours later.

Time: 6479.97

And I see what the mouse is licking or drinking from,

Time: 6483.78

that mouse is drinking almost exclusively

Time: 6486.9

from the sugar bottle.

Time: 6489.27

How could this be?

Time: 6491.1

It cannot not taste it.

Time: 6493.41

It doesn't have sweet receptors during those 48 hours

Time: 6498.87

the mouse learn that there is something

Time: 6502.17

in that bottle that makes me feel good.

Time: 6507.18

And that is the bottle I want to consume.

Time: 6512.009

Now does the mouse identify that bottle?

Time: 6514.26

It does so by using other sensory features,

Time: 6518.07

the smell of the bottle,

Time: 6520.53

the texture of the solution inside, sugar,

Time: 6523.98

the high concentrations is kind of goopy,

Time: 6527.52

the side in which the bottle is in the cage.

Time: 6531.102

It doesn't matter what,

Time: 6533.49

but the mouse realize there is something

Time: 6536.13

there that makes me feel good.

Time: 6538.92

And that's what I want.

Time: 6541.738

And that is the fundamental basis of our unquenchable

Time: 6547.71

desire and our craving for sugar

Time: 6551.16

and is mediated by the gut brain axis.

Time: 6555.42

The first clue is that it takes a long time to develop,

Time: 6559.98

immediately suggesting a post-ingestive effect.

Time: 6566.43

So we reason if this is true and it's the gut brain axis

Time: 6570

that's driving sugar preference.

Time: 6574.95

Then there should be a group of neurons in the brain

Time: 6577.44

that are responding to post-ingestive sugar,

Time: 6583.17

and low and behold,

Time: 6584.866

we identify a group of neurons in the brain

Time: 6586.773

that does this and these neurons receive

Time: 6588.66

their input directly from

Time: 6591.72

the gut brain axis.

Time: 6594.24

- From other neurons.

Time: 6595.86

- You got it.

Time: 6598.83

And so what's happening is that sugar is recognized normally

Time: 6603.9

by the tongue, activates an appetitive response.

Time: 6609.138

Now you ingest it and now it activates a selective group

Time: 6612.33

of cells in your intestines

Time: 6615.51

that now send a signal to the brain

Time: 6618.39

via the vagal ganglia that says,

Time: 6623.4

I got what I need.

Time: 6625.44

The tongue doesn't know that you got what you need.

Time: 6628.23

It only knows that you tasted it.

Time: 6631.38

This knows that it got to the point

Time: 6633.93

that it's going to be used, which is the gut.

Time: 6637.68

And now it sends the signal to now reinforce

Time: 6641.82

the consumption of this thing,

Time: 6644.28

because this is the one that I needed.

Time: 6646.86

Sugar, source of energy.

Time: 6649.931

- And are these neurons in the gut?

Time: 6652.513

- So these are not neurons in the gut.

Time: 6654.055

So these are gut cells that recognize the sugar molecule.

Time: 6657.12

- [Andrew] I see.

Time: 6658.859

- Send the signal and that signal is received

Time: 6660.3

by the vagal neuron directly.

Time: 6662.01

- Got it.

Time: 6662.88

- And it sends a signal through the gut brain axis

Time: 6667.677

to the cell bodies of these neurons in the vagal ganglia,

Time: 6671.367

and from there to the brain stem

Time: 6674.79

to now trigger the preference for sugar.

Time: 6678.66

- Two questions.

Time: 6679.53

One, you mentioned that these cells

Time: 6682.29

that detect sugar within

Time: 6683.67

the gut are actually within the intestine.

Time: 6685.68

You didn't say stomach.

Time: 6687.45

Which surprised me.

Time: 6688.283

I always think gut as stomach, but of course-

Time: 6690.69

- They're intestines,

Time: 6692.745

because that's where all the absorption happens.

Time: 6693.578

- [Andrew] Got it.

Time: 6695.117

- So you want the signal, you see,

Time: 6695.95

you want the brain to know that you had successful ingestion

Time: 6699.66

and breakdown of whatever you consume into

Time: 6703.17

the building blocks of life,

Time: 6706.53

and glucose, amino acids, fat.

Time: 6710.01

And so you want to make sure that once they are in the form

Time: 6713.94

that intestines can now absorb them,

Time: 6717.3

is where you get the signal back saying this what I want.

Time: 6721.65

Now let me just take it one step further.

Time: 6726.03

And this now sugar molecules activates this unique gut brain

Time: 6731.31

circuit that now drives the development

Time: 6735.373

of our preference for sugar.

Time: 6738.789

Now, a key element of this circuit

Time: 6743.73

is that the sensors in the gut that recognize

Time: 6747.42

the sugar do not recognize artificial sweeteners at all.

Time: 6753.42

- Because their nutrient value

Time: 6755.16

is uncoupled from the taste?

Time: 6759

- Generically speaking one can make that,

Time: 6760.429

but it's because it's a very different type of receptor.

Time: 6764.7

Turns out that it's not

Time: 6765.72

the tongue receptors being used in the gut.

Time: 6769.02

It's a completely different molecule that only recognizes

Time: 6772.53

the glucose molecule, not artificial sweeteners.

Time: 6776.46

This has a profound impact

Time: 6781.764

on the effect of ultimately artificial sweeteners

Time: 6786.137

in curbing our appetite, our craving,

Time: 6789.99

our insatiable desire for sugar,

Time: 6793.56

since they don't activate the gut brain axis,

Time: 6798.12

they'll never satisfy the craving for sugar like sugar does.

Time: 6805.37

And the reason I believe that artificial sweeteners

Time: 6807.78

have failed in the market to curb our appetite.

Time: 6813.96

Our need, our desire for sugar is

Time: 6817.89

because they beautifully work on the tongue.

Time: 6822.18

They liking to recognize sweet versus non-sweet,

Time: 6828.51

but they fail to activate the key sensors

Time: 6833.55

in the gut that now inform the brain

Time: 6837.848

you got sugar, no need to crave anymore.

Time: 6842.76

- So the issue of wanting,

Time: 6847.26

can we relate that to a particular

Time: 6849.18

set of neurochemicals upstream,

Time: 6851.4

so the pathway is, so glucose is activating the cells

Time: 6855.03

in the gut that through the vagus

Time: 6856.47

that's communicated through the presumably

Time: 6858.93

the nodus ganglia and up into the brain stem.

Time: 6861.12

- Very good, and from there, where does it go?

Time: 6863.16

- Yeah, where does it go,

Time: 6864.776

and what is the substrate of wanting?

Time: 6866.76

Of course I think molecules like dopamine craving,

Time: 6870.66

there's a book even called the molecule of more, et cetera.

Time: 6874.44

Dopamine is a very diabolical molecule, as you know,

Time: 6877.77

because it evokes both a sense of pleasure-ish,

Time: 6881.55

but also a sense of desiring more, of craving.

Time: 6886.165

So if I understand you correctly, artificial sweeteners are,

Time: 6889.14

and I agree are failing as a means

Time: 6891.42

to satisfy sugar craving at the level of nutrient sensing.

Time: 6897.66

And yet if we trigger this

Time: 6900.66

true sugar evoked wanting pathway too much,

Time: 6906.15

and we've all experienced this,

Time: 6907.38

then we eat sugar and we find ourselves

Time: 6909.6

wanting more and more sugar.

Time: 6910.95

Now that could also be insulin dysregulation,

Time: 6913.83

but can we uncouple those?

Time: 6916.05

- I mean, look,

Time: 6919.92

if we have a mega problem

Time: 6924.39

with over consumption of sugar and fat,

Time: 6929.43

we're facing a unique time in our evolution where

Time: 6934.98

diseases of malnutrition are due to over-nutrition.

Time: 6939.96

I mean, how nuts is that?

Time: 6943.936

Historically, diseases of malnutrition

Time: 6945.96

have always been linked to undernutrition.

Time: 6951.96

And so we need to come up with strategies

Time: 6954.69

that can meaningfully change

Time: 6960.63

the activation of these circuits that control

Time: 6966.18

our wanting certainly in the populations at risk,

Time: 6974.644

and this gut brain circuit that ultimately

Time: 6980.76

it's the lines of communication that are informing

Time: 6984.12

the brain, the presence of intestinal sugar,

Time: 6988.285

in this example,

Time: 6989.64

it's a very important target in the way we think about,

Time: 6994.41

is there a way that we can

Time: 6995.55

meaningfully modulate these circuits?

Time: 6998.813

So I make your brain think

Time: 7001.46

that you got satisfied with sugar,

Time: 7005.27

even though I'm not giving you sugar.

Time: 7007.55

- So that immediately raises the question are the receptors

Time: 7010.85

for glucose in these gut cells susceptible

Time: 7014.57

to other things that are healthier for us.

Time: 7017.81

- That's very good.

Time: 7019.49

Excellent idea.

Time: 7021.29

And I think an important goal will be to come up

Time: 7026.06

with a strategy and identify

Time: 7029.48

those very means that allow us to

Time: 7032.57

modulate the circuits in a way

Time: 7034.97

that certainly for all of those,

Time: 7038.96

where this is a big issue, it can really have a,

Time: 7044.33

you know, dramatic impact in improving human health.

Time: 7048.5

- I could be wrong about this.

Time: 7049.64

And I'm happy to be wrong.

Time: 7051.691

I'm often wrong.

Time: 7052.94

And told I'm wrong, that we have cells within our gut

Time: 7058.04

that don't just sense sugar, glucose to be specific,

Time: 7063.364

but also cells within our gut

Time: 7064.58

that sense amino acids and fatty acids.

Time: 7068.27

I could imagine a scenario where one could train themselves

Time: 7072.05

to feel immense amounts of satiety from the consumption

Time: 7077.12

of foods that are rich in essential

Time: 7079.46

fatty acids, amino acids,

Time: 7081.5

perhaps less caloric

Time: 7083.51

or less insulin dysregulating than sugar.

Time: 7088.31

I'll use myself as an example.

Time: 7089.57

I've always enjoyed sweets,

Time: 7092.059

but in the last few years,

Time: 7093.298

for some reason I've started to lose my appetite

Time: 7094.82

for them probably because I just don't eat them anymore.

Time: 7098.15

At first that took some restriction.

Time: 7100.13

Now I just don't even think about it.

Time: 7102.11

- And you're not reinforcing the circuits.

Time: 7104.96

And so you're in essence are removing yourself from,

Time: 7108.86

but you tend to be the exception.

Time: 7112.37

You know, we have a huge,

Time: 7116.877

a incredible large number of people that are being

Time: 7119.69

continuously exposed to highly processed foods.

Time: 7123.77

- And hidden, so called hidden sugars.

Time: 7126.2

- They don't even have to be hidden.

Time: 7128.39

You know, it's right there.

Time: 7129.68

- Hiding in plain sight.

Time: 7131.63

I agree so much is made of hidden sugars that we often

Time: 7135.71

overlook that there are also the overt sugars.

Time: 7139.37

- I mean, we can have a long discussion

Time: 7142.902

on the importance of coming up with strategies

Time: 7148.225

that could meaningfully change public health

Time: 7150.11

when it comes to nutrition.

Time: 7153.62

But I want to just go back to the notion of

Time: 7156.86

this brain centers that are ultimately the ones

Time: 7162.02

that are being activated by these essential nutrients.

Time: 7165.56

So sugar, fat, amino acids are building blocks of our diets.

Time: 7173.36

And this is across all animal species.

Time: 7176.6

So it's not unreasonable

Time: 7178.19

then to assume that dedicated brain circuits

Time: 7182.69

would've evolved to ensure their recognition,

Time: 7189.14

their ingestion and the reinforcement

Time: 7194.15

that that is what I need.

Time: 7198.77

And indeed animals evolve these two systems.

Time: 7207.02

One is the taste system that allows you

Time: 7210.59

to recognize them and trigger

Time: 7212.9

this predetermined, hardwired, immediate responses.

Time: 7217.85

Oh my goodness.

Time: 7218.683

This tastes so good.

Time: 7220.192

It's so sweet.

Time: 7221.025

I personally have a sweet tooth,

Time: 7223.58

and you know, oh my God, this is so delicious.

Time: 7225.95

It's fatty or umami, recognizing the amino acids.

Time: 7229.82

So that's the liking pathway,

Time: 7233.165

but in the wisdom of evolution,

Time: 7239.45

that's good,

Time: 7240.283

but doesn't quite do it.

Time: 7241.97

You want to make sure that these things get to the place

Time: 7244.04

where they're needed and they're not needed in your tongue.

Time: 7249.5

They're needed in your intestines

Time: 7251.51

where they're going to be absorbed

Time: 7253.73

as the nutrients that will support life.

Time: 7258.05

And the brain wants to know this,

Time: 7263.15

and it wants to know it in a way that it can now

Time: 7265.97

form the association between

Time: 7269.72

that I just tasted is what got

Time: 7274.64

where it needs to be.

Time: 7276.98

And it makes me feel good.

Time: 7280.16

And so now next time that I have to choose,

Time: 7283.82

what should I eat?

Time: 7286.67

That association now guides me to,

Time: 7290.45

that's the one I want.

Time: 7292.28

I want that fruit, not that fruit.

Time: 7296.12

I want those leaves,

Time: 7297.56

not those leaves, because these are the ones that activate

Time: 7301.73

the right circuits that ensure that

Time: 7304.083

the right nutrients got to the right place

Time: 7305.51

and told the brain, this is what I want and need.

Time: 7312.59

Are we on? - We're on.

Time: 7314.78

One thing that intrigues me and puzzles me,

Time: 7317.66

is that this effect took a couple of days, at least in mice.

Time: 7323.448

And the sensation, ah, sorry,

Time: 7324.41

the perception of taste is immediate.

Time: 7327.423

and yet this is a slow system.

Time: 7330.863

And so in a beautiful way, but in a kind of mysterious way,

Time: 7333.83

the brain is able to couple the taste of a sweet drink

Time: 7337.49

with the experience of nutrient extraction in the gut,

Time: 7341.36

under a context where the mouse and the human

Time: 7345.613

is presumably ingesting other things, smelling other mice,

Time: 7347.69

smelling other people.

Time: 7349.07

That's incredible.

Time: 7350.57

- But you have to think of it not as humans,

Time: 7353.33

remember we inherited the circuits of our ancestors

Time: 7360.11

and they through evolutionary from their ancestors.

Time: 7364.91

And we haven't had that many

Time: 7367.16

years to have fundamentally changed in many

Time: 7371.39

of these hardwired circuits.

Time: 7373.962

So forget as humans,

Time: 7375.56

let's look at animals in the wild.

Time: 7378.92

Which is easier now to comprehend the logic.

Time: 7383.24

You know, why should this take

Time: 7385.58

a long time of continued reinforcement

Time: 7389.21

given that I can taste it in a second?

Time: 7394.25

You want to make sure that this source of sugar, for example,

Time: 7397.49

in the wild is the best.

Time: 7400.22

Is the richest.

Time: 7401.69

It's the one where I get the most energy

Time: 7404.33

for the least amount of extraction,

Time: 7409.401

the least amount of work.

Time: 7412.1

I want to identify rich sources of sugar.

Time: 7417.618

And if the system simply responds immediately

Time: 7420.92

to the first sugar that gets to your gut,

Time: 7423.5

you're going to form the association

Time: 7426.68

with those sources of food,

Time: 7429.17

which are not the ones that you should be eating from.

Time: 7433.524

- Don't fall in love with the first person you encounter.

Time: 7436.029

- Oh my goodness, exactly.

Time: 7439.234

And so evolutionarily by having the taste system,

Time: 7442.82

giving you the immediate recognition,

Time: 7445.19

but then by forcing this gut brain axis

Time: 7448.294

to reinforce it only when sustained

Time: 7453.71

repeated exposure has informed the brain.

Time: 7459.555

This is the one you don't want to form the association before.

Time: 7463.91

And so when we remove it from the context

Time: 7467.03

of we just go to the supermarket,

Time: 7471.95

we're not hunting there in the wild where I need to form.

Time: 7476.12

And so what's is that highly processed foods are

Time: 7483.68

hijacking, co-opting these circuits in a way

Time: 7490.802

that it would've never happened in nature.

Time: 7492.986

And then we not only find these things appetitive

Time: 7496.01

and palatable, but in addition,

Time: 7497.42

we are continuously reinforcing,

Time: 7500.78

the wanting in a way that, oh my God, this is so great.

Time: 7504.29

What do I feel like eating?

Time: 7505.31

Let me have more of this.

Time: 7508.038

- You've just forever changed the way

Time: 7509.985

that I think about supermarkets and restaurants.

Time: 7512.51

There are understanding this fast signaling

Time: 7516.5

and this slower signaling

Time: 7518.361

and the utility of having both makes me realize

Time: 7523.018

that supermarkets and restaurants are

Time: 7523.851

about the most unnatural thing for our system ever,

Time: 7528.8

almost the equivalent of living

Time: 7531.02

in small villages with very few suitable mates

Time: 7534.05

versus online dating, for instance.

Time: 7536.15

- I'm not going to make a judgment call there

Time: 7539.283

because they do serve an important purpose.

Time: 7541.066

- I like restaurants too.

Time: 7542.12

- And so do supermarkets, thank God.

Time: 7545.39

I think they're not the culprits.

Time: 7547.85

I think the culprits of course

Time: 7552.895

are our reliance

Time: 7557.266

on foods that are not necessarily healthy.

Time: 7561.151

Now, going back to the supermarket,

Time: 7564.38

don't fall in love with the first, they need to work.

Time: 7567.914

You know, you take

Time: 7573.883

a tangerine,

Time: 7577.942

and you take an extract of tangerine

Time: 7582.8

that you used to cook that spiked, let's say with sugar

Time: 7588.439

and you equalize in both

Time: 7592.479

where they both provide the same amount of calories.

Time: 7595.55

If you eat them both,

Time: 7598.28

they're going to have a very different effect

Time: 7601.85

in your gut brain axis on your system.

Time: 7605.57

Once you make the extract and you process it and you add

Time: 7609.785

processed sugar to use it now to cook, to add,

Time: 7612.95

to make it really sweet tangerine thing.

Time: 7616.539

Now you're providing now fully ready

Time: 7619.13

to use broken down source of sugar,

Time: 7624.53

in the tangerine that sugar's mixed in the complexity

Time: 7631.451

of a whole set of other chemical components,

Time: 7636.44

fiber, long chains of sugar molecules

Time: 7640.22

that need a huge amount of work by your stomach,

Time: 7645.802

your gut system, to break it down.

Time: 7649.07

So you are using a huge amount of energy

Time: 7653.21

to extract energy and the balance

Time: 7659

it's very different that when

Time: 7663.351

I take this process highly extracted tangerine,

Time: 7667.548

by the way I use tangerines because I had a tangerine

Time: 7668.96

just before I came here.

Time: 7670.16

- Delicious, they are delicious.

Time: 7674.46

- And so this goes back to the issue of supermarkets.

Time: 7678.26

And to some degree, given a choice,

Time: 7683.54

you don't want to eat highly processed foods

Time: 7686.536

because everything's already been broken down for you.

Time: 7689.578

And so your system has no work.

Time: 7692.78

And so you are,

Time: 7694.574

co-opting hijacking the circuits in a way

Time: 7696.14

that they're being activated

Time: 7697.61

at a time scale that normally wouldn't happen.

Time: 7702.982

- Well, this is why I often feel that,

Time: 7706.14

and I think a lot of data are now starting to support

Time: 7708.505

the idea that while indeed the laws of thermodynamics apply

Time: 7711.41

calories ingested versus calories burned

Time: 7713.66

is a very real thing, right?

Time: 7718.724

That the appetite for certain foods

Time: 7723.29

and the wanting and the liking

Time: 7726.11

are phenomena of the nervous system,

Time: 7729.23

brain and gut as you've beautifully described.

Time: 7731.84

And that changes over time,

Time: 7734.75

depending on how we are receiving these nutrients.

Time: 7737.48

- Absolutely.

Time: 7739.508

Look,

Time: 7741.65

we have a lot of work to do.

Time: 7745.7

I'm talking as society.

Time: 7747.62

I'm not talking about you and I.

Time: 7749.99

- We also have a lot of work to do.

Time: 7753.122

- Now.

Time: 7755.12

I think understanding

Time: 7755.953

the circuits is giving us important

Time: 7759.32

insights in how ultimately,

Time: 7761.93

and hopefully we can improve human health

Time: 7766.25

and make a meaningful difference.

Time: 7770.78

Now, it's very easy to try to connect

Time: 7775.1

the dots A to B, B to C, C to D.

Time: 7779.177

And I think there's a lot more complexity to it,

Time: 7783.878

but I do think that the lessons that are emerging

Time: 7786.38

out of understanding how these circuits operate,

Time: 7790.73

can ultimately inform how we deal with our diets

Time: 7798.77

in a way that we avoid

Time: 7800.75

what we're facing now as a society.

Time: 7803.87

I mean it's nuts that over-nutrition happens

Time: 7809.24

to be such a prevalent problem.

Time: 7812.802

And I also think the training of people who are thinking

Time: 7814.73

about metabolic science and metabolic disease is largely

Time: 7818.09

divorced from the training

Time: 7819.23

of the neuroscientist and vice versa.

Time: 7821.39

No one field is to blame,

Time: 7822.71

but I fully agree that

Time: 7825.384

the brain is the key over, or the nervous system

Time: 7829.222

to be more accurate is one of the key overlooked features.

Time: 7832.43

- Is the arbiter,

Time: 7834.56

ultimately is the arbiter of many of these pathways.

Time: 7838.28

- As a final question.

Time: 7839.33

And one which is simply to entertain my curiosity

Time: 7842.96

and the curiosity of the listeners.

Time: 7846.05

What is your absolute favorite food?

Time: 7849.26

- Oh, my goodness.

Time: 7852.59

- Taste, I should say.

Time: 7855.14

Taste to distinguish between taste

Time: 7858.267

and the nutritive value or lack thereof.

Time: 7860.977

- Look, we, unlike every animal

Time: 7863.037

and every animal species eat for the enjoyment of it,

Time: 7870.47

it doesn't happen in the wild,

Time: 7873.02

most animals eat when they need to eat.

Time: 7876.74

Doesn't mean they don't enjoy it,

Time: 7880.261

but it's a completely different story.

Time: 7885.914

I have too many favorite foods because I enjoy the sensory

Time: 7889.4

experience rather than the food itself

Time: 7893.69

to me is the whole thing, is from the presentation, look,

Time: 7896.841

there've been these experiments done in psycho physics,

Time: 7899.3

I'm going to take a salad, made out of 11 components

Time: 7904.61

and I'm going to mix them all up in potpourri

Time: 7909.304

greens and other things here.

Time: 7910.55

And in the other one,

Time: 7912.355

I'm going to present it in a beautiful arrangement.

Time: 7914.26

And I'm going to put them behind

Time: 7915.52

a glass cabinet and I'm going to sell them.

Time: 7917.846

And I'm going to sell one for $2 and one for $8,

Time: 7921.5

precisely the same ingredient.

Time: 7923.3

Exactly the same amount of each.

Time: 7926.39

Ultimately you're going to mix them,

Time: 7928.27

they're all going to be the same,

Time: 7930.62

and people will pay the $8 because you know what,

Time: 7935.78

it evokes a different person.

Time: 7939.5

It gives you the feel that, oh my goodness,

Time: 7942.65

I'm going to enjoy that salad.

Time: 7946.07

So going back to,

Time: 7947.33

what is my favorite food,

Time: 7949.46

to me eating is really

Time: 7955.848

a sensory journey.

Time: 7959.116

I don't mean the every day, let me have some

Time: 7960.869

chicken wings, because I'm hungry it, but eh,

Time: 7967.49

every piece I think has an important

Time: 7973.82

evoking sensory role.

Time: 7979.61

And so in terms of categories

Time: 7983.18

of food I grew up in Chile.

Time: 7986.21

So meat has always been, but I eat it so seldom now.

Time: 7993.79

- Is that right?

Time: 7995.75

- Yeah, because I know that it's

Time: 7997.169

not necessarily the healthiest thing,

Time: 7998.15

red meat I'm talking about.

Time: 8001.45

And so I grew up eating it every day.

Time: 8004.96

I'm talking seven days a week, Chile, Argentina,

Time: 8008.23

that's the mainstay of our diet.

Time: 8013.21

Now maybe I have red meat.

Time: 8016.337

I don't know, once every four weeks.

Time: 8020.77

- And you enjoy it?

Time: 8021.64

- Oh, I love it.

Time: 8022.75

Part of it is because I haven't had it in four weeks.

Time: 8026.17

But you know, I love sushi,

Time: 8030.785

but I love the art of sushi, the whole thing,

Time: 8034.99

the way it's presented, it changes the way you taste it.

Time: 8041.32

I love ethnic food in particular.

Time: 8046.21

- You're in the right place.

Time: 8048.103

- You got it, that was the main reason

Time: 8049.33

I wanted to come to New York.

Time: 8050.5

No, I'm just kidding.

Time: 8052.834

- There's also that Columbia University that's-

Time: 8055.26

- And I came here because I wanted to be with

Time: 8057.79

people that are thinking about the brain,

Time: 8060.16

the same way that I like to think,

Time: 8061.78

which can we solve this big problem, this big question.

Time: 8067.001

- And certainly you're making amazing strides

Time: 8068.86

in that direction.

Time: 8069.79

And I love your answer because it really brings together

Time: 8073.48

the many features of the circuitries

Time: 8075.31

and the phenomena we've been talking about today,

Time: 8076.99

which is that while it begins with sensation and perception,

Time: 8080.32

ultimately it's the context.

Time: 8081.91

And that context is highly individual to person,

Time: 8084.7

place and time, and many, many other things

Time: 8088.72

on behalf of myself.

Time: 8090.76

And certainly on behalf of all the listeners,

Time: 8092.8

I want to thank you, first of all,

Time: 8094.57

for the incredible work

Time: 8096.703

that you've been doing now for decades,

Time: 8097.891

in vision, in taste and in this bigger issue

Time: 8100.06

of how we perceive and experience life,

Time: 8103.06

it's truly pioneering and incredible work.

Time: 8105.637

And I feel quite lucky to have been on the sidelines,

Time: 8109.45

seeing this over the years and hearing the talks

Time: 8111.28

and reading the countless beautiful papers,

Time: 8114.31

but also for your time today

Time: 8115.96

to come down here and talk to us

Time: 8118.901

about what drives you and the discoveries you've made.

Time: 8121.15

Thank you ever so much.

Time: 8122.77

- It was great fun.

Time: 8124.45

Thank you for having me.

Time: 8125.98

- We'll do it again. - We shall.

Time: 8128.83

- Thank you for joining me today

Time: 8129.91

for my discussion about perception and in particular,

Time: 8132.7

the perception of taste with Dr. Charles Zuker.

Time: 8135.85

If you're learning from, and or enjoying this podcast,

Time: 8138.37

please subscribe to our YouTube channel.

Time: 8140.08

That's a terrific zero cost way to support us.

Time: 8142.51

In addition, please subscribe

Time: 8143.95

to the podcast on Spotify and Apple

Time: 8146.47

and on Spotify and Apple,

Time: 8148.24

you can leave us up to a five star review.

Time: 8150.52

If you have feedback for us in terms

Time: 8152.35

of comments related to topics we've covered,

Time: 8154.66

or questions or topics or guests that you'd like us

Time: 8157.3

to cover on future episodes of the Huberman Lab Podcast,

Time: 8160.09

please put those in the comment section on YouTube.

Time: 8163.278

We do read all the comments.

Time: 8164.17

On today's episode of the Huberman Lab Podcast,

Time: 8166.48

we didn't talk about supplements,

Time: 8168.802

but on many previous episodes of the Huberman Lab Podcast,

Time: 8170.71

we talk about supplements that are useful for sleep,

Time: 8172.78

for focus, for hormone support

Time: 8174.25

and other aspects of mental health,

Time: 8175.84

physical health, and performance.

Time: 8177.64

If you're interested in some of those supplements,

Time: 8179.2

you can go to livemomentous.com/huberman to see

Time: 8182.11

the catalog of supplements that we've helped them formulate.

Time: 8184.87

And that map directly onto specific protocols described

Time: 8188.02

on the Huberman Lab Podcast.

Time: 8189.19

Again, that's livemomentous.com/huberman.

Time: 8191.631

If you're not already following us on social media,

Time: 8194.47

we are Huberman Lab on Twitter

Time: 8196.42

and Huberman Lab on Instagram.

Time: 8198.37

And at both Twitter and Instagram,

Time: 8200.95

I cover science and science related tools.

Time: 8203.17

Some of which overlap with the content

Time: 8204.73

of the Huberman Lab Podcast,

Time: 8205.99

but much of which is distinct

Time: 8207.01

from the information contained in the Huberman Lab Podcast.

Time: 8209.83

We also have a newsletter in which we spell out protocols

Time: 8212.8

and some summaries from previous podcast episodes.

Time: 8215.59

If you'd like to check those out,

Time: 8216.67

you can go to hubermanlab.com,

Time: 8218.5

go to the menu and you'll find

Time: 8220.03

the Neural Network Newsletter.

Time: 8221.53

You can sign up for that newsletter simply

Time: 8223.03

by providing an email, it's completely zero cost.

Time: 8226.09

We have a very strict privacy policy.

Time: 8227.62

We do not share your email with anybody.

Time: 8229.39

Again, that's hubermanlab.com,

Time: 8231.04

go to the menu and look for the Neural Network Newsletter.

Time: 8233.59

And if you'd like examples of previous newsletters,

Time: 8235.72

you'll also find those at hubermanlab.com.

Time: 8238.12

Once again, thank you for joining me today,

Time: 8239.62

my discussion with Dr. Charles Zuker

Time: 8241.75

about the biology of perception and the biology

Time: 8243.85

of the perception of taste in particular,

Time: 8245.95

I hope you found that discussion

Time: 8248.146

to be as enriching as I did.

Time: 8249.34

And last, but certainly not least,

Time: 8251.41

thank you for your interest in science.

Time: 8253.427

[light rock music]

Copyright © 2024. All rights reserved.